Curiosity in 21st Century
Curiosity in 21st Century
Educational Contexts
Author(s): Emily M. Grossnickle
Source: Educational Psychology Review , March 2016, Vol. 28, No. 1 (March 2016), pp. 23-
60
Published by: Springer
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].
Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms
Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Educational
Psychology Review
REVIEW ARTICLE
Emily M. Grossnickle
Abstract Curiosity has received increasing attention in the educational literature, yet empir
ical investigations have been limited by inconsistent conceptualizations and the use of curiosity
synonymously with other constructs, particularly interest. The purpose of this review is to
critically examine the dimensionality, definitions, and measures of curiosity within educational
settings, and address the boundaries between curiosity and interest. A systematic review of 39
articles from 2003 to 2013 revealed a reliance on self-report measures, a focus on curiosity as a
personality trait, and definitions characterized by four themes, the most common of which
were curiosity as a need for knowledge or information, and curiosity as a motivator of
exploratory behavior. The overlap and relations between curiosity and interest are discussed,
and it is proposed that an examination of (a) the role of knowledge, (b) goals and outcomes,
and (c) stability and malleability provide a basis for differentiating curiosity and interest
according to their essential characteristics.
Individuals who make advancements, be it in science, history, or art, are not satisfied to learn
solely what is already considered knowledge within a field. Rather, they move understanding
forward as their own need for knowledge leads their research and explorations into new and
uncharted directions (Kashdan 2004). It is this need for knowledge and exploration of the
unknown that most conceptions of curiosity hold in common (Loewenstein 1994), and it is
proposed that curiosity is a force behind research and academic explorations that require
countless hours to gain results (Bowler 2010).
Within educational contexts, curiosity is regarded as a means to increase and support the
outcomes and processes of learning (Arnone and Grabowsky 1994; Berlyne 1954; Spielberger
and Starr 1994). Curiosity has been found to enhance memory for new information (Kang
et al. 2009) and has been linked to higher academic performance on standardized tests (Wavo
2004). For students, curiosity has been positively associated with question asking (Peters
E. M. Grossnickle (El)
Department of Human Development and Quantitative Methodology, University of Maryland, 3304
Benjamin Building, College Park, Maryland, MD 20742-1131, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
Springer
Ö Springer
Given its potential importance for education, research on curiosity developed throughout the
20th century, from John Dewey's (1910) emphasis on curiosity-inspiring instruction to mid
century investigations of the role of curiosity in student achievement (e.g., Day 1968; Maw
and Maw 1972). Although research waned in the latter half of the 20th century, more recent
educational research has focused on the development of curiosity measures applicable to
educational investigations (Kashdan et al. 2009; Litman and Jimerson 2004; Woods-Groves
et al. 2009) and has examined curiosity as it relates to learning (Kashdan and Yuen 2007; Reio
and Wiswell 2000).
Ô Springer
Defining Curiosity
Dimensions of Curiosity
4Q Springer
ß Springer
Ô Springer
Springer
ö Springer
Method
Literature Search
In order to address the three main research questions, I conducted a systematic literature review
in a series of stages (Cooper 1982; Fink 2005; White 2009). Following the clarification of
research questions, databases and search terms were identified (Fink 2005). Given the three
major research questions, the search terms curiosity and curious were entered in a title and
abstract search in PsycINFO. Then, a series of screening measures were applied to determine
the suitability of studies for inclusion (Fink 2005). With regard to practical screening criteria,
the search was limited to studies that were: (a) peer-reviewed, (b) written in English, and (c)
used human subjects. This search produced 2856 results. In order to align with the aim of the
research questions in addressing recent empirical research, an additional criterion of date of
publication was applied (Fink 2005). This criterion served to provide a compendia of articles
for consideration and analysis (Hart 1998). The date was limited to capture recent research on
curiosity since the publication of Litman and Spielberger's (2003) seminal article introducing a
new measure and conceptualization of epistemic curiosity. The revised criteria resulted in a
pool of 851 hits.
