Learners' Perceptions of Listening Comprehension Problems
Learners' Perceptions of Listening Comprehension Problems
Learners' Perceptions of
Listening Comprehension
Problems
Ali S. Hasan
Published online: 23 Apr 2010.
Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all
the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our
platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors
make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy,
completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any
opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and
views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor
& Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information.
Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study
purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution,
reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any
form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access
and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-
conditions
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
Learners’ Perceptions of Listening
Comprehension Problems
Ali S. Hasan
Faculty of Education, Damascus University, Damascus, Syria
Many EFL learners find that they are unable to comprehend natural spoken English
delivered at normal speed. The paper reports a study of listening problems encoun-
tered in the EFL classroom in the ESP Centre at Damascus University, as reported by the
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
It should be made clear, right from the beginning, that listening and understand-
ing are two separate processes. We should distinguish between listening as a
process which requires mere listening to the message and which does not neces-
sarily involve interpretation or reaction to the text, and listening comprehension
as a process which involves the meaningful interactive activity for an overall
understanding of the text. Listening comprehension in this research is taken to
refer to the way listeners select and interpret information that comes from the
auditory and/or visual clues in order to come to better understanding and
comprehension of what speakers say. This view of listening comprehension is in
accordance with second-language theory which views listening to spoken
language as an active and a complex process in which listeners focus on selected
aspects of aural input, construct meaning, and relate what they hear to existing
knowledge (O’Malley & Chamot, 1989; Byrnes, 1984; Richards, 1985; Howard,
1983).
It should be also made clear that learners’ perceptions of their listening prob-
lems may or may not correspond to what actually happens as different factors
which the listener may not be aware of may interact and influence learners’
perceptions. The listener, however, might identify one factor, among others,
which he thinks can be behind some of his listening problems. For example, some
listeners might think that a spoken passage is difficult to understand because
speakers speak too fast, while in fact it is not the speed of the speaker but some
other feature (or features) which causes the difficulty, such as pronunciation,
hesitation, pauses, and varied accents. This research is, therefore, about learners’
reported perceptions of listening comprehension as listening cannot be observed
and defined precisely and directly. However, we can study listening by asking
learners to tell us about it. This is what the present research tries to demonstrate.
It reports the findings of a questionnaire study that revealed learners’ percep-
tions and beliefs about their listening comprehension problems.
the spoken text. Learners also need to be aware of the factors which contribute to
their difficulties in listening. When listeners know something about their own
strategies, problems, and attitudes they will be able to improve their listening
practices and become better listeners. On the other hand, teachers may wrongly
encourage learners to use ineffective strategies in their classrooms; they may
insist on complete understanding of every phoneme, syllable, word, or phrase in
the text. By focusing on the strategies effective listeners use and applying them in
the EFL classroom, teachers help their learners to overcome their listening prob-
lems and become better listeners.
The significance of the study stems from the importance of listening skills in
foreign language learning. Listening plays an important role in second-language
instruction for several reasons (Rost, 1994). First, listening provides comprehen-
sible input for the learner which is essential for any learning to occur. Second,
listeners need to interact with speakers to achieve understanding. Third, listen-
ing exercises help learners draw their attention to new forms (vocabulary, gram-
mar, interaction patterns) in the language. Thus listening comprehension
provides the right conditions for language acquisition and development of other
language skills (Krashen, 1995). Listening exercises, therefore, should be geared
to develop learners’ effective listening strategies and to overcome their listening
problems. More specifically, the study derives its importance from the context it
explores; that is, it tries to give an account of the way EFL learners at Damascus
University view listening comprehension, strategies and problems. These partic-
ular learners, as the author has observed, have listening comprehension prob-
lems as they are still at their intermediate level in English proficiency.
The research reported here is intended, therefore, to identify the listening
comprehension strategies used by this particular group of learners and the prob-
lems they encounter when listening to a spoken text. It is also intended to
acquaint teachers with these problematic areas in listening comprehension so
that necessary treatment measures, which will be suggested as an implication of
the study, can be taken. It is assumed that the ideas derived from this research
will provide insights for learning and teaching listening comprehension skills.
Listening becomes in this case as Field (1998: 112) puts it, ‘a diagonistic activity’,
the function of the teacher being to identify and redress learners’ weakness as
listeners’. Thus, the crucial question is not to say that listening comprehension is
an important skill, but rather how best to diagnose its problems and promote its
development.
