0% found this document useful (0 votes)
17 views3 pages

Attachment Theory - Britannica Online Encyclopedia

Attachment theory, developed by John Bowlby, posits that humans are born with an innate need to form emotional bonds with caregivers, which significantly influences emotional regulation and relationships throughout life. The theory outlines three stages of response to caregiver separation: protest, despair, and detachment, and highlights the importance of caregiver responsiveness in shaping attachment styles. Subsequent research by Mary Ainsworth and others has identified different attachment patterns in children and adults, linking early attachment experiences to later emotional and relational outcomes.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
17 views3 pages

Attachment Theory - Britannica Online Encyclopedia

Attachment theory, developed by John Bowlby, posits that humans are born with an innate need to form emotional bonds with caregivers, which significantly influences emotional regulation and relationships throughout life. The theory outlines three stages of response to caregiver separation: protest, despair, and detachment, and highlights the importance of caregiver responsiveness in shaping attachment styles. Subsequent research by Mary Ainsworth and others has identified different attachment patterns in children and adults, linking early attachment experiences to later emotional and relational outcomes.
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 3

2/1/25, 9:03 PM attachment theory -- Britannica Online Encyclopedia

attachment theory
A mother smiling at her child, reinforcing the emotional bonding that underlies attachment theory.attachment
theory, in developmental psychology, the theory that humans are born with a need to form a close emotional
bond with a caregiver and that such a bond will develop during the first six months of a child’s life if the
caregiver is appropriately responsive. Developed by the British psychologist John Bowlby, the theory focused
on the experience, expression, and regulation of emotions at both species (normative) and individual (person-
specific) levels of analysis.

Bowlby believed that the attachment system, as he and others called it, served two primary functions: to
protect vulnerable individuals from potential threats or harm and to regulate negative emotions following
threatening or harmful events. The normative component of attachment theory identifies the stimuli and
contexts that normally evoke and terminate different kinds of emotions, as well as the sequence of emotions
usually experienced following certain relational events. The individual-difference component addresses how
people’s personal histories of receiving care and support from attachment figures shape their goals, working
models (i.e., interpersonal attitudes, expectations, and cognitive schemas), and coping strategies when
emotion-eliciting events in relationships occur.

Normative features of attachment theory


Bowlby’s fascination with the emotional ties that bind humans to each other began with an astute observation.
In all human cultures and indeed in primate species, young and vulnerable infants display a specific sequence
of reactions following separation from their stronger, older, and wiser caregivers. Immediately following
separation, infants protest vehemently, typically crying, screaming, or throwing temper tantrums as they
search for their caregivers. Bowlby believed that vigorous protest during the early phases of caregiver absence
is a good initial strategy to promote survival, especially in species born in a developmentally immature and
very dependent state. Intense protests often draw the attention of caregivers to their infants, who would have
been vulnerable to injury or predation during evolutionary history if left unattended.

If loud and persistent protests fail to get the caregiver’s attention, infants enter a second stage, known as
despair, during which they usually stop moving and become silent. Bowlby believed that from an evolutionary
standpoint, despondency is a good second strategy to promote survival. Excessive movement could result in
accident or injury, and loud protests combined with movement might draw predators. According to this logic,
if protests fail to retrieve the caregiver quickly, the next best survival strategy would be to avoid actions that
might increase the risk of self-inflicted harm or predation.

After a period of despair, infants who are not reunited with their caregivers enter a third and final stage:
detachment. During this phase, the infant begins to resume normal activity without the caregiver, gradually
learning to behave in an independent and self-reliant manner. Bowlby believed that the function of emotional
detachment is to allow the formation of new emotional bonds with new caregivers. He reasoned that
emotional ties with previous caregivers must be relinquished before new bonds can fully be formed. In terms
of evolution, detachment allows infants to cast off old ties and begin forming new ones with caregivers who
might be able to provide the attention and resources needed for survival. Bowlby also conjectured that these
https://www.britannica.com/print/article/42057 1/3
2/1/25, 9:03 PM attachment theory -- Britannica Online Encyclopedia

normative stages and processes characterize reactions to prolonged or irrevocable separations in adult
relationships, which might also have evolutionary adaptive value in terms of maintaining, casting aside, or
forming new romantic pairings.

