Questões Reflexão Formação Risco Brincar
Questões Reflexão Formação Risco Brincar
Submitted by
Kristina M. Ihrig
Spring 2021
Master’s Committee:
Shelly Lane
Jennifer Aberle
Copyright by Kristina M. Ihrig 2021
A small amount of risk is a natural component of play that provides children with
purpose of this study is to examine validity and reliability of data produced with a
new instrument, Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-TRiPS). T-TRiPS, a 25-
12. Rasch analysis was used to analyze data. Findings support strong evidence for
fit statistics in the acceptable range, and a logical item hierarchy. A strata value of
3.01 and person reliability index of 0.80 support internal reliability. T-TRiPS
produces valid and reliable data regarding teacher tolerance of risk in play. T-TRiPS
can serve as the basis for self-reflection or intervention aimed at increasing children’s
ii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
I wish to express my deepest gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Anita Bundy, for your
guidance, patience, and support throughout this thesis journey. Thank you for generously sharing
your knowledge of this subject and encouraging me to follow my own passion advocating for
children’s right to play through this research. It has been an honor to learn from you. I am
especially grateful to you for helping to keep me grounded in my strengths and aware of my
progress throughout the stresses and celebrations of this process. Thank you to the other
members of my incredible thesis committee, Dr. Shelly Lane and Dr. Jen Aberle, for your
encouragement, insightful comments, and invaluable guidance along this thesis journey. I am
extremely grateful to you for the knowledge you shared and the input you provided to help me
successfully complete this research. My sincere thanks goes to my friends from RIOT for your
kindness and willingness to provide feedback at all stages of this thesis. I would also like to
acknowledge the incredible friends I’ve made through the last two years of this OT program.
Thank you for being a shoulder in tough times and a cheerleader in the excited times. Finally, I
must express profound gratitude to my dear friends and my family for providing me with
encouragement throughout the last two years of study and through the process of completing this
thesis. Thank you for believing in me when I needed to be reminded that I can do hard things and
taking every opportunity to celebrate with me. This accomplishment would not have been
iii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT.................................................................................................................................... ii
Measures of Child Play and Behaviors as Predictors of Risk and Injury ............. 21
iv
Measures of Adult Practices that Contribute to Risk and Injury .......................... 22
Measures of Play for Children with Autism and/or Intellectual Disability .......... 31
Conclusion .................................................................................................................... 37
Method .......................................................................................................................... 43
Participants ................................................................................................................ 43
Instrument ................................................................................................................. 44
Procedure .................................................................................................................. 46
v
Construct Validity ................................................................................................. 47
Results ........................................................................................................................... 48
Discussion ..................................................................................................................... 52
Limitations ................................................................................................................ 56
Conclusions ................................................................................................................... 57
Personal Reflections...................................................................................................... 60
REFERENCES ............................................................................................................................. 62
APPENDIX ................................................................................................................................... 72
vi
CHAPTER 1
INTRODUCTION
Every day, all over the world, children are exposed to risks. But not all risks are bad. In
fact, some risk that is a natural part of children’s play provides vital opportunities for
development (Sandseter, 2012). Through active engagement in “risky play,” children learn to
interact with others, trust their own abilities, discover the boundaries of their skills and imagine
their potential (Beetham et al., 2019; Brussoni, Gibbons, et al., 2015; Garwood, 1982; Piaget,
1972; Sandseter & Kennair, 2011; Yogman et al., 2018). Children must learn to recognize and
respond appropriately to risks in order to promote their health and well-being (International
Opportunities to respond to safe degrees of fear, physical, social and emotional challenge,
uncertainty, and the possibility of failure support children in learning to assess risks and manage
their responses to these situations (Sandseter, 2007; Spencer et al., 2016). Although several
variations of the definition of ‘risky play’ exist, Sandseter (2009) defined this type of play as
activities that are exciting, thrilling, and involve a risk of physical injury. Additional authors
expanded this definition by including that risky play involves uncertainty and exploration - of
one’s physical capabilities, emotions, perceptions, and environment - and often involves a degree
of relinquishing control by teachers and children (Ball, Gill, & Spiegal, 2012; Kleppe, Melhuish,
& Sandseter, 2017; Lester & Russell, 2014; Spencer et al., 2016). For the purposes of this paper,
I adopted a combination of these as my definition of risky play: activities that are exciting and
thrilling, that involve exploration (of physical, social, emotional, perceptions, and environment)
and that require children and adults to accept uncertainty and relinquish control.
1
While risky play can result in injury or even death, it is important to understand that risk
of varying degrees is inherent in every situation and represents a spectrum of severity (Ball,
2002; Ball et al., 2012). Many advocates for risky play use terms such as “manageable risky
play” or “safe risks” to refer to a level of risky play that is severe enough to be exciting and
challenging to a child, but not so severe that likelihood of injury are obvious and expected (Ball
et al., 2012; Sandseter, 2009). Perceptions of risk are subjective, indicating the severity of risky
play will vary with individuals’ comfort with risk and perceptions of how dangerous an
experience is.
Children typically engage in risky play during physical activity and while playing
outdoors but the types of activities that are considered risky are numerous and complex. To help
children and teachers on the different risky play activities they enjoy and organized these into
eight categories (Kleppe et al., 2017; Sandseter, 2009): (A) great heights, (B) high speeds, (C)
dangerous tools, (D) dangerous elements, (E) rough-and-tumble, (F) disappear/get lost, (G) play
with impact, (H) vicarious risk. (See Table 2.1 for in-depth descriptions of the categories of risky
play.) Each of these categories houses numerous activities that comprise different severity of
risk; several activities involve a combination of different categories. Even though there is a risk
of injury inherent in risky play, the thrill and excitement of experiencing this type of play is one
reason children return to risky play experiences time and time again. In addition to being a fun
and satisfying experience, risky play has significant developmental benefits that are important to
consider.
2
Benefits of Risky Play for Children of All Abilities
Increasing recognition of risky play led researchers to seek to understand the benefits to
children’s health, development, and wellbeing. In a systematic review, Brussoni, Gibbons, et al.
(2015) found that the impact of risky play on development is positive. The most commonly
identified benefit is an increase in physical activity, but social, psychological, and cognitive
benefits have also been identified (Brussoni, Gibbons, et al., 2015; Bundy et al., 2011). Children
learn social interaction skills through opportunities to problem-solve when faced with conflict
and while engaging in rough and tumble play with peers (Pellegrini, 1988). Problem-solving
related to assessing risks and learning to manage them also promotes executive functioning,
creativity, confidence and a positive sense of self. Being able to assess a risky play opportunity
and decide how to engage while best managing the risk of injury are protective factors against
injury because children have learned the boundaries of their own skills and how to best engage
with risky environments (Brussoni, Olsen, Pike, & Sleet, 2012; Christensen & Mikkelsen, 2008).
Additionally, when provided with opportunities to engage in risky play, children face less
boredom and do not so often seek out exciting opportunities that can be beyond their ability
(Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Sandseter, 2009). The benefits of risky play are not limited to
typically developing children; they also are impactful for children with disabilities like autism
and intellectual disability as well and should, therefore, be a priority for this population (Bundy
et al., 2015).
remain in practice (Ball, Brussoni, Gill, Harbottle, & Spiegal, 2019; Sandseter, 2014). When
risky play is prohibited or a child’s skills are more advanced than the challenges their play
3
environment can offer, children often seek out exceedingly challenging experiences that provide
them with the slight thrill of fear and uncertainty to combat boredom (Sandseter, 2007). This
thrill-seeking play can result in injury as children are ill-equipped to manage this spike in
severity of risk. Therefore, it is important to provide children with gradually increasing risky
play opportunities that support them in assessing their ability to manage the risks, and ultimately,
Despite the well documented benefits of play on development and the UN’s declaration
that all children have the right to engage in play (U.N.C.R.C., 1989), children with disabilities
experience play inequity compared to their typically developing peers (Sterman, Naughton,
Bundy , Froude, & Villeneuve, 2019). The cause of this imbalance in opportunities for play is
physical environments, school playground policies limiting risk to prevent injury, adult fears
related to their understanding of children’s capabilities, and lack of support from the local
social interactions and is often accompanied by repetitive and restrictive interests or behaviors
disability that is usually diagnosed in children prior to the age of 18 and is characterized by
limitations in intellectual function and challenges with developing adaptive behaviors (American
presenting as varying cognitive, social interaction, and physical limitations, all of which are
important to play. Because of these challenges, children with ASD and ID are often viewed as
4
lacking the skills to engage successfully in play and are often not given the opportunity based on
Limiting risky play is not done with mal intent, but instead is an attempt by adult care
providers to limit injury for children with disabilities, those who are considered our most
vulnerable population (Beetham et al., 2019; Newnham, 2000; Wolfensberger, Nirje, Olshansky,
Perske, & Roos, 1972). Without opportunities to take on increasingly complex challenges during
play, children with disabilities miss out on opportunities to develop skills that help them manage
risky play in addition to all of the health and well-being benefits mentioned earlier (Beetham et
al., 2019; Bundy et al., 2015). Unfortunately, adult aversion to risk, sometimes called surplus
safety, results in fewer opportunities for children to engage in risky play- this is especially true
on school playgrounds and for children with disabilities (Brussoni, Gibbons, et al., 2015;
Buchanan, 1999; Bundy et al., 2015; Spencer et al., 2016; Wyver et al., 2010).
In the school setting, recess on the playground is children’s primary opportunity for play
(Ramstetter, Murray, & Garner, 2010). During the school day, recess serves as a much-needed
break from the rigorous requirements placed on children to remain still and maintain cognitive
focus during academic instruction. Educators and experts in child development recommend
protecting a regular opportunity for unstructured outdoor play (Ball, 2002; Ball et al., 2019;
Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Gill, 2018). Unfortunately, even when this unstructured play
time is available, recess is often considered a break for teachers as well as children, rather than
an opportunity to provide quality play experiences. Further, even though the benefits of physical
activity and play are well understood, the time available for engaging in play continues to be cut
back in favor of increasing time for academic instruction (Gill, 2018; Ramstetter et al., 2010).
5
In addition to the emphasis on academics over play, the fear of child injury on the
playground has only contributed to the problem of decreasing opportunities to engage in play,
especially risky play. Policies for playground safety also serve as a significant barrier to risky
play. In many countries, school playgrounds have become very regulated by focusing on the
characteristics of the playground structures and teacher supervision to limit injury. Policies, with
good intentions, focus on removing risk and do not consider the impact to a child’s opportunities
for play or the stress they put on teachers (Ball et al., 2019). Teachers often make decisions about
which play activities to condone and which to interrupt based on their discomfort and fear of
potential consequences for allowing risky play that could be viewed as violating play policy
(Spencer et al., 2016). We must understand the reasoning behind teachers’ fears of situations
they consider risky and support them in shifting their perspectives to promote important learning.