Listening Comprehension Problems 139
Research Findings
Research on second-language listening comprehension draws on studies
done on first-language learning (Anderson & Lynch, 1988; Devine, 1978, 1967;
Duker, 1964; Dunkel, 1991; Keller, 1960). It can be said that much of the informa-
tion we have about L2 listening comprehension is rooted in the work of
first-language researchers.
The research available on second-language listening comprehension is insuf-
ficient. There is little knowledge about how listening takes place. Comparing it
with other language skills, Goh (1997: 161) states that, ‘there are fewer insights
about the process of listening and the way it is learnt’. Similarly, Richards (1985:
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
189) says that ‘there is little direct research on second language listening compre-
hension’. We can say that this statement is still true despite the importance given
to listening comprehension (see Goh, 1997; Brown, 1992; Rost, 1994).
The literature available on L2 listening includes empirical research (Oxford,
1993; Rubin, 1994; Flowerdew, 1994) as well as theoretical and pedagogical stud-
ies (Ur, 1984; Rixon, 1986; Mendelsohn & Rubin, 1995). Research on
second-language listening has also attempted to point out the factors that may
influence learners’ comprehension (Dunkel, 1991; Lynch, 1988; Richards, 1985).
Grant (1997) chooses four strategies which would help learners to listen to
English: activating/building schema, guessing/inferencing/predicting, listen-
ing selectively, and negotiating meaning. Effective listeners use background
knowledge and relate this prior knowledge or schemata to the new information
contained in the spoken text and this will help them to comprehend the text as
they process it. As Faerch and Kasper (1986: 264) put it, ‘comprehension takes
place when input and knowledge are matched against each other’.
Pre-listening activities, in particular, facilitate second-language listening
comprehension (Herron, 1994; Berne, 1995). They help students develop back-
ground knowledge and improve their understanding of the spoken text.
Students will be able to generate ideas about the topic and such information will
help them activate relevant schema for the listening text. Students become able to
connect new information with prior knowledge schema. In this way processing
the new language becomes feasible as it becomes connected with concepts and
words students have already used. Research findings have indicated that famil-
iarity with passage content facilitates second-language listening comprehension
(Chiang & Dunkel, 1992; Brown & Yule, 1983).
The difficulties encountered by Arab EFL learners in listening comprehension
have also been pointed out (Hasan, 1993). Yagang (1994) attributes the difficulty
of listening comprehension to four sources: the message, the speaker, the listener
and the physical setting. Higgins (1995) studied Omani students’ problems in
listening comprehension and found that the factors which facilitate or hinder
listening are speech rate, vocabulary, and pronunciation. Flowerdew and Miller
(1996) studied learners’ strategies and difficulties in listening to academic
lectures. They found that students’ problems were speed of delivery, new termi-
nology and concepts, difficulties in concentrating, and problems related to phys-
ical environment. Rubin (1994) identified five factors which affect listening
comprehension: text characteristics,interlocutor characteristics,task characteris-
tics, listener characteristics, and process characteristics. Further research investi-
140 Language, Culture and Curriculum
tice. As Vogely (1995: 41) states, ‘We still need research that documents empiri-
cally the relationship between what theory says and what learners actually know
and more importantly do’. To locate the sources of listening comprehension, we
need to consider the discourse itself in the context of the classroom.
The present research is a further contribution to the investigation of foreign
language listening comprehension problems in actual classroom practice. Based
on a field study done at Damascus University, the present research provides
empirical evidence to identify the kinds of listening problem encountered by
intermediate EFL learners. It shows how listening is practised and perceived by
the students themselves; it delineates their perceptions of listening problems
pertinent to the message they listen to, the speaker, and the listener. It also
provides clues about the strategies which learners use to overcome their prob-
lems in listening comprehension. The research also aims to arrive at implications
for pedagogy.
the end of the questionnaire to point out the factors which help and hinder their
listening comprehension in English. Oral instructions and explanations were
given to the respondents by the researcher himself to avoid any misunderstand-
ing. Responses were then computed and frequencies and percentages were
calculated. Only 81 questionnaires were accepted for the study; the other seven
questionnaires were not taken seriously by the respondents, and were excluded
from the study.
Learner strategies
Effective listening strategies improve the quality of listening comprehension.
If this is the case, it becomes important to know what strategies proficient listen-
ers use, and which strategies affect the quality of listening comprehension
adversely and lead to students’ problems.
Table 1 shows that the strategies which students use in listening comprehen-
sion tasks are partly effective and partly ineffective. On the one hand, students
use effective listening comprehension strategies such as the use of pre-listening
information and background knowledge of the topic to help them understand
the text. Clearly, these are considered to be effective strategies of listening
comprehension which would help learners to overcome their listening problems
(O’Malley & Chamot, 1989; Vogely, 1995; Thompson & Rubin, 1996).