In addition to identifying the course and function of these three distinct stages, Bowlby also identified several
normative behaviours that infants commonly display in attachment relationships. Such hallmark behaviours
include sucking, clinging, crying, smiling, and following the caregiver, all of which serve to keep the infant or
child in close physical proximity to the caregiver. Bowlby also documented unique features of caregivers and
their interactions with the infant that are likely to promote attachment bonds. The features include the
competence with which the caregiver alleviates the infant’s distress, the speed with which the caregiver
responds to the infant, and the familiarity of the caregiver to the infant. These behaviours and features are also
believed to be critical to the development of adult attachment relationships.
Individual-difference features of attachment theory
Attachment theorists after Bowlby have proposed that different attachment patterns (in children) and
attachment styles or orientations (in adults) reflect different ways of regulating affect (observable
manifestations of emotion), particularly controlling or dampening negative affect in stressful, threatening, or
particularly challenging situations. Individual differences in patterns of attachment in 12- to 18-month-old
children were first documented by Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues using the Strange Situation. The
Strange Situation involves a sequence of separations and reunions of caregivers (usually mothers) and their
children. It assesses how children regulate negative emotions regarding their caregivers when the children are
upset. Even though most children are distressed when left alone at this age, securely attached children tend to
reduce their negative emotions by using their caregivers as a “secure base,” and they resume other activities
fairly quickly after reuniting with them in the Strange Situation. “Anxious-resistant” children, by comparison,
remain distressed and often exhibit anger or resentment toward their caregivers during reunion episodes.
“Anxious-avoidant” children, who display fewer overt signs of distress but usually have elevated heart rates,
remain distant and emotionally detached from their caregivers during reunions, opting to calm themselves in a
self-reliant manner.

During later stages of development, one of the key differences between secure individuals and different types
of insecure individuals is how their negative emotions are regulated and controlled based on their specific
beliefs and expectations about the availability of comfort and support from their attachment figures. Highly
secure individuals have learned from past caregiving experiences to follow “rules” that permit distress to be
acknowledged and motivate them to turn toward attachment figures as sources of comfort and support. Highly
avoidant adults, in contrast, have learned to follow rules that limit the acknowledgment of distress and
encourage the use of self-reliant tactics to control and reduce negative affect when it arises. Highly anxious
people have learned to use rules that direct their attention toward the possible source of distress, to ruminate
about it, and to worry that their attachment figures will never fully meet their persistent needs for comfort and
support.

Mario Mikulincer and others have proposed a process model that outlines the sequence of events that underlie
the emotional coping and regulation strategies of people who have different attachment histories. For
example, when stress or a potential threat is perceived, highly secure individuals remain confident that their

https://www.britannica.com/print/article/42057 2/3
2/1/25, 9:03 PM attachment theory -- Britannica Online Encyclopedia

attachment figures will be attentive, responsive, and available to meet their needs and help them lower their
distress and anxiety. These beliefs, in turn, should increase their feeling of security, which should deactivate
their attachment systems, allowing them to use constructive, problem-focused coping strategies that over time
are likely to solve their problems.

Highly insecure individuals follow different pathways. When highly anxious individuals encounter
attachment-relevant stress or threats, they are uncertain as to whether their attachment figures will be
sufficiently attentive, available, and responsive to their needs. Such worries sustain their distress and keep
their attachment systems activated, resulting in the use of emotion-focused coping strategies such as
hypervigilance to signs of possible relationship loss and ruminating over worst-case scenarios. When highly
avoidant individuals feel stressed or threatened, they experience—but may not consciously acknowledge—
anxiety at a physiological level. To keep their attachment systems deactivated, highly avoidant persons work
to inhibit and control their emotional reactions by using avoidant coping strategies.

These three emotion regulation and coping strategies—problem-focused, emotion-focused, and avoidance-
focused strategies—are the source of many of the interesting cognitive and behavioral outcomes that have
been discovered in people who have different attachment styles or orientations. More securely attached
individuals, for instance, typically experience more intense and mild positive emotions in their romantic
relationships and fewer intense and mild negative emotions, whereas the reverse is true of more insecurely
attached persons. Longitudinal research has also documented connections between an individual’s early
attachment pattern (being classified as secure or insecure in the Strange Situation at age one) in relation to the
mother and emotions experienced and expressed with a romantic partner 20 years later. In addition,
individuals classified as insecure (either anxious-avoidant or anxious-resistant) in the Strange Situation at age
one are rated by their teachers as less socially competent during early elementary school. Lower social
competence, in turn, predicts greater likelihood of being rated as insecurely attached to same-sex friends at
age 16, which in turn predicts both the experience and expression of greater negative affect in relationships
with romantic partners when individuals are in their early 20s. Thus, there are indirect but theoretically
meaningful links between early attachment experiences and later attachment-based relationships in early
adulthood, just as Bowlby anticipated.

Jeffry A. Simpson Lane Beckes

Citation Information
Article Title: attachment theory
Website Name: Encyclopaedia Britannica
Publisher: Encyclopaedia Britannica, Inc.
Date Published: 12 April 2017
URL: https://www.britannica.comhttps://www.britannica.com/science/attachment-theory
Access Date: February 01, 2025

https://www.britannica.com/print/article/42057 3/3

You might also like