Teachers have an invaluable role in the care of children. They are called to recognize the
unique skills and experiences that shape each child while at the same time recognizing children’s
future potential. The often-opposing responsibilities of teachers to protect children from any
potential of harm while simultaneously providing rich opportunities for learning so children may
develop into independent adults can be summarized by the concepts ‘duty of care’ and ‘dignity
of risk’. The juxtaposition of duty of care and dignity of risk is especially relevant when
considering the responsibilities of teachers working with children with disabilities. Duty of care
is defined by the law in relation to school liability; teachers are responsible to supervise children
and prevent their exposure to situations that could result in injury (Newnham, 2000). Dignity of
risk is the right of a person, especially those with a disability, to experience manageable
exposure to risky situations in order to learn skills for risk management in anticipation of future
6
autonomy (Wolfensberger et al., 1972).
At school, the threat of liability following injury on the school playground is a significant
concern for most teachers. Injury may give rise to charges of negligence or failure to fulfill their
duty of care. However teachers are rarely negligent (Newnham, 2000). Instead, injuries may
result from children seeking exciting play situations when opportunities for play are not
challenging enough and therefore boring (Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015). Fear of liability has
resulted in an imbalance between duty of care and dignity of risk with an emphasis on duty of
care (Little, 2010). Additionally, many teachers simply have their own personal reservations
regarding risk and believe that all risky situations are dangerous, have high likelihood of
resulting in injury, and should be avoided (van Rooijen & Newstead, 2016).
Even teachers who would allow their own children to engage in risky play feel uncertain
when supervising students engaging in risky play on the playground and interrupt the play in
response. The cultural shifts towards risk aversion and surplus safety (Buchanan, 1999; Gill,
2018; van Rooijen & Newstead, 2016; Wyver et al., 2010) make it difficult for teachers to feel
they can allow risky play, without fear of consequence, even if they perceive associated benefits.
In one study, Little (2010) interviewed teachers on their perspectives of risky play and
safety during outdoor play. Even though many teachers acknowledged the value of risky play,
they feared litigation and lack of support from school administrators and child health and safety
regulations Regulatory factors that limit play include playground structure guidelines (e.g.,
height, fall material, surface texture), teacher to child ratios on the playground, and school-
specific play policy like restricting children to only going down a slide, feet first, on their bottom
(Ball, 2002; Little & Eager, 2010; van Rooijen & Newstead, 2016). Little (2010) acknowledged
the contradictory perceptions held by teachers (i.e., on one hand children are competent and
7
capable, but on the other hand, they are vulnerable and teachers must protect them) that cause
conflict between teachers’ pedagogical beliefs and their actions in the face of risk, furthering the
In recent years, with increasing recognition of the benefits of risky, many education
programs have attempted to prioritize opportunities for risky play at school during play outdoors
(Brussoni, Gibbons, et al., 2015; Brussoni et al., 2012; Wyver et al., 2010). Thus, educators need
to adopt a more nuanced view of risk, beyond good or bad, and understand that risky play holds
opportunities for development including confidence, healthy lifestyle, resilience, social skills,
and problem solving (Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Cooke, Wong, & Press, 2019).
Nonetheless, several researchers (Cooke et al., 2019; Spencer et al., 2016) have found that, while
societal understandings of risky play are shifting towards valuing these benefits, the initial
reaction of teachers when supervising a child in a risky play situation is still to interrupt the play
to protect themselves from negative consequences. In order for a child to truly reap the benefits
of learning through the dignity of risk, their care providers must cease overprotection and instead
My research takes place within the context of a larger cluster trial (CT) called The
Sydney Playground Project (SPP). SPP featured a 2-part intervention: (1) introduction of loose
parts materials to school playgrounds and (2) risk-reframing workshops to help adults
acknowledge and change their beliefs of risk with the end goal of promoting risky play (Bundy et
al., 2015). Risk-reframing workshops provided opportunities for adults to discuss their beliefs
about risky play as well as educate them about: its benefits, what to do when faced with
discomfort while supervising play, ways of scaffolding learning for children during risky play,
8
and how to evaluate the severity of a risky play situation (Bundy et al., 2015; Niehues et al.,
2013; Spencer et al., 2016). These trainings helped teachers feel more comfortable with
balancing duty of care and dignity of risk and increasing children’s opportunities for risky play
(Bundy et al., 2015; Niehues et al., 2013; Spencer et al., 2016). Three iterations of the SPP
occurred: a pilot, a cluster randomized trial in mainstream primary schools and a cluster trial in
substantially separate programs for children with autism and intellectual disability in greater
Sydney, Australia.
The SPP team developed an instrument to measure adult risk tolerance and help
determine the impact of the SPP. During the risk reframing sessions of the first CT, the
researchers administered the Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) to teachers and parents (7
out of 100 were teachers) to examine evidence for construct validity and reliability and provide a
baseline measure of their tolerance of risky play (Hill & Bundy, 2014).
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS). The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale
measured caregivers’ tolerance of risk when considering various risky play activities. The
original measure includes several yes/no items based on categories of risky play, risk propensity,
parental overprotection and risk tolerance in supervision and childhood injury risk (Morrongiello
& Corbett, 2006; Sandseter, 2007, 2009). The items included range from “very risky,” and
therefore difficult to endorse, to those that are “not risky at all,” and therefore easy to endorse.
TRiPS produced valid and reliable data (Hill & Bundy, 2014). See Table 1.1 for psychometric
data and Figure 1.1 for a complete list of TRiPS items and their corresponding risky play
categories. During its use in the SPP however, it became clear that this measure was not a good
reflection of the roles of teachers. The responsibilities of teachers and play situations they
supervise while at school differ greatly from those of parents. Additionally, the TRiPS was
9
written in consideration of an individual (or few) child’s play behaviors and is less applicable for
teachers who are responsible for supervising a group of children. There are few instruments
available that evaluate how adult caregivers (parents and teachers) feel about risky play, and
none that evaluate teachers’ tolerance of risky play on the playground. A new measure
specifically for teachers of children of all abilities and for the school playground context is
needed. In order to shift teachers’ perspectives of risk and support them in providing manageable
risky play opportunities for children, researchers require a valid and reliable tool to identify a
teacher’s current tolerance (or intolerance) of risky play. The ability to measure teacher tolerance
of risky play may serve as a mechanism for change by offering opportunities for intervention as
new instrument may support the effort to promote autonomy, health, and well-being for all
children beginning with the ability to accurately measure teacher tolerance of risk in play.
Modifications to the TRiPS (Hill & Bundy, 2014) to better suit the roles and situations of
teachers resulted in a new instrument called the Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-
TRiPS). T-TRiPS will help teachers acknowledge their perceptions of risk and the impact of
their intolerance of risky play on children’s development. The purpose of this research is to
evaluate data produced with the T-TRiPS for evidence of validity and reliability.
10
Table 1.1
Psychometric Properties of the Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) (Hill & Bundy, 2014)
Purpose Description Psychometric data
Measure adult 32 items with Internal validity:
caregiver dichotomous response goodness of fit statistics for all items within
tolerances of (yes / no) ranging from acceptable values:
risk during “very risky” to “not MnSq: <1.5
children’s play risky at all” ZSTD: <2
2
Primary school External validity:
aged children Risk Averse Children 3 to 4 years:
M = -1.62 M = -0.13
SD = 1.68 SD = 1.25
Somewhat Risk Children 5 to 7 years:
Averse M = 0.20
M = -0.37 SD = 1.78
SD = 1.30 Children 8 to 10 years:
Somewhat Risk M = 1.5
Tolerant SD = 1.78
M = 1.20 Children 11 to 13 years:
SD = 1.81 M = 1.95
Risk Tolerant SD = 1.82
M = 2.64
SD = 2.1
Person Separation Index 2.56
Person Reliability Index.87
11
Figure 1.1. Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale Item Map and Corresponding Category of
Risky Play (Hill & Bundy, 2014; Sandseter, 2009)
12
Drawn to Advocate for Risky Play
Before I was an occupational therapy student, I was an early childhood educator for 5
years at a local play-based lab school called the CSU Early Childhood Center (ECC). The ECC
is inspired by the Reggio Emilia approach to teaching which informed my professional practice
and interactions with children. I learned alongside the children, viewing them as capable and
competent, and the environment as an important source of learning that supports or limits
During my time at ECC, I worked with many age groups--the school provides care for
children ages 6 weeks to 6 years--but spent the most time teaching older preschool children: the
4-6-year-olds. I witnessed firsthand the incredible impact play has on learning and development.
For 5 years, I tracked immense change in my students’ development in all educational domains,
from traditional academic skills to motor development to cognitive and social-emotional skills.
Even though I attributed these changes to their engagement in play and the belief of our school
that all children are competent and capable, the limitations of licensing and societal fears
surrounding injury were ever-present and often restricted the play opportunities I was able to
offer my students. Many times, my fear of children becoming injured and the consequences that
could follow, not to mention licensing restrictions, led me to limit play experiences that children
considered valuable and exciting. A key example of this is the opportunity to climb. Even though
we had a wonderful tree in the yard with branches perfectly spaced for young children to climb, I
had to redirect the children to climb on the playground structure because it was approved by
licensing and was considered safe. I understood the value of the challenge that comes with
climbing trees, but I feared children getting hurt and my own liability as a result of such play.
13
development from a new perspective and value it as one of the key occupations of childhood
(Parham & Fazio, 2008). The role of occupational therapists is to empower and support our
clients to participate in the occupations (all the ways people spend their time) that they find
meaningful in order to promote their health, well-being, self-efficacy, and quality of life. Upon
reflection of my time as a teacher from this occupational therapy perspective, I recognize the
barrier to many types of play activities is a serious risk to the health and wellbeing of children.
In a written address to educators in Reggio Emilia, Italy titled “The Image of the Child--
Where Teaching Begins”, Loris Malaguzzi (1994) wrote about the powerful transaction between
children and available environments and opportunities. He called on teachers to recognize the
power of play experiences when children are allowed to be children and engage in social
relationships, experiment with materials, and follow their interests without adult interruption.
“What we want to do is activate within children the desire and will and great pleasure that comes
from being the authors of their own learning” (Malaguzzi, 1994, p. 3). Malaguzzi (1994, p. 5)
concluded his address with a powerful challenge for teachers: “Instead of always giving children
protection, we need to give them the recognition of their rights and of their strengths.” I believe
one way of supporting children’s learning through risky play is by helping teachers gain
knowledge and strategies to face feelings of uncertainty and refrain from interrupting play and,
instead, empower children to acknowledge their own strengths and boundaries related to risk.
