learner mining of task and pre task
learner mining of task and pre task
task input
Jeremy Scott Boston
The findings reported in this article suggest that learners inevitably ‘mine’
wordings contained in pre-task and task materials when performing tasks, even
when the teacher did not explicitly draw learner attention to these features.
However, this was found to be true only with written materials, and learners
did not appear to mine specific wordings from audio pre-task materials. Learner
mining of language input from written pre-task/task materials opens the
possibility of deliberately embedding specific language items into such materials.
While the article acknowledges that such an approach to task design is
controversial, with some arguing that it is inconsistent with principles behind
task-based learning, the article leaves it to the reader to decide if finding ways
of harnessing learner mining of wordings from pre-task input is in harmony
with the principles of a task-based approach.
Language input in Tasks need setting up with a pre-task stage, at the very least to give task
pre-task and task instructions. One purpose of pre-task preparation is to establish for learners
materials what the task’s communicative goal and communicative context are.
Another is to give learners ideas on how to approach the task. Willis (op. cit.)
suggests possibly playing recordings of fluent TL speakers performing
tasks learners are about to undertake.
An unresolved issue is whether a task-based approach precludes the use of
linguistically enhanced pre-task input. Nunan (2001b) believes that tasks
can include ‘reproductive’ language use, where learners reproduce
language models provided by the teacher, textbook, or some other source:
. . . communicative tasks can also involve reproductive language use.
For example, a classroom survey in which a student has to identify
classmates’ food preferences is both reproductive (the speaker will be
using the predictable form ‘Do you like hamburgers?’) and also
communicative (he or she doesn’t know how the interlocutor will
respond).
(Nunan 2001b: vi)
Willis (1990) believes that providing language models prior to tasks is likely
to cause learners to concentrate more on remembering and replicating the
formulae as accurately as possible than on communicating meaning:
The danger with focusing mechanically on form too early [in a lesson]
is that students see what follows not as an opportunity to use language
for communication, but rather as an opportunity to produce the
prescribed form as often as possible.
(Willis 1990: 73)
Willis (ibid. 64) notes that while during the pre-task stage some language
input is inevitable, but believes that in TB L exposure to any particular
language forms is intended to be incidental.
Mining language The purpose of this paper is not to take sides on the issue of whether it
input from pre-task is ‘permissible’ to linguistically enhance pre-task and task input. The
and task materials primary focus here is simply to find whether learners use language items
figure 1
Extract from task chart
(Samuda 2001: 127)
Samuda (ibid. 127–8) found that during the learner performance of the
task, out of 124 learner expressions of probability/possibility, 33 (26%) of
these expressions were conveyed using items ‘mined’ from the task
materials. These were ‘(it’s) possible’, ‘(it’s) probable’, ‘90%’, ‘certain’, and
‘50%’. The remaining 91 (74%) of learner expressions of possibility/
probability were items mobilized from their existing interlanguage.
These were ‘maybe’, ‘(I’m) sure’, ‘(I’m) not sure’.
The study Firstly, what input do learners mine on their own initiative? Do learners
Research questions mine input only from written task materials, or do they mine input from
audio pre-task recordings as well?
Audio recordings, whether scripted or authentic, are used in the
classroom often for the reason that they contain repeated examples of
certain language features. Do learners mine these features from recordings
and incorporate them into their task production?
Secondly, do learners mine specific language features deliberately
embedded by the teacher into pre-task/task materials? Do learners notice
and use these features even when the teacher does not explicitly draw
learner attention to them?
Minimizing learner Task-activities taken from the students’ coursebook were edited to ensure
focus on wordings that specific language features in, for example, the form of model
during pre-task dialogues or ‘speech bubble’ prompts, were not embedded in the
activities
figure 2
Example listening task
from Expressions Book 1
(Nunan 2001a: 57)
Only listening activities similar to steps ‘A’ and ‘B’ above were performed
during the pre-task stage, as these focus only on displaying
comprehension (which can be displayed non-verbally), whereas, step ‘C’
and the ‘Try this’ step require students to recall and focus upon specific
wordings from the audio recordings.