On the other hand, Table 1 also shows that students partly use ineffective
strategies in listening comprehension. In real-life situations, effective listeners do
not normally process all words of the discourse; they skim parts of the message
which are not related to their purposes and pay attention to relevant details.
Contrary to what effective listeners do, students do not focus their attention on
the message selectively. They listen to every detail to get the main idea of the
spoken text (59.4% do that sometimes, often, and always). They probably do
because they think that every word or detail is important and must be under-
stood. They are thus under the false impression that they must understand every
word they hear and this exacerbates their anxiety as they panic when they are not
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
able to hear or understand every single word. This would, in turn, undermine
their ability to become good listeners. Furthermore, when learners try to follow
every word they do not know which is the key word or most important word
which gives them a clue for understanding the text. This way of processing infor-
mation might be due to learners’ preference for rote-memorisation of learning in
general. If this is so, it might be the case that students suffer from information
overload in the passage which hinders their ability to monitor the message and
get an overall comprehension of the text.
It should be noted that lexis is an important factor in listening comprehension,
but words are not the whole story. Brown (1992) indicates that we do not process
discourse as though all of it were equally interesting or equally worthy of being
remembered. In our first language, we skim over parts of the message and pay
attention to relevant parts only. Thus sentences are not processed word by word
and the focus is placed on the ideas behind these words and how these ideas are
linked together to draw conclusions. This focus on the underlying meaning of
utterances can help learners to overcome their problem of focusing on individual
words (McNeill, 1997: 86). It helps them to relax the intensity of their listening
efforts through using clues from the context and background knowledge to
understand the text as a whole.
Listening task
Several interesting tasks and activities encourage listeners to develop their
listening comprehension though they encounter certain problems in doing such
tasks. Table 3 illustrates these difficulties.
Students (sometimes or often) find difficulty in doing certain prediction tasks
144 Language, Culture and Curriculum
Table 3 Learners’ perceptions of listening problems: Tasks and activities
Item Statements Never Seldom Some- Often Always
no. % % times % % %
12 I find it difficult to predict 11.1 23.4 40.7 23.4 1.4
what speakers are going to
say from the title of the
spoken text.
13 After my teacher stops the 3.7 22.2 48.3 20.9 4.9
tape I find it difficult to
predict what will come next.
17 I find it difficult to do 18.5 34.7 33.3 11.1 2.4
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
(either from the title of the spoken text, 64.1%, or in what will come next after a
certain point, 69.2%). This may be due to the techniques which learners use in
processing every single word of the text rather than focusing their attention on
certain cues which would help them to set up predictions. This may be attributed
to students’ limited knowledge of the language; students who are not familiar
with language cues, clichés and collocations will find it difficult to predict a miss-
ing word or phrase. Thus lack of contextual knowledge and vocabulary will be
an obstacle to prediction and comprehension. It should be noted that in real life
when we go for a job interview we usually know what the interviewer is going to
talk about; this is something we can predict as we often know some background
information about the job and the subject which is going to be discussed. Simi-
larly, in order to help our learners predict what speakers are going to say we
should provide them with enough information before we give them a listening
comprehension exercise. Preliminary elicitations such as ‘What do you know
about this topic?’, ‘What could the passage be about?’ are helpful for learners to
set up predictions.
With certain types of activity which are more interactive in nature, such as
listening activities which are done either in pairs or in groups, students show
only minor problems. This may be attributed to the fact that interactive listening
of this sort provides learners with a supportive environment for both listening
and speaking. Here we find learners exchanging roles and the interaction is
sustained by listening and speaking through participation (Hasan, 1988). Such
activities would improve learners’ listening comprehension as they encourage
students to work with one another and require them to negotiate meaning by
listening and asking questions.
Listening Comprehension Problems 145
Finally, listening tasks may be facilitated by linking them with other language
skills. The present research shows that students are not trained to practise listen-
ing activities in relation to speaking and writing in some cases. Thus 53.2% of
students (sometimes 37%, often 16.2%) find it difficult to hold a discussion after
listening to the spoken text, and 69.2% of students (sometimes or often) find it
difficult to write a summary of the text. This finding accords with a previous
study (Hasan, 1997) which pointed out that listening comprehension was poorly
linked with its related skills of speaking, reading, and writing. The solution in
this case would be to incorporate listening with other skills such as speaking and
writing to reinforce learners’ understanding of the spoken text.