All children, regardless of ability and background, have the right to play (U.N.C.R.C.,
1989). During the United Nation’s Convention on the Rights of the Child in 1989, people all over the
world received a call to action to protect this right and provide children with safe and nurturing
opportunities for learning and development through play (U.N.C.R.C., 1989). Through this research,
I respond to this call and uphold my responsibility to advocate for children’s rights to play by arguing
that the best possible opportunity for development lies in benefits gained through opportunities for
14
risky play. I chose to participate in this research to help address this problem and remove barriers
to risky play by contributing a tool that will help teachers think differently about their own
tolerance of risky play and help identify ways to promote this important right for all children.
Research Questions
The aim of this study is to assess the psychometric properties of data gathered with the
Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale for Teachers (T-TRIPS). This aim is guided by the following
research questions:
Overview of Thesis
In this chapter, I presented the background to the research, explained the rationale for and
significance of the study, introduced the research that gave rise to this study and my own
and play as well as instruments to assess the play environment on school playgrounds.
Chapter 3 comprises includes a manuscript formatted for the journal to which I intend to
submit it.
In Chapter 4, I discuss the findings and implications for future research as well as personal
15
CHAPTER 2
LITERATURE REVIEW
This critical synthesis of the literature includes two sections and several sub-sections. The
first evaluates existing adult-report measures related to children’s play and risk. The second
focuses on available observation-based tools that evaluate the school playground and children’s
To find relevant literature for this study, I completed a search of electronic databases,
sections of related literature and theses. Electronic databases that were searched include: ERIC,
CINAHL, MEDLINE, Academic Search Premier, and PubMed. Search terms included: risk,
tolerance, attitudes, perceptions, measure, instrument, play, children, teacher, playground, fear,
risk assessment, risky play, tool, questionnaire, measurement, survey, questionnaire, attitude to
risk, risk reframing, playthings, child, children with disabilities, children with autism. I searched
(https://www.sralab.org/
16
rehabilitation-measures), and Sydney Playground Project (https://www.sydneyplayground
Inclusion criteria for instruments and observation tools in this literature review are: (1)
those intended for use in reference to children 4- to 14-years-of-age, (2) those to be completed by
adult caregivers (teachers and parents) , and (3) those that assess children’s play and play
behaviors, risky play, adult opinions of risky play, adult behaviors that contribute to risk, and
perspectives of the play environment. Few measures and observation tools exist that are
specifically intended for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and intellectual disability
(ID); however, several are appropriate for use with children of all abilities. I included those.
Topics of play and risk that are beyond the scope of this review are those that are:
indirectly relate to play; observational tools focusing only on child physical activity levels; child-
report measures of play and risk tolerance; adult-report measures of a child’s capacity,
playfulness, or skill related to play; and observational tools intended for use anywhere except the
school playground. I was unable to access some instruments mentioned in the literature. I
attempted to access these instruments through interlibrary loan at Colorado State University and
conducted an extensive search of the sites included above; many instruments were published
found during this literature review that examine teacher’s perspectives or beliefs of risky play
through various data collection methods-- interviews, focus groups, email correspondence, and
17
Little, 2010; Little, Sandseter, Hansen, & Wyver, 2012; McFarland & Laird, 2017; Niehues et
al., 2013; Sandseter, 2011; Sandseter, 2012, 2014; Spencer et al., 2016). I mention these studies
only briefly as they, too, are beyond the scope of this review. Researchers studied a variety of
topics related to risky play using qualitative methods to understand teacher perspectives
including: tolerance of risky play by the children in their care, beliefs of the value of risky play,
perspectives of barriers to risky play, and perspectives of consequences associated with risky
play. While these studies align with the purpose of this thesis and offer valuable information on
teacher perspectives of risky play, they did not use a standardized instrument to measure adult
risk-tolerance. Additionally, many of these studies had small sample sizes and included time-
consuming procedures, which limit the generalizability or replicability of these methods in future
studies. Some studies used tools specifically developed for their research including “self-report
scale”, “survey”, or “tool” (Sandseter, 2014), but the majority of these were difficult to find and
if found, they have not been evaluated for their psychometric properties.
I found several adult-report measures that evaluate the behaviors of children and adults
related to play and risk. Few considered risk to be a benefit; most considered risk to be a definite
precursor to injury. Measures that describe risk as detrimental are intended to aid in mitigating
any possibility of risky experience, therefore removing any possibility of injury. Other
instruments assess a child's behaviors as a precursor to injury; some assess adult behaviors and
consider them a precursor to injury. Several measures were intended for a specific age group and
are not appropriate for use with children in a primary school setting; none are intended
18
Two measures I reviewed in this section evaluate two different constructs within the
same instrument: perceptions of risk and adult practices that contribute to risk. Table 2.1 contains
a brief description of these measures along with their psychometric data. While several adult-
report measures of children’s play and risk exist, none of the available instruments assesses
Adults’ fear of the consequences of child injury and a societal tendency towards risk
avoidance has resulted in surplus safety (Buchanan, 1999; Wyver et al., 2010); many measures
were developed that view risk as dangerous and some measure adult perceptions that risks are
detrimental. For example, the Worry Assessment and Risk Estimation Scale (WARE) (Will,
Lorek, Sabo, & Kidd, 2009) considers risk as a direct cause of injury.
Nonetheless, some measures that capture adults’ perspectives that risk could be beneficial
do exist. The Risk Engagement and Protection Survey (REPS) (Olsen, Ishikawa, Masse, Chan, &
Brussoni, 2018), the Parental Perception of Positive Potentiality of Outdoor Autonomy for
Children (PPOAC) (Prezza, Alparone, Cristallo, & Luigi, 2005), and an untitled scale used by
(Sandseter, 2014) all perceive mild risk or risky play as potentially beneficial for child
development and assess adult perceptions of risk from that perspective. The REPS and the
WARE each measure two different constructs, therefore, I discuss them in two sections of this
literature review.
The REPS was administered to fathers visiting the hospital with their child and assesses
two seemingly contrasting concepts (Olsen et al., 2018): a father’s approach to protect his child
from injury and his approach to engaging his child in healthy risk-taking. Olsen et al. (2018)
discovered that while the subscale for each concept is psychometrically sound, the two concepts
19
have a weak correlation and thus belong to two independent constructs. Additionally, REPS
assesses the fathers’ perceptions that risky physical play has developmental value but does not
consider other types of risky play (Sandseter, 2009) and therefore provides an incomplete picture
The PPOAC is intended to assess the degree to which parents perceive their children
playing outdoors alone in their neighborhood is a worthwhile risk that may promote autonomy
(Prezza et al., 2005). While this instrument’s assumption that risky situations could positively
impact child development is promising for shifting perspectives of risky play, it provides only a
limited glimpse into adult perceptions of risk and does not touch on different types of risky play.
Additionally, the PPOAC does not evaluate the adult caregiver’s tolerance of risky play (Prezza
et al., 2005).
Following her work to understand risky play, Sandseter (2014) conducted a new study
using a custom scale to collect data on teacher’s perceptions of risky play. This new scale,
however, has not been proven to produce data that are psychometrically sound and is therefore
Unlike the previous three measures, the WARE assesses levels of parent concern for risk
and the relationship between their concerns and potential injurious situations (Will et al., 2009).
This measure is intended for use with 6-year-old children. Notably, the authors of the WARE
intentionally chose the items to cover a risk continuum, indicating their understanding that risk
has different degrees of severity. However, unlike other positive risk scholars, the authors of the
WARE view risk as detrimental (Ball, 2002; Ball et al., 2012; Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015).
demonstrate some understanding of the benefits of risky play; however, with the exception of the
20
tool used by Sandseter (2013), they only scratch the surface of this important topic. These
measures consider only one type of risky play and or only take a narrow, negative assumption of
risk. This narrow consideration is in stark contrast to risky play scholars’ arguments that (1) there
are various severities of risk and (2) several unique categories of risky play exist, each with
specific benefits to child development (Ball, 2002; Sandseter, 2009). In addition, these measures
are intended to consider children of specific limited age ranges; none are appropriate for all
children in a primary school setting (4-12 years). The scale developed by Sandseter (2013) is the
only tool in this review that specifically measures teacher perspectives of risky play activities in
a school setting; all others are intended for parents in a variety of environments that differ greatly
from the school playground. Unfortunately, Sandseter’s (2013) scale is not a standardized
measure. Instruments like the ones mentioned above are impractical for teachers to use on a
playground because they refer to a much smaller number of children than the entire class for
which the teacher would be responsible. There are significant limitations with all of these
measures, but it is important to acknowledge the efforts of the authors towards valuing the
benefits of risk.
Researchers have attempted to understand how children spend their time and measure the
behaviors children engage in that can potentially result in dangerous situations and injury.
Unfortunately, measures like the Play Activity Questionnaire (PQ) (Finegan, Niccols, Zacher, &
Hood, 1991), the Sensation Seeking Scale (SSSC) (Morrongiello & Lasenby, 2006) and the
Injury Behaviors Checklist (IBC) (Speltz, Gonzales, Sulzbacher, & Quan, 1990) place the cause
of injury within the child rather than an interaction between the child and the physical and social
environment. The PQ (Finegan et al., 1991) is intended to assess children’s preferences for
21
different types of play activities and identify gender preferences for these activities. While this
measure provides adults with a glimpse of how children spend their time and the item ‘rough and
tumble’ play equates to one category of risky play (Sandseter, 2009), the PQ does not consider
The SSSC (Morrongiello & Lasenby, 2006) is grounded in the assumption that sensation
seeking behaviors will result in a child engaging in physically risky play and potentially
becoming injured. The Injury Behaviors Checklist (IBC) (Speltz et al., 1990) also evaluates the
behaviors of a child that could result in injury. Both measures assume the cause of injury resides
within the behaviors and actions of the child and both measures view risky situations as being
inherently negative. Additionally, the PQ and the SSSC are parent-report and are inappropriate
for use by teachers because of the stark differences in activities and environments experienced
between parents and teachers. These measures fall short of addressing adult’s perceptions of
In an attempt to mitigate injury, researchers have tried to understand adults’ roles in risky
situations by measuring parents’ behaviors related to risk and injury. Instruments including the
Parent Supervision Attributes Profile Questionnaire (PASPQ) (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006),
the Supervision Attributes and Risk-taking Questionnaire (SARTQ) (Morrongiello, Corbett, &
Kane, 2011), the Worry Assessment and Risk Estimation Scale (WARE) (Will et al., 2009), and
the ‘protection’ component of the Risk Engagement and Protection Scale (REPS) (Olsen et al.,
2018) place blame on parents for child injury instead of considering the interaction between the
22
The PASPQ is intended for parents of preschool-aged children and the SARTQ is
intended for primary school-aged children. In an attempt to identify potential cause of injury,
both measures evaluate supervision-related behaviors and beliefs of parents as predictors of their
children’s risk of injury (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006; Morrongiello, Corbett, & Kane, 2011).