figure 3
Task activity from
Expressions Book 1
(Nunan 2001a: 108)
figure 4
Recordings from
Expressions Book 1:
Teacher’s Annotated
Edition (Nunan 2001b:
T105)
Learner performance When performing Task 1, both the R P T and RAT pairs mined the words
of Task 1 ‘suggest’ and ‘suggestion’ from the written task instructions to make gift
suggestions. (See Figure 5 below.) While the recordings contain the
phrases ‘Let’s [verb]’ and ‘How about [verb] + ing’ to make suggestions, the
RPT pair seemed uninfluenced by this, instead using the mined words
‘suggest’ and ‘suggestion’. All learners initiated exchanges by stating
what a person likes (for example, ‘Connie likes going to gym’) or by asking
‘What does [name] like?’ whether they listened to pre-task recordings or
not, making it less likely that the R P T pair mined these wordings from
the audio input.
Similar results were found from learner performance of the self-made task
below. Students mine input from written task materials in lieu of audio
pre-task input.
Task 2:
1 Task instructions/materials:
figure 6
Written instructions for
Task 1
2 Example textbook recording (1 of 4, ‘Do you know’ and ‘What does ____
look like’ in all recordings).
figure 7
Recordings from
Expressions Book 1: TAE
(Nunan 2001b: T25)
figure 8
Excerpts of task 2 pair
performance
Summary and With such a small sampling of student task production, any conclusions will
reflections on learner have to be tentative. Nevertheless, the data above appears to indicate that
performance of low-level learners do not mine whole structures or phrases from pre-task
Tasks 1 and 2 audio recordings. That the learners did not mine input from the audio
recordings was somewhat surprising, given that the RPT learners listened
to the audio recordings 3–4 times before being able to complete the listening
comprehension activities. Playing pre-task recordings, therefore, may not
result in low-level learners focusing on and employing specific language
features from the audio input, unless teachers explicitly draw learner
attention to these features.
Task 3: manipulating Learner performance of Tasks 1 and 2 above, show use of input mined
language input in from the written task materials. This opens up the possibility of covertly
written task ‘seeding’ written task materials with wordings for learners to mine and use
materials during task production. For example, in Task 1 above, the coursebook
RAT Pair
S2 I suggest you to buy exercise clothes.
S1 She already has a lot of work out clothes, is same mean as exercise
clothes.
RPT Pair
S2 I suggest tennis balls
S1 No
S2 Why? Already has?
In the following task, I recorded one RP T pair, and three R AT pairs. Each
RAT pair were given differently worded task instructions, all other written
task materials being the same for all three R AT pairs.
figure 9
Task instructions for RPT
pair and R AT pair 1
figure 10
RAT pair 2 task
instructions
figure 11
RAT pair 3 task
instructions
figure 12
Task activity from
Expressions Book 1
(Nunan: 2001a: 92)
Example textbook recordings (1 of 2, ‘going to . . .’ recurring in both
recordings).
figure 13
Recording from
Expressions Book 1 TAE
(Nunan 2001b: T89)
figure 14
Excerpts of R P T pair and
RAT pair 1, Task 3
performance
figure 15
Excerpts of Rat pair 2 and
RAT pair 3, Task 3
performance
(Note: I am sure readers will notice the flaw in the design of Task 3, 1b.
The mismatch between the linguistic focus ‘be going to’ the writer uses
in the written instructions and the present progressive (albeit using the
lexical verb ‘go’) in the recording. Nevertheless, the fact R P T Pair 2 mined
‘be going to’ from the rewritten task instructions still stands.)
Summary of learner The findings from Task 3 support previous observations that low-level
performance of learners do not appear to mine whole structures from pre-task audio
Task 3 input. Rather, it seems that low-level learners mine linguistic input
from written pre-task and task materials to incorporate into their spoken
task production. From the R P T pair 2 task performance, it appears
possible to plant specific input into written pre-task/task materials for
learners to mine.
Discussion and The debate in TB L over whether tasks can include linguistically
conclusion enhanced pretask and task input materials may never be resolved. As
Skehan (1998: 96) notes, ‘[the] two underlying characteristics of tasks,
avoidance of specific structures, and engagement of worthwhile meanings,
are matters of degree, rather than being categorical’.
I suggest that if the most natural task instructions happen to include specific
structures that also comprise the lesson’s language focus, so be it. For
example, in my experience, task instructions such as ‘Find out who has