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
The speaker
Problems of listening comprehension related to speaking can be seen in rela-
tion to natural speech, pronunciation, varied accents, and a voice heard only on
an audio-tape cassette recorder without seeing the speaker.
Table 4 shows that students (sometimes or often, 69.3%) find it difficult to
understand natural speech which is full of hesitations and pauses. This is in line
with the findings of previous research which indicate that hesitations and
pauses in spontaneous speech cause perceptual problems and comprehension
errors for non-native speakers (Voss, 1979). Listening activities need language
input taken from audio or video recordings, live output from the teacher, or the
learners themselves. The critical question is whether to choose ‘authentic’ or
‘prepared’ input for such activities. Rost (1994) indicates that many teachers
prefer prepared materials with controlled vocabulary, usage and speed in
order to allow learners to comprehend more easily. Others prefer authentic
material in order to acquaint learners with ‘real’ input which has the character-
istics of spoken language and which is not found in written texts to which
students are usually accustomed. Berne (1998) reports on the findings of a
study by Herron and Seay (1991) which indicates that the use of authentic as
opposed to pedagogical listening passages leads to greater improvement in
listening comprehension and performance. A middle position is the use of
simulated materials which keep many authentic features with shorter presen-
tations. It could be the case that the presentation of simulated input to EFL
learners may reduce their difficulty in listening to natural spoken texts. This
input must be typical of everyday language which contains a few colloquial
words and expressions as learners who listen only to formal English may find it
difficult to understand such natural spoken texts.
Table 4 also shows that learners encounter listening problems when speakers
speak too fast or with varied accents and produce words which are not clearly
pronounced. Slow speech is characterised by pauses, whereas fast speech is
distinguished by the elimination of clausal pausing. Rivers (1981) considers fast
speech is the one which is spoken on the average of 220 w.p.m., whereas slow
speech is below 130 w.p.m. Some learners cannot remember words they have just
heard; they are slow to recall the meaning of words and phrases spoken too
quickly. It seems possible that such speech should be understood by FL learners
without adequate training. However, if instructors were to reduce the speed of
their speech, listeners might end up with the belief that listening comprehension
is equivalent to word-for-word processing of information. A more effective tech-
146 Language, Culture and Curriculum
Table 4 Learners’ perceptions of listening problems related to the speaker
Item Statements Never Seldom Some- Often Always
no. % % times % % %
21 I find it difficult to 2.4 23.4 44.7 24.6 4.9
understand natural speech
which is full of hesitation
and pauses.
22 I find it difficult to 0 10.0 25.9 30.8 33.3
understand the meaning of
words which are not
pronounced clearly.
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
nique to counteract this problem would be to break the listening text into natural
segments or phrases and deliver them as ‘chunks’ or meaningful units of speech
and maintain intonation, emphases, and pauses (Lee & Van Patten, 1995).
Learners also reported that they find it difficult to understand the meaning of
the spoken text without seeing the speaker’s body language. This deprives them
of some contextual clues which make the message easily understood as they
cannot see the speakers’ faces and gestures. Moreover, they have the added diffi-
culty of remembering the setting and the individual speakers when the speakers
are not seen. Visual support, whether it is in the form of pictures, a video, or writ-
ing helps students understand the text. Students reported that visual clues in the
form of pictures, diagrams, and charts help them understand the spoken text
(41.9% always and 58.1% sometimes or often). This is in line with previous stud-
ies which show that ‘the use of video in listening comprehension facilitates infor-
mation processing’ (Thompson & Rubin, 1996: 333). It can be said that those
activities which involve the use of video in presenting listening passages provide
interesting and motivating input and facilitate second-language listening
comprehension (Thompson & Rubin, 1996; Secules et al., 1992; Baltova, 1994).
Visual support not only makes the topic more comprehensible to listeners but
Listening Comprehension Problems 147
also helps them to reduce the anxiety that can occur when they do not know what
speakers are talking about.
Learners also reported that written support in the form of tapescripts
provided before listening exercises help them understand the text (27.1% often
and 45.6 % always). These written forms of language act as visual reinforcement
for listening. This may be due to the fact that tapescripts allow listeners to check
and make sure that they have listened to everything on the tape. However, it
seems logical not to introduce tapescripts unless students have made their initial
attempts to understand the spoken text without written support. When listeners
understand the spoken text without reference to the written form they become
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
Listener attitudes
Listeners’ problems may arise from their lack of interest in the spoken text, the
demand for full and complete answers to listening comprehension questions,
and the recorded message on the audio-tape (Table 5).
revealed (41.9% often and 38.2% always). Learners also reported (44.4% often
and 29.6% always) that unclear sounds resulting from poor classroom conditions
or outside noise interfere with their understanding. In addition, contrary to what
takes place in real life, speakers cannot be seen nor can they be interrupted to ask
for clarification as spoken texts are not understood instantly in some cases. To
make things easier, contextual clues should be provided. The study shows that
students find it difficult to get a general understanding of the spoken text from
the first listening (32% sometimes; 40.7% often). Moreover, this last factor may
create psychological problems for listeners as 54% (28.3% often, 25.9% always)
feel nervous and worried when they fail to understand the spoken text.