Notably, both the PASPQ and the SARTQ evaluate parents’ tolerance of their children’s risk-
taking, albeit with a detectable negative perception of risk. Both the PASQ and the SARTQ
consider all risk to be dangerous and detrimental, therefore assuming the need to minimize risk
to protect the child from injury. These measures view the potential cause of injury as residing
within the parent because of their behaviors, even when asked to consider the child’s risky
behaviors.
The other half of the REPS assesses fathers’ approach to protecting their children from
injury (Olsen et al., 2018) by measuring the degree to which fathers engage in various behaviors,
for example, knowing what their child is doing at all times and making sure the home is free of
all hazards (Olsen et al., 2018). While some items in this measure do address father comfort with
risky situations, child injury is the assumed result if a father fails to fully engage in the item
behaviors. There is no middle ground to allow for engaging in manageable risky play.
Similar to previous subsections of this review, these measures have significant limitations
for providing safe opportunities for risky play. A chief issue of these measures is the assumption
that a parent’s behaviors are directly responsible for risk resulting in negative and dangerous
consequences. Additionally, none of the above measures are intended to be used by a teacher in
consideration of a group of children; none of the measures are appropriate for a school
23
Table 2.1
24
Questionnaire of Teachers Assess practitioner’s 11 item scale, each item Not tested
practitioner’s attitudes of risky play with two statements (one
personal risk-taking more risk tolerant, one more
and perceptions of risk averse)
children’s risk-
taking (scale)
(Sandseter, 2014)
Child Play and Behaviors as Predictors of Risk and Injury
Play Activity 4-12 Parents Assess children’s 15 items (active and Moderate level of
Questionnaire (PQ) years interest in different adventurous, athletic, rough- parent agreement
(Finegan, Niccols, types of play and and-tumble, and quiet) r(79) = 0.70, p <
Zacher, & Hood, distinguish between 0.001
1991) boys’ and girls’ 7-point scale α = 0.81
preferred play
activities
25
Injury Behaviors 2-5 Parent or Measure of behavioral 24 items describing various α =0.87
Checklist years teacher characteristics that specific risky behaviors r =0.81 (p < .01)
(Speltz et al., 1990) predict injury in
children. Possible total score range 0- Validity of high injury
96 liability:
M = 33.7
SD = 13.4
Validity of moderate
injury liability:
M = 24.4
SD = 9.7
Validity of low injury
liability:
M = 22
SD = 10.5
26
Adult Practices that Contribute to Risk and Injury
Worry Assessment 6 Assess levels of 21 injury and health hazard Internal Consistency:
and Risk Estimation years parental concern and scenarios, intended to cover α = 0.89
Scale (WARE) perceptions of risk a risk continuum
(Will et al., 2009) associated with injury
hazards. 11-point scale
Parent Supervision 2-5 Parents Assess parent 29 items addressing (1) α = > 0.70 for all
Attributes Profile years supervision in relation protectiveness, (2) subscales
Questionnaire to child risk of injury supervision beliefs, (3) r = > 0.70 for all
(PASPQ) tolerance for children’s risk subscales
(Morrongiello & taking, (4) belief in fate Goodness of Fit Index
Corbett, 2006) determining safety = 0.93
Comparative Fit Index
5-point scale = 0.96
Standardized Root
Mean Square Error of
Approximation = 0.06
x2 = 1.79
Supervision 7-10 Parents Assess perceptions of 3 subscales: Overall α = 0.73
Attributes and Risk- years supervision and risk- - parent need for
taking Questionnaire taking in relation to psychological control C α = 0.72
(SARTQ) risk of injury (C)=15 items
(Morrongiello et al., - beliefs in the value of VS α = 0.78
2011) Assumes the need to supervising children at
minimize risk these ages (VS)= 17 items CRT α = 0.75
- children’s risk-taking
(CRT)= 18 items
5-point scale
*These instruments fit within two categories: adult perceptions of risk and adult practices that contribute to risk
27
Observational Measures of Play and Environment on the School Playground
To understand the role of teachers and risky play experiences on the school playground,
researchers have developed several measures to evaluate affordances of the environment, how
children spend their time, and the dynamic between the two during play at school. I identified
playground observational measures that evaluate characteristics of the play context, measures of
children’s play and behaviors, and two measures specific to children with disabilities. I only
found one measure that acknowledges the existence of risky situations present in the playground
environment and evaluates the impact of those situations on play. I did find one measure that
evaluates play policy and opportunity related to play participation. I identified some measures
that evaluate the play environment and several observational measures that assess children’s
levels of physical activity, types of play, and social interactions on the playground. Table 2.2
comprises a brief description of these measures along with information about psychometrics.
risky play. Nonetheless, these measures provide insights into the play behaviors of children,
which can then help inform opportunities for safely managing risky play (Cooke et al., 2019;
Researchers have attempted to measure the quality of the play environment and the
resulting activities and behaviors of children through use of observation measures like the
School Physical Activity Policy Assessment (S-PAPA) (Lounsbery, McKenzie, Morrow, Holt, &
Budnar, 2013) and the Great Recess Framework Observation Tool (GRF-OT) (Massey, Stellino,
Mullen, Claassen, & Wilkison, 2018). Both measures focus on the availability and quality of
physical and social environment variables and the play activities and behaviors that take place on
28
primary school playgrounds. S-PAPA is unique in that it considers school policies of physical
activity alongside characteristics of physical education and school recess with the intention of
identifying opportunities to increase physical activity (Lounsbery et al., 2013). The GRF-OT is
intended to assess the quality of the playground environment and resulting child play behaviors,
al., 2018). Notably, both measures emphasize the physical components of a playground but only
briefly consider social components such as adult supervision or interaction with peers and
teachers. These measures address important concepts related to children’s opportunities for play
on the playground but fail to provide a complete picture of the characteristics contributing to
Physical play is not the only type of play, nor the most important, that children engage in
on the school playground; however, S-PAPA’s primary outcome is physical activity (Ball et al.,
2012; Gill, 2018; Lounsbery et al., 2013). Additionally, the S-PAPA is concerned about policies
surrounding play and the impact of policies on promoting opportunities for physical activity. The
GRF-OT does expand its focus from physical activity to other play behaviors with peers and
appropriate use of materials; only one item on the GRF-OT evaluates teacher interaction with
children during play, and two items assess behaviors that are included in risky play categories
(Sandseter, 2009)- solving social conflicts with peers and appropriate, and ‘safe’ use of materials
(Massey et al., 2018). An emphasis on safety is evident in both measures and any mention of risk
is considered negative and dangerous; neither include perspectives on the developmental benefits
of risky play.
One other tool is worthy of consideration, however, it is a protocol, not a measure. The
Risk-Benefit Assessment (RBA) (Ball et al., 2012) was developed as a strategy for evaluating the
29
beneficial or detrimental consequences of environmental conditions that are considered risky.
The protocol acknowledges that there are varying degrees of risk and many risky situations
provide opportunities for child development of physical, social, and cognitive skills. While the
Risk-Benefit Assessment documents the assessor's understandings of the pros and cons of risky
play, it is a protocol for assessing the environment, designed to take place before play begins and
All three of these measures focus on the objective details of the playground environment;
however, only the RBA assumes that children want to engage in risky play, and none evaluates
teachers’ perspectives of risky play. An understanding of risky play opportunities afforded by the
environment alone is not enough; instead, adults must gaining an understanding of how children
want to spend their time on the playground which subsequently may assist teachers in identifying
do, both play and non-play, on the school playground. The System for Observing Play and
Leisure Activity in Youth (SOPLAY) (McKenzie, 2002) assesses physical activity level and
group size during play on the playground. The System for Observing Children’s Activities and
Relationships during Play (SOCARP) (Ridgers, Stratton, & McKenzie, 2010) evaluates
children’s physical activity, group size, types of play activities, and social behaviors during play
on the playground. The System for Observing Outdoor Play (SOOP) (Engelen et al., 2017)
measures any change in play following interventions or changes to the playground environment
and also considers peer interaction, types of play activities, and group size. All three of these
measures focus on children’s play as the primary outcome and acknowledge social and physical
30
environmental variables, however, they emphasize how much a child is moving around on the
playground over other important types of play or the quality of the play experience. Only the
SOOP documents the location of children and the presence of teachers on a map, but it does not
evaluate impact of teacher-child interaction on play behaviors. One key limitation of these
measures is that they are time sampling techniques--a researcher documents one area of a
playground in a snapshot of time. While this method is effective for understanding transactional
relationships among children, the environment, and teachers by tracking patterns of play, group
sizes, and materials, it only considers these briefly. None includes types of risky play and none
evaluates teachers’ (or children’s) level of tolerance of risky play on the playground.
Observation measures have been developed that are specifically intended to assess the
play behaviors of children with disabilities, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and intellectual
described a new purpose-built iPad application intended to measure characteristics of play and
play behaviors of children with ASD and ID on school playgrounds; the assessment produced
psychometrically sound data. Like the three observational measures mentioned above, this
measure uses a digitally-prompted time sampling technique but this measure does prompt
examiners to document the play activities, social interactions between peers, and characteristics
of the environment. This measure evaluates the difficulty of the physical and social
characteristics of the playground environment and the ability of the environment to facilitate
various complexities of play behaviors of children with disabilities, but it does not include risky
play specifically. It also does not evaluate teachers’ tolerance of the various play behaviors,
31
One playground observation measure specifically related to children with disabilities is
the Playground Observation Checklist (POC) (Ingram, Mayes, Troxell, & Calhoun, 2007).
Unlike the other measures in this section, this instrument is intended to aid members of a school
disability assessment team in diagnosing children as having ASD or ID based on the play
behaviors they demonstrate on the playground (Ingram et al., 2007). Children with autism have
expected play behaviors according to past research and this measure has strong predictive power
to diagnose autism based on those expectations. That being said, this instrument is not
appropriate for evaluating a child with autism’s preferred play activities on the playground
because it does not document preferences. Furthermore, this instrument, like others, does not
include risky play activities and does not evaluate teachers’ levels of risky play tolerance. Both
of these features could help promote opportunities for children with ASD and ID to engage in
32
Table 2.2
Open-ended, dichotomous,
multichotomies, and checklist
Great Recess Primary Observational tool to assess 17 items of recess Significant overall
Framework school the quality of the playground environment: safety and (p < 0.001)
Observational Tool context (environmental, structure, student behaviors,
(GRF-OT) social, behavioral) and the adult engagement/ Convergent validity:
(Massey et al., 2018) behaviors that take place supervision, transitions. structure and safety
during recess (p < 0.001, β = 0.272)
4-point scale adult engagement/
supervision
(p < 0.001, β = 0.246)
student behaviors
(p = 0.024, β = 0.102)
interrater reliability:
33
strong agreement with an
ICC (2,1) of 0.951 (95%
CI, 0.932, 0.964).
