Why Spelling is Important and How to Teach It Effe
Why Spelling is Important and How to Teach It Effe
net/publication/254948833
CITATIONS READS
16 43,753
2 authors, including:
Fayol Michel
University of Clermont Auvergne
332 PUBLICATIONS 11,281 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Fayol Michel on 27 June 2014.
Introduction
Spelling is a code that uses letter sequences to represent specific words that have an
associated pronunciation and meaning within the mental dictionary. Three kinds of
codes contribute to spelling: a phonological code (coding and awareness of sounds in
spoken words), an orthographic code (coding and awareness of letters in written
words), and a morphological code (word parts at the beginning of words that modify
shade of meaning and at end of words that mark tense, number, or part of speech). For
example, the word “jumped” has five small sounds in it: /j/, /u/, /m/, /p/, and /t/ (these
sounds are called phonemes). However, it has six letters. That is because the last two
letters correspond to a word part (morpheme) that marks the past tense but
corresponds to a single sound. In other words, that sound might be /d/ as in “named” or
/ed/ as in “wanted.”
The contrasting instructional implications of these different kinds of knowledge will now
be considered. Rules are articulated declarative knowledge, applied at the
metacognitive level in guiding, self-checking, or revising spelling. Teachers verbalize
these rules and ask children to verbalize them too. Linguistic awareness is not acquired
by verbalizing rules but rather by conscious reflections and operations on phonological,
orthographic, and morphological word-forms and their parts in conscious memory. For
example, children may engage in word sorts in which they have to reflect about
common and unique sounds or morpheme patterns in written words as they classify
them into categories. Little is known about how to teach children to abstract statistical
patterns related to sound sequencing or letter sequencing, however, it may help to draw
their attention to these patterns by playing games in which children judge whether
scrambled sequences of sounds in spoken words sound like words in their language or
scrambled sequences of letters look like written words in their language. Using
anagrams in which they unscramble the letters to spell a real word may also benefit
their spelling. Research reported later shows how specific procedures can be taught
through modeling for helping children generate word spellings at the levels of phoneme-
grapheme correspondences, onset-rimes, and whole words. Not all of the necessary
spelling knowledge can be taught as declarative knowledge or rules.
Past views that spelling goes through sequential stages from phonological to
orthographic to morphological (e.g., Templeton & Bear, 1992) are being reconsidered
based on research showing that first graders have not only phonological but also
orthographic (Cassar & Treiman, 1993; Pacton et al., 2001; Treiman, 1993) and
morphological (Carlisle, & Nomanbhoy,1993; Pacton et al., 2005b; Treiman & Cassar,
1996) knowledge that they apply to spelling. Both beginning spelling and developing
spelling, when words are longer and morphologically more complex (Carlisle & Fleming,
2003), draw on phonology, orthography, and morphology (Berninger, Garcia, & Abbott,
2009; Silliman, Barr, & Peters, 2006; Walker & Hauerwas, 2006). Woodcock Johnson
Third Edition (WJ III) Spell Sounds Subtest (Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001)
assesses phonological spelling with pseudowords. Process Assessment of the Learner,
Second Edition (PAL II) Word Choice subtest (Berninger, 2007) uses real words and
pseudohomophones to assess orthographic spelling. PAL II Find the Fixes (Berninger,
2007) uses real words with common spelling units that are true affixes (e.g. reread) and
foils (e.g., ready) that are spelled like a real prefix or suffix but are not morphemes to
assess morphological spelling.
Half the children reached grade level and maintained gains at beginning and end of
third grade. The other half received additional spelling instruction in third grade that
included the three mapping procedures for spelling two-syllable words with and without
syllable awareness training; they also wrote sentences from dictation. All reached
average range for grade and maintained gains at end of third grade; syllable awareness
training showed an advantage for silent-e words (e.g., became) (Berninger, Vaughan,
Abbott, Brooks, Begay, Curtin et al., 2000). Spelling mastery of practiced words was
For evidence-based instructional tools and strategies for explicit spelling instruction, see
Berninger and Abbott (2003, Lesson Sets 4, 5, 7, and 10); Dixon and Engelmann (2001;
Fry (1996); Graham, Harris, and Loynachan (1996); Henry (2003); Masterson, Apel, and
Wasowicz (2002); Nunes and Bryant (2006); and Schalagar (2001). Also see readings
at end for teachers to expand their knowledge of the role of phonology, orthography,
morphology, vocabulary knowledge, phonotactics and orthotactics, and syntax in
spelling. Research has shown that teachers’ knowledge of language processes is as
important as the instructional tools they use in increasing their students’ literacy skills
including spelling.
In languages such as French or English in which the same letter or letter group is not
always pronounced the same and semantic access is not direct: spelling, decoding, and
semantic access (to word meaning) are mediated by morphology (Pacton, & Fayol,
2005a). For French and English spelling, it is necessary to learn orthographic and
morphological regularities (Pacton et al: 2001, 2005b) and some lexical items (see
Martinet, Valdois, & Fayol, 2004). For example, in French it helps to find most silent
Training mapping procedures at three unit sizes (whole word, onset-rimes, and
phoneme-grapheme correspondences) improved word-spelling and transferred to
longer compositions (Berninger et al., 1998). Composing activities using grade-
appropriate high frequency words (Graham, Harris, & Loynachan, 1993, 1994) improved
spelling and composing (Berninger et al., 2000). Graham et al.’s (2002) spelling
instruction also transferred to improved composition. Therefore, evidence is growing for
the benefits of teaching spelling on another writing skill, namely, composition.