From the initial pool of 851 articles, the titles and abstracts were examined to determine
their relevance in addressing the questions outlined in this review. To do this, a series of
inclusion and exclusion criteria were applied, which resulted in the final inclusion of 39
studies. An ancestry approach (Cooper 1982; White 2009) was also applied to search through
Springer
The following inclusion criteria were applied. First, only empirical research studies were
included. As such, commentaries, theoretical reviews, book reviews, and similar publications
were not included. Second, as the focus of this review was on understanding curiosity within
educational contexts, only studies that examined educational outcomes, situated the study
within educational contexts, or studies of curiosity measure development that included the
development or analysis of specific or general measures that could be applicable to education
were retained. For this reason, it was decided that studies of more general measure develop
ment or factor analyses of measures should be included, provided that the measure was linked
or potentially linked to educational studies. For instance, although a multidimensional measure
of curiosity developed by Litman and colleagues (Litman and Jimerson 2004; Litman and
Spielberger 2003) was designed to measure curiosity generally as a personality trait, it has
been more recently employed in educational contexts, and was therefore included. In contrast,
although curiosity is examined as a component of the Career Adapt-Abilities Scale (e.g.,
McMahon et al. 2012; Maree 2012), it was not included in this review due to the focus of this
measure on vocational beliefs and behaviors. In order to focus on curiosity within educational
settings, studies that examined either epistemic curiosity or general curiosity were retained,
while studies that examined specific types of curiosity other than epistemic or general curiosity
(e.g., interpersonal curiosity) were excluded.
Third, the decision was made to exclude studies that addressed the relation between
curiosity and solely emotional learning (e.g., Leonard and Harvey 2007) or interpersonal
learning (e.g., Litman and Pezzo 2007). This meant that studies such as one conducted by Reio
and Callahan (2004) were not included because the learning they focused on was socialization
related. Similarly, studies using clinical populations, examining curiosity as related to risk
behaviors or addiction (e.g., Garnier-Dykstra et al. 2012), and studies related to economics or
consumer behavior (e.g., Menon and Soman 2002) were not included. Fourth, given the
question of the relation between interest and curiosity, studies that included measures of both
curiosity and interest were included, even if these studies did not examine these factors within
an educational context. Finally, studies that did not include a measurement of curiosity or a
manipulation of curiosity were excluded. This included studies that examined collative
variables (e.g., novelty, uncertainty) rather than curiosity, as well as one study that measured
"frequency of engaging in online research when curious" rather than the frequency of curiosity
itself (Arnone et al. 2009, p. 123). Although the frequency of engaging in research when
curious provides an indicator of curiosity-enacted behaviors, the focus of this measure was the
online research behavior rather than curiosity itself.
It is important to note that the search criteria dictated that studies about curiosity were
defined for inclusion by the authors' labeling it as such. While this may limit the extent to
which these studies examine a consistent understanding of curiosity as opposed to other,
Ô Springer
Coding
Definitions of curiosity were coded according to a two-level process. First, studies were coded
as having an implicit or explicit definition (Dinsmore et al. 2008). Definitions of curiosity or
epistemic curiosity provided in studies, whether the authors' own, or quoted or paraphrased
from a previously published work, were considered explicit. Studies not stating a definition of
curiosity were considered implicit. The author and a second rater coded 38.5 % of included
studies with 93.3 % perfect agreement («=.87). Any disagreements were resolved through
discussion and the remainder of the coding was completed by the author. Second, of the
studies providing explicit definitions, themes were identified using content analysis. This was
guided by overarching definitional themes in the literature, concurrent with a bottom-up
analysis. Through the process of content analysis, the definitions were inspected for recurring
words or phrases. From these recurrences, patterns were identified. This process was conduct
ed iteratively until the coding scheme reached saturation. These identified themes aligned
closely with the definitional components first conceptualized by Maw and Maw (1966) and
later elaborated by Olson and Camp (1984). The themes were not mutually exclusive, with
many definitions falling into multiple categories.
The categories included curiosity defined: (a) as a need for knowledge or information, (b) as
exploratory behavior, (c) in relation to collative variables, and (d) as characterized by emotions
or arousal. Definitions of curiosity as a need for knowledge or information described curiosity
as a desire or need for knowledge, information, or learning something new. Definitions of
curiosity as exploratory behavior described curiosity as a motivator behind exploration, or as a
motivator behind the "pursuit" of knowledge, information, or learning. Curiosity in relation to
collative variables described curiosity as arising from any of the collative variables, or as a
desire to seek out any collative variables. Collative variables included novelty, complexity,
ambiguity, challenge, disequilibrium, uncertainty, or variables with similar characteristics.
Finally, curiosity as characterized by emotions or arousal defined curiosity as including
positive or negative emotions (or arousal). A complete coding scheme with examples is
included as Table 2.