Test-retest reliability:
(ICC = 0.949, 95% CI,
0.882, 0.979) w/ 3-day
average across 2 time point
Risk-benefit n/a Protocol to evaluate the Four levels: (1) policy Not reported
Assessment potential risks and associated framework, (2) risk-benefit
(Ball et al., 2012) benefits of environmental assessment, (3) technical
conditions and paly activities inspection, (4) dynamic risk-
on playgrounds . Used to benefit assessment.
determine if play policy
needs to change to mitigate Keep detailed documentation
risk. of evaluation of risks and
associated benefits to make
Teachers, school admin, judgements about the play.
policy makers
Children’s Play on the Playground
System for Observing School- Systematic observation tool Momentary time sampling Interrater reliability:
Play and Leisure aged (K-12) to measure the number of technique: observe activity of contextual variables
Activity in Youth students and their levels of participating children = 88-97%
(SOPLAY) physical activity during play (‘sedentary’, ‘walking’, or intraclass correlations
(McKenzie, 2002) on the playground ‘very active’ and type of = 0.76 - 0.98
activity), alternating
boys/girls Interobserver agreements:
(IOA=80%)
Contextual conditions: time,
temp, area/ equipment Intraclass correlations:
accessibility, presence of (R=0.75)
supervision, presence/ type of
organized activity
34
System for Observing School- Systematic observation tool Time sampling technique: 10 Interrater reliability = 88-
Children’s Activities aged (K-12) to assess children’s physical sec observation followed by 90%
and Relationships activity, social group size, 10 sec record period for each
during Play types of activities, and social child Concurrent validity with
(SOCARP) behaviors during play accelerometry = r =0.67, p
(Ridgers et al., 2010) <0.01
System for Observing Primary Systematic observation Counter-clockwise scanning Interrater reliability = 95%
Outdoor Play school-aged instrument to assess changes areas of playground 1 x/ min
(SOOP) in social and creative play for 20 min
(Engelen et al., 2017) and use of loose parts recorded on map:
materials. Documented: - number of kids (boys/girls)
number of children playing - presence of teachers
together, number of children
in an area, types of 10 categories (active play;
activities/play construction; creative/
imaginative play; eating,
Assess play and non-play moving-not playing; inactive
activities, groups, number of (sitting or standing) play;
children playing together, and inactive – not social (alone or
presence of teachers. no interaction); and inactive
– social, sports)
Play for Children with ASD / ID
*Purpose-built iPad Primary Observation tool to measure 19 item tool Infit MnSq = 0.83-1.19
application to school-aged characteristics of play and Researcher scored one Outfit MnSq = 0.46-1.45
measure what play behaviors of children quadrant of the playground at
children do on the with autism and intellectual a time, moving clockwise Point Measure Correlation
playground disabilities on the school = 0.11-0.61
(Grady-Dominguez et playground
al., 2019) Test reliability = 0.60-0.67
35
Playground 5 – 11 years Assess children’s play and Completed by members of Interrater reliability: 100%
Observation social behaviors in school assessment team.
Checklist (POC) playground environments (F = 102.7, p < 0.0001)
(Ingram et al., 2007) 10 item checklist
Tool to aid identification of (engages in social play; not Positive predictive power
children with disabilities socially isolated; respects for autism: 69%
(ASD and ID) boundaries/personal space;
not exhibit socially ‘Uses playground
inappropriate behavior; equipment functionally’
follows game rules; responds did not differentiate
to winning/losing; initiates between groups (x2= 0.3–
conversation w/ peer; 4.2, p > 0.04)
sustains conversation w/
peer; does not exhibit gross
motor incoordination; uses
playground equipment
functionally)
36
Conclusion
tolerance of risky play in a school setting. Few existing measures have a positive perspective of
risky play and consider the potential benefits risky play can have on child development (Olsen et
al., 2018; Prezza et al., 2005). Many measures identify the cause of risk and injury as originating
within the behaviors of the child or parent and categorize risk as a negative, dangerous, and an
inevitable precursor to child injury (Morrongiello & Corbett, 2006; Morrongiello, Corbett, &
Kane, 2011; Will et al., 2009). This latter category of measures assumes all possibilities of
observation measures in an attempt to identify any that could provide insight into the transaction
between children’s play and characteristics of the environment, available materials, opportunities
for peer interaction, supervision, and activities. Only one protocol for evaluating the play
environment included an assessment of the possible benefits and detriments of risk and risky
play (Ball et al., 2012). Other playground-specific measures are intended to assess activity levels,
types of play, and social interactions of children during recess, but do not identify opportunities
for risky play (Engelen et al., 2017; Grady-Dominguez et al., 2019; Massey, Stellino, Hayden, &
Thalken, 2019; McKenzie, 2002; Ridgers et al., 2010). I found only two observation measures
specific to children with disabilities for use on school playgrounds (Grady-Dominguez et al.,
2019; Ingram et al., 2007). One measure is intended to aid in diagnosis of ASD or ID though
observation of play on the playground and is therefore not relevant to my research questions.
One playground measure specifically evaluates the school’s play policies related to child play
behaviors and availability of play resources, but it does not assess teacher tolerance of risk or the
37
impact of teacher tolerance on children’s opportunities for risky play (Lounsbery et al., 2013).
While many available playground observation measures consider the characteristics of the
environment in relation to play, they fail to consider the role of the environment in providing
Available research on risky play advocates for offering manageable risky play
experiences to children of all abilities on the school playground, but also identifies teacher risk
aversion, fear, and lack of understanding of how to offer manageable risks as a barrier to this
important developmental opportunity (Ball et al., 2012; Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Brussoni
et al., 2012; Little et al., 2012; Sandseter, 2011; Sandseter, 2012, 2014; Wyver et al., 2010).
Unfortunately, there are no measures currently available that appropriately evaluate teacher
tolerance of risky play on the school playground in order to help teachers reframe their
understanding of risk and develop strategies for implementing safe risky play. The shortcomings
of available measures indicate there is a gap in the literature- there is a need for a new measure
for primary school teachers, in reference to children of all abilities, and one that is appropriate
for use in the unique physical, social, and behavioral context of a school playground. The
Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-TRiPS) was developed from the original TRiPS (Hill
& Bundy, 2014) to fill this gap and establish a means for evaluating teacher tolerance of risk in
play. This instrument must be evaluated to determine if the data produced suggests evidence for
tolerance of risk in play will help increase opportunities for risky play experiences for children
38
CHAPTER 3
JOURNAL ARTICLE
Much of children’s active play inherently holds the possibility of minor injuries, like
bumps and bruises, and thus is considered to be “risky.” Risky play includes exciting and
thrilling activities, involves exploration of physical and social environments, and includes
exploration of emotions and perceptions of the world (Ball, 2002; Kleppe et al., 2017; Lester &
Russell, 2014; Sandseter, 2009). Because risky play includes potential injury, it requires children
and adults to accept uncertainty and relinquish control (Ball, 2002; Kleppe et al., 2017; Lester &
Previous researchers have identified the following risky play categories: (a) great heights,
(b) high speeds, (c) dangerous tools, (d) dangerous elements, (e) rough-and-tumble, (f)
disappear/get lost, (g) play with impact, and (h) vicarious risk (Kleppe et al., 2017; Sandseter,
2009). The above categories focus on physical play; however, other risky play activities exist
that do not fit within these categories such as challenges during social problem solving or
While many adults attempt to avoid any possibility of injury for children, engaging in
risky play promotes development including physical activity, executive functioning, skills for
et al., 2015; Brussoni et al., 2012; International School Grounds Alliance, 2017; Wyver et al.,
2010). Children of all abilities develop an understanding of their own skills and boundaries
through opportunities to independently manage play (Ball, 2002). Challenging play experiences
39
help children become aware of, and increase their competencies for, managing risky situations
without relying on adults (Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Bundy et al., 2015).
Alternatively, dignity of risk implies the right to gain skills to manage risk and develop
autonomy (Wolfensberger et al., 1972). At school, recess provides an essential opportunity for
play that supports development of autonomy (Bundy et al., 2015; Hyndman, 2015; Ramstetter et
al., 2010). However, teachers’ fears of liability (Newnham, 2000; Niehues et al., 2013) often lead
to play supervision practices that reflect an imbalance between duty of care and dignity of risk.
playgrounds (Spencer et al., 2016). Even though many teachers recognize benefits associated
with risky play, fear of the consequences of child injury often results in attempts to eliminate any
form of risk, no matter how minor, without regard for possible benefits (Little , 2016; Wyver et
al., 2010). Consequently, children miss opportunities to experience its benefits (Ball et al., 2019;
Newnham, 2000; U.N.C.R.C., 1989). While most people who work with children have children’s
best interests at heart and enact protection strategies with good intentions, teachers’ tendency
towards surplus safety, fear of social consequences and lack of understanding of the benefits
Risky play situations on school playgrounds are especially limited for children with
disabilities. Children with disabilities, particularly those with cognitive limitations such as
judgement, inhibition management, and problem solving (e.g., children with autism spectrum
40
developmentally important risky play than do their typically-developing peers (Bundy et al.,
2015).
In order to develop the important skills associated with risky play, children with
difficulty (Bundy et al., 2015). Thus, risky play is especially important for children with
disabilities (Bundy et al., 2015; Gilman, 2007). Nonetheless, despite its benefits, many adults
perceive risky play as dangerous, unnecessary, and offering only negative consequences,
particularly for children with disabilities. Low tolerance of risky play leads adults to limit
opportunities for these developmentally meaningful play experiences (Beetham et al., 2019;
Bundy et al., 2015; Little et al., 2012; Scott, Jackson, & Backett-Milburn, 1998)
In a study of teachers’ responses to risky play while supervising children with disabilities
on the playground, Spencer et al. (2016) found that adults often justified their interruptions of
play by citing duty of care coupled with the perception that children with disabilities are
Findings from this study suggested not only that teachers view a child’s disability as a potential
risk for injury but also that teachers are strongly influenced by fears of social consequences such
as having to report injuries to parents or being perceived as negligent (Spencer et al., 2016).
Consequently, teachers often determine safety rules based on the least capable child in the group
and apply these to the entire group, eliminating any opportunity for children to develop new risk
management skills regardless of individual children’s varying capabilities (Ball et al., 2012;
Risk-benefit analysis allows teachers to consider the benefits and dangers of various play
41
situations. Ball and colleagues (2012) defined a difference between “good risks” (i.e.,
experiences that hold developmental value and have few unforeseen negative outcomes) and
“bad risks” (i.e., experiences that are too complex for children to assess independently and have
no developmental benefit like broken, sharp objects). Opportunities for good risk not only
promote development but help to mitigate injury. Attempting to mitigate or eliminate risk not
only has negative consequences for development, but often results in boredom, leading children
to seek out exceedingly risky play (Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Brussoni et al., 2012;
Christensen & Mikkelsen, 2008; Wyver et al., 2010). Thus, risk avoidance strategies can result in
more serious injury than supporting children to manage risks through appropriate risky play
opportunities.