A longitudinal writing study (grades one to seven) showed that spelling at one grade
level contributes to spelling and often written composition at the next grade level
(manuscript in preparation). Orthographic, phonological, and morphological awareness
showed significant growth from grades one to two to three with continued morphological
awareness growth thereafter (submitted manuscript). At grades two, four, and six, a
second-order factor underlying these three kinds of linguistic awareness explained
unique variance in spelling and fit the model better than if each factor (code) was
considered alone (Berninger, Raskind, Richards, Abbott, & Stock, 2008). The
instructional application is that spelling benefits from instruction not only in each of the
three codes but also in their interrelationships. An example of instruction that teaches
the interrelationships is word sorting in which children sort words using suffixes to mark
number into these categories: plural pronounced /ez/ (e.g., busses), plural pronounced
/s/ (e.g., cats), plural pronounced /z/ (bees), or no suffix (e.g. miss).
Future Directions
More research is needed on phonotactic and orthotactic knowledge and their influences
on word storage and access in the mental lexicon. Fast mapping, which involves whole
spoken and written words that are learned quickly in one exposure or a few exposures,
is likely to be influenced by phonotactic and orthotactic knowledge.
With increasing globalization and immigration and more than one language spoken at
work and in the home, multi-lingualism is increasing and requires greater research
attention to spelling across languages: (a) transfer of spelling from one’s first language
to one’s second language; (b) similarities and differences in spelling related to how
Conclusions
To learn more about phonological codes in spelling, see Berninger, Cartwright, Yates,
Swanson, and Abbott (1994), Treiman, Berch, Tincoff, and Weatherston (1993),
Varnhagen, Varnhagen, and Das (1992), and Varnhagen, Boechler, and Steffler
(1999).
To learn more about orthographic coding in spelling, see Berninger, Yates, Cartwright,
Rutberg, Remy, and Abbott (1992), Caravolas, Kessler, Hulme, and Snowling
(2005), Holmes and Davis (2002), Jaffre and Fayol (2006), Johnson (1986), Olson,
Forsberg, and Wise (1994), Seymour (1997), Pacton, Fayol, and Perruchet
(2005b), and Varnhagen et al., (1992, 1999).
To learn more about morphological coding in spelling, see Bourassa, Treiman, and
Kessler (2006), Carlisle (1994), Derwing, Smith, and Wiebe (1995), Green,
McCutchen, Schwiebert, Quinlan, Eva-Wood, and Juelis (2003), Jaffré and Fayol
(2006), Leong, (2000); Nagy, Berninger, and Abbott (2006), Nagy, Berninger,
Abbott, Vaughan, and Vermeulen (2003), Nunes, Bryant, and Bindman (1997),
Pacton et al. (2005b), Treiman and Cassar (1996).
To learn more about the role of vocabulary knowledge in spelling, see Berninger et al.
(1992, 1994) and especially Stahl and Nagy (2005).
To learn more about the role of phonotactics in spelling, see Apel, Wolter, and
Masterson (2006), Bernstein and Treiman (2001), Kessler and Treiman (1997),
and Treiman, Kessler, Knewasser, Tincoff, and Bowman (2000),
To learn more about the role of orthotactics in spelling, see Apel et al. (2006) and
Pacton et al. (2005).
To learn more about the role of syntax in spelling, see Bryant, Nunes, and Bindman
(1997, 2000), Fayol, Totereau, and Barrouillet (2006), and Muter and Snowling
(1997).
References
Apel, K., Wolter, J., & Masterson, J. (2006). Effects of phonotactic and orthotactic
probabilities during fast mapping on 5-year-olds' learning to spell. Developmental
Neuropsychology, 29(1), 21-42.
Arnback, E., & Elbro, C. (2000). The effects of morphological awareness training on the
reading and spelling skills of young dyslexics. Scandanavian Journal of
Educational Research, 44, 229-251.
Berninger, V. (2007). Process assessment of the learner, 2nd Edition: PAL II RW
diagnostic for reading and writing. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Berninger, Abbott, R., Rogan, L., Reed, E., Abbott, S., Brooks, A., et al. (1998).
Teaching spelling to children with specific learning disabilities: The mind's ear and
eye beat computer or pencil. Learning Disability Quarterly, 21(2), 106-122.
Berninger, V., & Abbott, S. (2003). PAL Research-supported reading and writing
lessons. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt/PsyCorp.
Berninger, Cartwright, A., Yates, C., Swanson, H., & Abbott, B. (1994). Developmental
skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades. Reading
and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 161-196.
Berninger, V., & Fayol, M. (2008). Why spelling is important and how to teach it
effectively. Encyclopedia of Language and Literacy Development (pp. 1-13).
London, ON: Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network. Retrieved from
http://www.literacyencyclopedia.ca/pdfs/topic.php?topId=234