Of the 26 articles with definitions present, 46.2 % of definitions were coded for theme
by the author and a second rater. Each definition was coded for the presence or absence
of each of the four themes. Perfect agreement was reached at a level of 83.3 % («=.67)
for three themes: need for knowledge or information (NK), association with collative
variables (CV), exploratory behavior (ExB), and at 91.7 % («=.83) for emotions or
arousal (EmA). Any disagreements were resolved through discussion and the remainder
of the definitions were coded by the author.
Springer
Defined curiosity as something that includes "the emotion trait associated with feelings of
emotions (positive or negative) or arousal interesf' (Silvia 2008, p. 95)
"a desire for new information aroused by novel,
complex, or ambiguous stimuli" (Koo and Choi
2010, p. 14)
Definitional Themes
As reviews of curiosity theories have been conducted elsewhere (Kashdan 2004; Loewenstein
1994; Litman 2005), the central focus of this analysis will be on patterns of the definitions
provided within recent empirical research on curiosity. Notably, a substantial number of th
studies («=13; 33.3 %) provided no explicit definition of curiosity. Therefore, descriptions o
the definitions relies on the 26 studies that included an explicit definition, and is organize
around the four identified and coded themes: (a) need for knowledge or information, (b
exploratory behavior, (c) collative variables, and (d) emotions and arousal.
<£} Springer
Exploratory Behavior
Ô Springer
Harkening back to initial definitions of curiosity (Berlyne 1960), collative variables were used
in 24.0 % of the reviewed definitions of curiosity (n=6). The definitions included variables of
novelty, complexity, ambiguity, challenge, and uncertainty (Cavojovâ and Sollâr 2007;
Kashdan et al. 2004; Kashdan and Yuen 2007) and defined curiosity as occurring in the
presence of phenomena that are unexpected or puzzling, including conceptual conflict or
questions (Engel and Randall 2009). These variables are typically related to attentional
processes (Berlyne 1960), and the definitions indicated two different relations between
curiosity and attention. First, the presence of these variables was most frequently suggested
as arousing cognitions and motivations that constitute curiosity, indicating that attention may
be regarded as a precursor to curiosity. The resolution of such moments of disequilibrium is
not mentioned as a requirement for curiosity, rather the presence and subsequent identification
of these variables is critical. Second, several definitions suggested that these variables are not
only something to be resolved, but that curiosity also involves seeking these variables
(Kashdan et al. 2004; Kashdan and Yuen 2007). For instance, Kashdan and Yuen, who based
their definition on a previous conceptualization (Izard 1977), suggested that curiosity involves
"the recognition, pursuit, and intense desire to investigate" collative variables (p. 260).
Definitions such as these suggest that attention to collative variables is a consequence of
curiosity.
Emotions or Arousal
Of the studies reviewed, nine (36.0 %) definitions included positive feelings or emotions.
Characterizations of curiosity in terms of emotions and arousal align with conceptuali
zations of curiosity as a spontaneous and dynamic capacity (Iran-Nejad and Cecil 1992;
James 1890/1950). The inclusion of curiosity as "a positive emotional-motivational
system" (Kashdan et al. 2004, p. 291) was adopted by Cavojovâ and Sollâr (2007).
The need for positive feelings was present in the definition of I-type by Litman et al.
(2010), but not D-type, curiosity. Silvia (2008) also indicated the significance of emo
tions in curiosity, when he defined curiosity as "the emotion trait associated with feelings
of interest" (p. 95). The use of the term emotion as opposed to mood or affect is a critical
feature in understanding how curiosity is defined. Affect is typically described as an
overarching term that includes moods and emotions (Linnenbrink 2006; Rosenberg
1998). Compared to the term mood, emotions are regarded as having a specific referent
and are experienced with greater intensity (Russell 2003). As indicated in the section
describing the need for knowledge or information and the relation to collative variables,
the referent in the case of curiosity is typically disequilibrium. Moreover, the motivation
of exploratory behavior as a defining feature of curiosity suggests that the intensity of
Ô Springer
Ô Springer
Measurement of Curiosity
•£) Springer
Curiosity and Exploration Inventory II Kashdan et al. 2009 Modification of CEI; 10-item scale di
(CEI 11) into subscales of stretching and embracing
Sensation-seeking scales applied
in educational settings
Novelty Experiencing Scale (NES) Peareon 1970 20-item subscale
sensation, external sensation, internal
cognitive desires, and external cogni
desires
Sensation-Seeking Scale V (SSS-V) Zuckerman 1979 40-item scale with four subscales: thrill and
adventure seeking, experience seeking,
disinhibition, and boredom susceptibility
Epistemic or academic curiosity scales
Academic Curiosity Scale (ACS) Vidler and Rawan 80 true-false items measuring curiosity in
1974 educational settings
Curiosity as a Feeling of Deprivation Litman and D-type curiosity scale; 15-ite
Scale (CFD scale) Jimerson 2004 subscales of competence, problem-solving,
and intolerance, although typically
examined as a single scale
Epistemic curiosity scale Litman 2008 I-type curiosity scale; 3- to 40-item versions;
10-item scale with subscales of specific
and diversive curiosity is most common
the Scale of Behavioral Engagement (Neblett et al. 2006; Smalls et al. 2007), used in two
studies. These measures were not originally designed to measure curiosity per se.