Teachers are the gatekeepers of children’s opportunities for, and access to, risky play at
school. Healthy teacher tolerance of risky play is important for promoting developmental
opportunities on the playground. A valid and reliable instrument to accurately measure teacher
tolerance of risky play would help teachers understand their risk tolerance and measure change
as needed. A review of available literature revealed few instruments to evaluate adult caregivers’
perceptions of risky play, and none to measure teachers’ tolerance. One instrument, the
Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) (Hill & Bundy, 2014), measures parents’ tolerance of
risk in play. While the TRiPS produced valid and reliable data for parents, it is not suitable for
use with teachers as many items do not reflect the kind of play that occurs at school (Hill &
Bundy, 2014). Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine evidence for construct
validity and reliability of the Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-TRiPS).
42
Method
This study was part of a larger investigation known as the Sydney Playground Project
(SPP) (Bundy et al., 2015). SPP comprised two interventions: risk reframing for adult
participants and addition of loose materials to the school playground. Approval for this research
was granted by the Human Research Ethics Committee at The University of Sydney and the trial
was registered with the Australian and New Zealand Clinical Trial Registry
Participants
A total of 99 preschool and primary school teachers of children, ages 4-12 years and
diagnosed with ASD and/or ID, completed the instrument. Teachers ranged in age from younger
than 20 years to older than 56 years and included lead and assistant teachers. Duration of
teachers’ experience teaching children with disabilities ranged from 4 months to 29 years (M =
10.14; SD = 4.99). See Table 3.1. Of the 99 participants, 48.4% reported growing up in Australia.
43
Table 3.1
< 1- 4 yrs. 30
4 - 6 yrs. 3
7 - 9 yrs. 10
10 - 12 yrs. 4
multiple 13
5 - 9 yrs. 29
4 - 6 yrs. 4
7 - 9 yrs. 9
10 - 12 yrs. 2
multiple 14
10 -19 yrs. 28
4 - 6 yrs. 7
7 - 9 yrs. 6
10 - 12 yrs. 1
multiple 14
≥ 20 yrs. 11
4 - 6 yrs. 1
7 - 9 yrs. 1
10 - 12 yrs. 2
multiple 7
unknown 1
7 - 9 yrs. 1
Note: Teacher experience M=10.14; SD=4.99
Instrument
The T-TRiPS includes 25 items with dichotomous answers (find abbreviated items Table
3.2 and full instrument in the Appendix) and 5 short answer questions probing teachers’ personal
experiences of risky play. Items reflect the six categories of risky play identified by Sandseter
(2009) (great heights, high speed, dangerous tools, dangerous elements, rough-and-tumble,
typically is completed while teachers are not on the playground but considering the play of the
44
children in their classroom.
The Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-TRiPS) was adapted from the original
Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (TRiPS) (Hill & Bundy, 2014) by modifying or removing items
as they apply to the context of the teacher. The T-TRiPS items reflect activities teachers
commonly supervise their students doing during a school day. The TRiPS produced valid and
reliable data. All items fit the expectations of the Rasch model and unexplained variance in a
principal components analysis of residuals was only the strength of about 2 ½ items,
data was demonstrated by a person reliability index of .87 and a person separation index value of
2.63, indicating the instrument is capable of separating participants into >2 levels of risk
tolerance.
45
Table 3.2
Procedure
Participants completed the T-TRIPS during the pre-intervention phase of SPP risk
reframing workshops. Teachers chose between an electronic (iPad with the instrument
assistant entered data from paper assessments into the Survey Monkey platform.
46
Data Analysis
Rasch analysis was employed using Winsteps version 4.3.1 software (Linacre, 2018) to
transform raw data to interval-level data and examine the evidence for construct validity and
internal reliability. The analysis reveals the degree to which data from items and people fit the
assumptions of the Rasch model. As applied to this study, (1) easier items (reflecting less risk)
are easier for all people to approve; and (2) participants with greater tolerance will be more likely
to endorse more risky items than people with less tolerance (Bond & Fox, 2015).
Construct Validity
Rasch allows for consideration of one human attribute, or latent trait, (i.e., the
“construct,” in this case, tolerance of risky play). The model organizes items and people along a
single continuum based on strength of the latent trait within each item and person (Bond & Fox,
2015). Several findings from Rasch analysis were examined. To determine if data from
individual items corresponded positively with total measure scores, point measure correlations
(PMC) were examined and only items with positive PMCs, indicating the items contribute
positively to the construct, were retained. Goodness of fit statistics, expressed in mean square
(MnSq) and z (ZSTD) values, provide evidence of the degree to which the items conform to the
expectations of the Rasch model. MnSq values between 0.5 and 1.5 and ZSTD values ± 2 were
accepted; the researchers expected that 95% of items would meet those criteria.
To determine the degree to which the range of T-TRiPS item difficulty matched the range
of teacher tolerance, the Wright Map - a visual representation of the item and person hierarchies-
was examined. The researchers expected to find similar item and participant means and ranges
and small gaps between item difficulty. Large gaps in the item hierarchy suggest that the items
do not adequately cover the range of risk tolerance observed in the sample population and
47
cannot, therefore, yield precise measurement of teacher tolerance at the level of a gap.
Correspondingly, items that are very close together along the construct represent a similar level
of risk tolerance and may therefore provide redundant information. See Figure 3.1. The
researchers also examined the hierarchy of the items to determine if they represent a logical
In order to test the strength of any additional dimensions in the data, Winsteps provides a
with each contrast reveal the strength of a second underlying construct. Eigenvalues less than 3
(i.e., the strength of three items) were accepted as evidence that any underlying construct is not
Internal Reliability
Two sources of evidence for internal reliability were considered: strata, calculated from
the person separation index, and the person reliability index. Strata indicate the levels of teacher
tolerance of risky play revealed in the data; at least two levels are needed. Strata were calculated
using the equation (H= (4G+1)/3), where H = strata and G = person separation index. The person
reliability index, a Rasch equivalent to Cronbach’s alpha, suggests the ease with which measures
would be reproduced with a similar sample. Person reliability index scores ≥0.8 were accepted.
Results
The evidence for construct validity of the data was excellent. Specifically, each item
correlated positively (PMC) with overall measure scores. (See Table 3.3). Further, goodness of
fit statistics for all 25 items were within the acceptable range, indicating that the items conform
48
Table 3.3
Infit Outfit
As shown in Figure 3.1, the mean of item difficulty and teacher tolerance were similar
(item mean = 0; person mean = 0.19); while the person range (-4.09 – 4.56) was greater than the
item range (-3.76 – 3.11), most teachers’ scores fell within the item range. Items expected to be
more difficult to endorse did, in fact, earn higher measure scores, indicating logic of the
construct. See Table 3.3. However, some gaps existed along the item hierarchy and 8 items
49
achieved the same level of difficulty (i.e., located on the same line), suggesting item redundancy.
The PCA revealed that person and item measures explained 48.5% of variance in TRiPS scores.
After removing the Rasch construct, the largest remaining contrast had an eigenvalue of 2.29,
The evidence for internal reliability was also strong. T-TRiPS data yielded 3.01 strata,
suggesting that the instrument reliably discriminated more than 3 levels of tolerance of risky
50
Figure 3.1. T-TRiPS Person-Item Map (Wright Map)
51
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to examine construct validity and internal reliability of data
collected with T-TRiPS, an instrument to measure teacher tolerance of risky play. Results
suggested the strength of the instrument; all items contribute to a logical unidimensional
construct. Specifically, Rasch analysis revealed that all items form a logical hierarchy and
The T-TRiPS item hierarchy reflected risky play literature. Items located toward the top
of the construct (Figure 3.1) are, understandably, much more difficult for teachers to endorse
(i.e., “run in a place with an open fire”, “use adult tools unsupervised”, and “climb as high as
they wanted in a tree”). Our findings supported Cevher-Kalburan’s (2014) research in which
many teachers identified children’s play with “dangerous tools” and “dangerous elements” as
more difficult to endorse than other risky play activities. Endorsing items located in the middle
of the T-TRiPS construct (i.e., “ride a bicycle down a steep hill”, “walk on a slippery surface”,
“rough and tumble”, “climb a tree within your reach”, and “resolve disagreements independently
if not hitting”) requires moderate risk tolerance. Endorsing items at the bottom of the hierarchy,
represented in Figure 3.1, (i.e., “continue playing challenging activities if they may not
succeed?”, “continue playing after a scrape”, and “play chase with one another”) requires very
little risk tolerance. A teacher whose measure score is at the bottom of the range is quite risk
Findings from a study of teacher responses to uncertainty while supervising children with
disabilities on the playground suggested teachers have greater difficulty tolerating risky play in
groups of children compared to risky play by a single child (Spencer et al., 2016). Interestingly,
according to the item hierarchy represented in Figure 3.1, group play items were fairly evenly
52
distributed along the continuum with some like “play chase with others” fairly easy to endorse,
others involving social problem-solving like “resolving disagreements” both with and without
physical contact and “rough and tumble play” posing moderate difficulty to endorse. Supporting
the findings of Spencer et al. (2016), Cevher-Kalburan (2014) found teachers are uncomfortable
with possible consequences of risks, especially “hurting themselves and peers”. The two
difficult-to-endorse play-fight items support this research as they are located at the top of the
Several items are located on the same line along the construct indicating that endorsing
them requires the same level of risk tolerance. However, the items represent different categories
of risky play, therefore, we recommend retaining all items. For example, the items “go headfirst
down a slide” and “resolve disagreements independently if not hitting” are located on the same
difficulty level. While these two items require the same level of tolerance of risk to endorse, they
Findings demonstrated the T-TRiPS is able to separate participants into three levels of
risk tolerance: high, moderate, and low. This strength of the T-TRiPS to determine the level of a
teacher’s tolerance of risky play will allow researchers and interventionists to differentiate
between participants more precisely than simply determining who is more or less tolerant. The
ability to identify risk averse teachers from those with moderate and even high tolerance will
measure change in the future. Additionally, the strong person reliability index indicated
researchers can expect future research with new participants (e.g. teachers of children of varying
abilities or from different regions of the world) will yield similar results.
53
Recommendations to Strengthen T-TRiPS
Findings from this research serve as evidence of the strength of the T-TRiPS as a valid
and reliable instrument that will provide researchers and practitioners with the important
opportunity to measure teacher tolerance of risk in play. The following recommendations for
improving this already strong instrument will support accuracy of measurement and provide
While items are logically distributed along the construct, there is one notable gap along
the hierarchy (Figure 3.1) suggesting addition of new items is warranted. Gaps in item
distribution represent points where precise measurement of a teacher’s tolerance is not possible.
The most concerning gap is located approximately 1 standard deviation below the item mean
(0.0) between item 13, “care for pets in classroom,” and item 18, “wait to see how students
manage challenges before getting involved.” Several participants have risky play tolerance at the
same level as this gap, represented in Figure 3.1 by ‘X’, indicating they were not measured
precisely. Participants at this level of the construct are fairly intolerant of risks and could
therefore be an appropriate target group for intervention to improve risk tolerance. Introduction
of new items or modifying existing items to fill these gaps will improve measurement precision
Another example of a gap, but one that is not very concerning, is located nearly one
standard deviation above the item mean (S), between item 21, “play on the playground
this gap (and the two smaller gaps above) are already fairly risk tolerant, there is less need to
54
Many T-TRiPS items reflect the categories of risky play identified by Sandseter (2009),
see Table 3.2. However, the item “use adult tools unsupervised” is the only item representing the
category “dangerous tools” and is located more than one standard deviation above the mean
indicating it is fairly difficult to endorse. Another easier item representing this category would be
beneficial.
Kleppe et al. (2017) defined two new risky play categories, “play with impact” and
“vicarious risk” after this instrument was developed; therefore, these categories are not
represented. Assuming such items fit within the same unidimensional construct, new items
representing these categories as well as items reflecting social, emotional, and intellectual risk,
might provide more thorough information on teacher tolerance of risk in play. Including new,
non-physical items in the T-TRiPS (e.g. social conflict or trying novel activities) could support
the growing body of knowledge of risky play. This research is especially important as children
with disabilities may struggle with these types of play and would benefit from opportunities to
An additional opportunity to improve the T-TRiPS lies in modifying the language of the
instructions to consider the majority of students. Currently the instructions state “Think about
what you would do for the majority of your students when responding to each question” (see
Appendix). While the concept of considering the majority is clear enough, we do not know the
extent to which teacher responses were influenced by the least capable students. Safety rules are
often determined by the capabilities of the least skilled child in the group with blanket
expectations given to the whole. “If this child cannot do a particular activity, then no one should
be allowed to do it.” With more restrictions on play, more skilled children may seek excessively
risky situations to combat boredom, resulting in injury. I propose modifying the instructions of
55
the T-TRiPS (Appendix) from “think about the majority of your students” to something more
explicit like “think about the majority of your students excluding the most and least capable” and
include “majority of students” within each T-TRiPS item. This simple modification will help
guide teachers to consider the whole group when completing the T-TRiPS and consider the
Limitations
While findings from this research provide strong evidence that the T-TRiPS produces
data that are valid and reliable, like all research, this study had limitations. Firstly, the sample
used in this study is fairly small and only included teachers of children with ASD and/or ID.
Additional research is needed with a broader range of teachers of children both with disabilities
and those who are typically developing. Secondly, the T-TRiPS used in this study did not ask for
teacher or student gender. Not knowing if participants and children were male or female
prevented us from determining the extent to which gender was a factor in teacher tolerance of
risk. Sandseter (2014) indicated male teachers experience greater excitement when engaging in
risks and are therefore more risk tolerant than female teachers. Additionally, adults are often
more tolerant of risky play by male children than female children (Sandseter, 2014).
Future research is needed to create additional items and subsequently ensure the revised
T-TRiPS produces data that represent a unidimensional construct. Another possible opportunity
for future research lies in creating an abbreviated version of the T-TRiPS to use as a screening
tool. Participant scores on a brief screening tool could help researchers, interventionists, and
teachers identify those at greatest risk of hindering children’s right to risky play and therefore
56
Conclusions
The strength of these findings suggest the T-TRiPS can be used not only to help teachers
understand their own risk tolerance, but also as a basis for intervention to help them shift
perspectives and increase risk tolerance. Items located just above a teacher’s tolerance level can
provide opportunities for goals and ideas for intervention to improve risk tolerance. Additionally,
these findings suggest T-TRiPS could be used in future research to provide insights into short-
57
CHAPTER 4 REFLECTIONS AND CONCLUSIONS
Completing this thesis not only reinforced many concepts from my coursework but also
provided me with knowledge and skills that will strengthen my future career as an occupational
therapist. Through this research I have learned that several factors influence teacher tolerance of
risk in play as well as the importance of advocating for opportunities to engage in risk for all
children through compassionate education of teachers. This research is an important step towards
Several notable findings support Teacher Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (T-TRiPS) as
an instrument that produces psychometrically sound data. All items measure a unidimensional
construct of tolerance of risk in play. A variety of play activity items represent a hierarchy of
difficulty ranging from ‘easy to endorse’ to ‘difficult to endorse.’ The placing of these items
along the hierarchy is consistent with previous risky play research and is representative of
activities teachers often supervise on the playground. T-TRiPS reliably discriminated more than
three levels of tolerance of risky play (low, moderate, and high) which may make it sensitive to
The ability to measure teacher tolerance of risk in play has important implications for
child development and opens several opportunities for children of all abilities. Children with
disabilities often are considered more vulnerable than their typically developing peers, increasing
the already over-protective practices of teachers designed to prevent injury on the school
playground (Spencer et al., 2016). Being able to assess a play opportunity and decide how to
engage while best managing risk is a protective factor; children with that ability have learned the
boundaries of their own skills (Brussoni et al., 2012; Christensen & Mikkelsen, 2008).
58
Adults are responsible for providing opportunities for development through play,
however adults’ misunderstanding of risky play, perceptions of the limited abilities of children,
and fear of consequences to themselves as well as children interfere with their providing risky
play opportunities (Ball et al., 2019; Brussoni, Brunelle, et al., 2015; Lester & Russell, 2014;
Spencer et al., 2016). T-TRiPS can help adults reflect on their own tolerance of risk and may
ultimately help them shift these perspectives and increase opportunities for risky play
experiences.
Future Research
While findings from this research demonstrated the strength of this instrument, there are a
few opportunities for modification to improve the instrument. One notable gap is present in the
item hierarchy at a level that would require fairly low tolerance of risk in play to endorse items.
The lack of items in this region of the hierarchy makes it impossible to precisely measure
teachers whose tolerance falls in this general range. Therefore, I propose new items be added to
fill this gap. Through my review of risky play literature and reviewing the results, I also propose
adding new items that represent risky play categories ‘play with impact’ and ‘vicarious risk’ that
Kleppe and colleagues identified after the T-TRiPS was developed. Additionally, I would modify
some existing items to be either easier or more difficult to endorse by adjusting characteristics
like level of adult supervision (e.g. “use adult tools while you are close by” or “use adult tools
while you provide hand-over-hand support”) and involvement of peers (e.g. “go head-first down
a slide without peers close by” or “go head-first down a slide with peers playing at the bottom”).
I also would modify the instructions of the instrument to help participants consider the whole
group of children rather than the least or most capable child in their care. This effort may help
shift away from the tendency to make safety rules for the group based on the capabilities of one
59
child. I would include an opportunity for participants to self-identify their gender as well as
report the gender of the children in their care in order to determine the extent to which gender
may play a role in tolerance of risky play. Finally, while our small sample size is acceptable for
Rasch analysis, a larger sample size of teachers from a variety of backgrounds teaching a variety
of children will help continue this research and support the goal of ultimately using the T-TRiPS
in practice. Overall, our findings suggest this instrument produces psychometrically sound data
and will serve as an important tool to help teachers understand their tolerance of risk in play and
serve as a mechanism for change to improve opportunities for all children to engage in risky play
experiences.
Personal Reflections
After teaching young children and experiencing my own fears of social consequences and
litigation, this research helped me understand the profound impact gaining knowledge can have
childhood education center helped me view children as competent and capable, but I found I did
not always practice this belief. My hope is that this instrument will help others acknowledge their
own barriers to risky play tolerance and work to shift their perspectives and provide the children
Through this thesis process and learning about the many factors that influence teacher
tolerance of risk in play, I am inspired to share my findings from the T-TRiPS and support
teachers in acknowledging and changing their perspectives of risky play. Through my role as an
occupational therapist, I plan to partner with adults who supervise play to educate them on the
importance of risk in play to develop children’s skills. In the future I would like to present at
conferences and schools to teachers and other professionals working with children on the
60
benefits of risky play. In these situations, I hope to use the T-TRiPS to measure their tolerance of
risk in play and provide opportunities to shift their perspectives of risky play through workshops
and discussions of risk. I will use the knowledge I’ve gained through this research to continue re-
61
REFERENCES
Ball, D. (2002). Playgrounds- risks, benefits, and choices Ball, D..pdf>. Retrieved from
Ball, D., Brussoni, M., Gill, T., Harbottle, H., & Spiegal, B. (2019). Avoiding a dystopian future
doi:10.1080/21594937.2019.1582844
Ball, D. J., Gill, T., & Spiegal, B. (2012). Managing risk in play provision: Implementation
provision-implementation-guide.aspx
Beetham, K. S., Sterman, J., Bundy , A. C., Wyver , S., Ragen, J., Engelen, L., . . . Naughton, G.
(2019). Lower parent tolerance of risk in play for children with disability than typically
doi:10.1080/21594937.2019.1643980
Bond, T. G., & Fox, C. M. (2015). Applying the Rasch Model: Fundamental measurement in the
Brussoni, M., Brunelle, S., Pike, I., Sandseter, E. B., Herrington, S., Turner, H., . . . Ball, D. J.
(2015). Can child injury prevention include healthy risk promotion? Inj Prev, 21(5), 344-
347. doi:10.1136/injuryprev-2014-041241
Brussoni, M., Gibbons, R., Gray, C., Ishikawa, T., Sandseter, E. B., Bienenstock, A., . . .
Tremblay, M. S. (2015). What is the relationship between risky outdoor play and health
62
in children? A Systematic Review. International Journal of Environmental Research and
Brussoni, M., Olsen, L. L., Pike, I., & Sleet, D. A. (2012). Risky play and children's safety:
Buchanan, C. (1999). Building Better Playgrounds: A project for parents? UAB Magazine, 19(3).
Bundy , A., Wyver , S., Beetham, K., Ragen, J., Naughton, G., Tranter, P., . . . Sterman, J.
(2015). The Sydney playground project- levelling the playing field: a cluster trial of a
Bundy , A. C., Naughton, G., Tranter, P., Wyver , S., Baur, L., Schiller, W., . . . Brentnall, J.
(2011). The sydney playground project: popping the bubblewrap - unleashing the power
intervention aiming to increase children’s physical activity and social skills. BMC Public
doi:10.1080/14729679.2014.950976
Christensen, P., & Mikkelsen, M. R. (2008). Jumping off and being careful: children's strategies
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9566.2007.01046.x
63
Cooke, M., Wong, S., & Press, F. (2019). Towards a re-conceptualisation of risk in early
doi:10.1177/1463949119840740
Engelen, L., Wyver , S., Perry, G., Bundy , A., Chan, T. K. Y., Ragen, J., . . . Naughton, G.