However, the inclusion questions about an individual's desire to seek collative variables
seem to have supported their continued use as measures of curiosity. For instance, the
SSS-V directs individuals to select options to describe their likes and feelings on topics
ranging from whether one prefers to see new movies or rewatch previously seen movies,
to whether or not one would like to learn to fly an airplane, to items asking about drug
use and activities such as parachute jumping. Similar in its focus on interest in pursuing
collative variables, the NES (Pearson 1970) includes subscales measuring internal and
external sensation as well as internal and external cognitive desires. The cognitive
subscales relate more closely to epistemic curiosity, including items such as "Learning
£) Springer
Ô Springer
Observational Measures
Behavioral measures were not used frequently, appearing in only 5.1 % (n=2) of the studies
examined. The focus of the observations tended to be on one of two features of curiosity: (a)
attention and resources paid toward the collative variables as identified by Berlyne (1954,
1960), or (b) exploratory behaviors manifest in the form of question asking or exploration in a
digital environment. Focus on the former of these observational measures was evident in a
study by Gilmore and Cuskelly (2011) that asked children ages 3-8 to select doors to open, a
task taken from Harter and Zigler (1974). Children were forced to select between opening a
door with a picture of what was inside, or a blank door with no indication of what was inside.
Selection of the blank doors was determined for the purposes of this study to be indicative of
higher curiosity, as it suggested exploration of the unknown.
In a study investigating curiosity in college students playing an educational game, written
comments communicated through online chat and behaviors recorded during the game were
analyzed for indications of curiosity (Dickey 2011). In addition, interviews were conducted
after completion of the game. Although the author noted that the interviews revealed two
different themes in students' curiosity, curiosity for specific pieces of evidence versus curiosity
to explore, the specific findings from the observational measures were not reported beyond a
few examples of curiosity questions posed by students.
Ô Springe:
Manipulation of Curiosity
4) Springer
Interest
£l Springer
Conceptual Overlaps
■ö Springer
£) Springer
Springer
Role of Knowledge
Ô Springer
<0 Springer
In its more enduring form, interest is characterized by positive feelings for, knowledge
about, and value of a particular topic, domain, or object. In comparison, curiosity is charac
terized as a disposition toward novelty-seeking with a focus on the questioning itself rather
than the content of the questioning. With some exceptions (e.g., Krapp 2007), discussions of
£) Springer
<ö Springer
Conclusions
Since curiosity's first entry into the psychological and educational literature over a century ago
(e.g., Dewey 1910), and particularly the empirical literature of the past decade analyzed in this
review, research reveals a focus on curiosity in its trait, rather than state form. The prepon
derance of studies considering trait curiosity has been acknowledged (Arnone et al. 2011), but
deserves further examination. While state and trait curiosity have been found to be highly
associated (Kashdan and Roberts 2004; Reio et al. 2006), there is evidence that state curiosity
â Springer
Ô Springer
â Springer
References
References marked with an asterisk (*) are included in the systematic portion of this review
Ainley, M. D. (1987). The factor structure of curiosity measures: breadth and depth of interest curiosity styles.
Australian Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 53-59. doi:10.1080/00049538708259035.
Ainley, M. (2006). Connecting with learning: motivation, affect, and cognition in interest processes. Educational
Psychology Review, 18, 391—405. doi:10.1007/s 10648-006-9033-0.
Springer
Ô Springer
â Springer
£) Springer
•£) Springer
Ô Springer
Ô Springer