(2017). Spying on children during a school playground intervention using a novel method
for direct observation of activities during outdoor play. Journal of Adventure Education
Finegan, J., Niccols, G. A., Zacher, J. E., & Hood, J. E. (1991). The play activity questionnaire:
Finegan, J. K., Niccols, G. A., Zacher, J. E., & Hood, J. E. (1991). The Play Activity
Garwood, S. G. (1982). Piaget and play: Translating theory into practice. Topics in Early
Gill, T. (2018). Playing it Safe? A global white paper on risk, liability and children's play in
public space.
Gilman, S. (2007). Including the Child With Special Needs: Learning From Reggio Emilia.
Grady-Dominguez, P., Bundy , A., Ragen, J., Wyver , S., Villeneuve, M., Naughton, G., . . .
79-93. doi:10.1080/21594937.2019.1580340
64
Hill, A., & Bundy , A. C. (2014). Reliability and validity of a new instrument to measure
tolerance of everyday risk for children. Child: Care, Health, and Development, 40(1), 68-
76. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2214.2012.01414.x
Hyndman, B. (2015). Where to next for school playground interventions to encourage active
doi:10.1080/19411243.2015.1014956
Ingram, D. H., Mayes, S. D., Troxell, L. B., & Calhoun, S. L. (2007). Assessing children with
autism, mental retardation, and typical development using the Playground Observation
International School Grounds Alliance. (2017). Declaration on Risk in Play and Learning.
Kleppe, R., Melhuish, E., & Sandseter, E. B. H. (2017). Identifying and characterizing risky play
in the age one-to-three years. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal,
Lester, S., & Russell, W. (2014). Turning the world upside down: Playing as the deliberate
Linacre, J. M. (2018). Winsteps Rasch Measurement Computer Program: User's Guide (Vol.
Lindqvist, P., Nordänger, U. K., & Landahl, J. (2009). Insurance and Assurance: Teachers'
Strategies in the Regimes of Risk and Audit. European Educational Research Journal,
Little, H. (2010). Finding the balance: Early Childhood practitioners’ views on risk, challenge
65
Little, H. (2016). Promoting risk-taking and physically challenging play in Australian early
Little, H., & Eager, D. (2010). Risk, challenge and safety: implications for play quality and
playground design. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 18(4), 497-
513. doi:10.1080/1350293x.2010.525949
Little, H., Sandseter, E., Hansen, B., & Wyver , S. (2012). Early Childhood Teachers' Beliefs
about Children's Risky Play in Australia and Norway. Contemporary Issues in Early
Lounsbery, M. A. F., McKenzie, T. L., Morrow, J. R., Holt, K. A., & Budnar, R. G. (2013).
School Physical Activity Policy Assessment. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 10,
496-503.
Malaguzzi, L. (1994). Your image of the child: Where teaching begins. Child Care Information
Exchange.
Massey, W. V., Stellino, M. B., Hayden, L., & Thalken, J. (2019). Examination of the
doi:10.3390/ijerph17010225
Massey, W. V., Stellino, M. B., Mullen, S. P., Claassen, J., & Wilkison, M. (2018). Development
of the great recess framework - observational tool to measure contextual and behavioral
doi:10.1186/s12889-018-5295-y
66
McFarland, L., & Laird, S. G. (2017). Parents’ and early childhood educators’ attitudes and
practices in relation to children’s outdoor risky play. Early Childhood Education Journal,
McKenzie, T. L. (2002). System for Observing Play and Leisure Activity in Youth (SOPLAY).
Morrongiello, B. A., & Corbett, M. (2006). The Parent Supervision Attributes Profile
Morrongiello, B. A., Corbett, M. R., & Kane, A. (2011). A measure that relates to elementary
school children's risk of injury: the supervision attributes and risk-taking questionnaire
Morrongiello, B. A., Corbett, M. R., & Kane, A. . (2011). A measure that relates to elementary
school children’s risk of injury: The Supervision attributes and risk-taking questionnaire
Morrongiello, B. A., & Lasenby, J. (2006). Finding the daredevils: development of a Sensation
Seeking Scale for children that is relevant to physical risk taking. Accid Anal Prev, 38(6),
1101-1106. doi:10.1016/j.aap.2006.04.018
Niehues, A. N., Bundy , A., Broom, A., Tranter, P., Ragen, J., & Engelen, L. (2013). Everyday
67
Olsen, L. L., Ishikawa, T., Masse, L. C., Chan, G., & Brussoni, M. (2018). Risk Engagement and
Protection Survey (REPS): developing and validating a survey tool on fathers' attitudes
towards child injury protection and risk engagement. Inj Prev, 24(2), 106-112.
doi:10.1136/injuryprev-2017-042413
Parham, L. D., & Fazio, L. S. (2008). Play in Occupational Therapy for Children (2nd ed.). St.
org.ezproxy2.library.colostate.edu/10.1037/0012-1649.24.6.802
Piaget, J. (1972). Some Aspects of Operations. In M. Piers (Ed.), Play and Development (1st
Prezza, M., Alparone, F. R., Cristallo, C., & Luigi, S. (2005). Parental perception of social risk
and of positive potentiality of outdoor autonomy for children: The development of two
doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.12.002
Ramstetter, C. L., Murray, R., & Garner, A. S. (2010). The crucial role of recess in schools
Ridgers, N. D., Stratton, G., & McKenzie, T. L. (2010). Reliability and Validity of the System
for Observing Children’s Activity and Relationships During Play (SOCARP). Journal of
Sandseter, E. B. (2007). Categorizing risky play - How can we identify risk-taking in children’s
doi:10.1080/13502930701321733#.VTDjW2ccSUk
68
Sandseter, E. B. (2009). Characteristics of risky play. Journal of Adventure Education &
Sandseter, E. B. (2011). Children's risky play in early childhood education and care.
Sandseter, E. B., & Kennair, L. E. O. (2011). Children's risky play from an evolutionary
9(2), 257-284.
perceptions and practices concerning children's risky play. Childcare in Practice, 18(1),
83-101. doi:10.1080/13575279.2011.621889#.VS9wTmccSUk
children's risky play; examining the influence of personality and gender. Early Child
Scott, S., Jackson, S., & Backett-Milburn, K. (1998). Swings and roundabouts: Risk anxiety and
Speltz, M. L., Gonzales, N., Sulzbacher, S., & Quan, L. (1990). Assessment of Injury Risk in
Spencer, G., Bundy , A., Wyver , S., Villeneuve, M., Tranter, P., Beetham, K., . . . Naughton, G.
(2016). Uncertainty in the school playground: shifting rationalities and teachers’ sense-
making in the management of risks for children with disabilities. Health, Risk & Society,
69
Sterman, J. J., Naughton, G. A., Bundy , A. C., Froude, E., & Villeneuve, M. A. (2019). Planning
for outdoor play: Government and family decision-making. Scand J Occup Ther, 26(7),
484-495. doi:10.1080/11038128.2018.1447010
van Rooijen, M., & Newstead, S. (2016). Influencing factors on professional attitudes towards
risk-taking in children’s play: a narrative review. Early Child Development and Care,
Will, K. E., Lorek, E. J., Sabo, C. S., & Kidd, D. G. (2009). Measuring Injury Risk Perceptions:
Feasibility of a Risk Estimation Scale. American Journal of Health Behavior, 33(6), 639-
649.
Wolfensberger, W. P., Nirje, B., Olshansky, S., Perske, R., & Roos, P. (1972). The Dignity of
Wyver , S., Bundy , A., Naughton, G., Tranter, P., Sandseter, E. B., & Ragan, J. (2010). Safe
outdoor play for young children: Paradoxes and consequences. In S. Howard (Ed.),
Wyver , S., Tranter, P., Naughton, G., Little, H., Sandseter, E., Hansen, B., & Bundy , A. (2010).
Ten Ways to Restrict Children's Freedom to Play: The Problem of Surplus Safety.
doi:10.2304/ciec.2010.11.3.263
Yogman, M., Garner, A., Hutchinson, J., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., AAP COMMITTEE
70
C. O. C. A. (2018). The power of play: A pediatric role in enhancing development in
71
APPENDIX
This survey is part of the Sydney Playground Project, a scientific research project conducted with Ethics approval
from the University of Sydney. All information gathered from this survey is anonymous and will be used only for
research purposes.
Think about what you would do for the majority of your students when responding to each question.
How long have you been teaching/in this role for? (including other schools)
7-9
10-12
It varies
4. Questionnaire
Think about all students when responding to the following questions. If the answer is ever “yes”, select “yes".
*Do you trust your students to play in the classroom without constant supervision?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
72
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Do you let your students play chase with one another?
Yes
No
Comments (optional)
*Would you let your students go head first down a slippery dip/slide?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Do you allow your students to continue playing after s/he gets a scrape?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Do you let your students continue to play very challenging activities even when you
know they may not succeed?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
5. Questionnaire (cont.)
*Would you let your students climb a tree or other surface within your reach?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
73
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Would you let your students run in a place where there was an open fire or portable
heater?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you let your students walk on a slippery surface if there was a chance they
may fall?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Do you let your students use 'adult tools' (e.g. hammer and nail, knife, scissors)
unsupervised?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
6. Questionnaire (cont.)
*Do you allow your students to engage in rough and tumble play?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
74
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Would you let the majority of your students jump down from a height of 3-4 metres?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you allow your students to care for pets in the classroom?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you let your students swim close to the pool edge while you were watching
from the side?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
7. Questionnaire (cont.)
*Do you let your students resolve disagreements (without stepping in) if they are
pushing or poking one another?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you allow your students to play on equipment if you thought there was the
potential s/he may break a bone?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
75
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Do you allow your students to play-fight, testing who is strongest?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
8. Questionnaire (cont.)
*Do you wait to see how well your students manage challenges before getting
involved?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you let your students climb as high as they wanted in a tree or on another
surface?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you allow your student to ride a toy/bicycle down a steep hill?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
9. Questionnaire (cont.)
76
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Would you let your students climb as high as they wanted in a tree?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
No
Comment (optional)
*Do you let your students resolve disagreements (without stepping in) if the children
are shouting but not hitting one another?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you let your student(s) go off on their own in a new environment if you were
able to watch them from afar?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
*Would you let your students balance on a fallen tree or other narrow surface 2 metres
above the ground?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
77
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
*Do you encourage your students to take some risks if it means having fun during
play?
Yes
No
Comment (optional)
Share an example of a time when you allowed your student to do something that made
you feel uncomfortable? Please describe.
What were the benefits associated with allowing this child to do that?
Can you think of something you used to do regularly as a child that may be considered
'risky'? Please describe.
No
Comment (optional)
Describe what you fear most for your students and why?
78
The Tolerance of Risk in Play Scale (staff)
12. Information about you
21-25 years
26-30 years
31-35 years
36-40 years
41-45 years
46-50 years
50-55 years
79
LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS
ID Intellectual Disability
80
WARE Worry Assessment and Risk Estimation Scale
81