Listeners find it difficult to answer questions which require other than a short
answer, e.g. why or how questions (44.7% sometimes, 28.3% often). These ques-
tions which require detailed and long answers may sound misguided. Listening,
in this case, is often treated as a test in which learners are asked to listen to a long
stretch of English and to recall the facts. They are in reality being tested rather
than taught (Sheerin, 1987).
It should be noted that students are not expected to produce 100% correct
answers because as Brown and Yule (1983: 59) say, ‘a student trained in such
expectations constantly experiences panic as he practises listening’. Brown and
Yule (1983: 57) maintain that only a reasonable interpretation is required, ‘what
native listeners operate with are partial, reasonable, interpretations of what they
are listening to, and it seems unjustifiable to require of non-native listeners that
they do more’.
Finally, matters of motivation and interest are also important factors in under-
standing the topic of the spoken text. It was found that students find it difficult to
understand the spoken text which is not of interest to them (29.6% sometimes
and 41.9% often).
Other factors
At the end of the questionnaire learners were asked to list important factors
aiding listening comprehension. Learners’ answers centred on pre-listening
information related to the text, good quality tape-recorders, familiar vocabulary,
reading the text before listening to it, and interesting topics.
These factors were in learners’ opinions determinant factors in developing
their listening comprehension skills. Learners have rightly pointed out the
importance of pre-listening information and interesting topics in helping them to
understand the spoken text. However, they were misguided by focusing on
vocabulary and reading the text before listening to it. It was pointed out earlier
Listening Comprehension Problems 149
that vocabulary should be guessed from the context of the spoken text and learn-
ers should first listen to the text before they read it.
Learners were asked to list important factors which hinder their listening
comprehension. In reply, their answers centred on poor conditions in the class-
room which interfere with clear sounds, absence of visual aids, unfamiliar vocab-
ulary, unclear pronunciation and fast speech, a boring subject, and a long text.
It can be seen that learners realised the importance of interesting topics which
help them to understand the spoken text and considered a boring subject a
hindrance to their listening comprehension. Moreover, they have rightly sensed
the lack of visual aids, unclear pronunciation, and a long spoken text as impor-
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
tant factors hindering listening comprehension. It was pointed out earlier that
listeners would not be able to keep in memory the information heard from a long
text. On the other hand, learners still emphasise the fact that unfamiliar vocabu-
lary interferes with their listening comprehension. This is perhaps due to ineffec-
tive ways of teaching by which teachers ask their students to focus on vocabulary
before they listen to the spoken text and discourage them from guessing them
from the context.
Discussion
This research has investigated learners’ perceptions of listening comprehen-
sion strategies and problems. The evidence presented points out that EFL learn-
ers encounter various kinds of listening problems. Moreover, it was found that
EFL learners were in some respects poorly equipped with effective strategies,
skills, and activities to help them to improve their listening comprehension.
The identification of listening comprehension problems pertinent to tasks and
activities, the message, the speaker and the listener as reported in this research
has significant implications. These problems can direct teachers to determine the
causes which make comprehension break down and design remedial tasks for
each problematic area. When such remedial tasks are formed listening compre-
hension skills can be improved. Based on the findings of this research the follow-
ing outline of guidelines of listening activities are proposed for tackling learners’
problems of listening comprehension.
(1) Pre-listening
Discussion: Discussions to elicit information about the topic. Select certain
words, difficult grammatical structures and expressions to be explained
through the discussion.
Prediction: Ask students to predict the content or what speakers are going to
say, based on the information they have already got through the prelimi-
nary discussion.
Questions: Pre-setting questions to be answered upon listening to the text.
(2) Listening in progress:
Identifying the gist: Students note down main points and key words to work
out the gist of the talk.
Identifying features of natural input: Students listen to natural spoken
discourse. They are asked to identify stress patterns, stress contrasts, weak
forms, etc.
150 Language, Culture and Curriculum
text.
(3) Post-listening
Evaluation: Students answer true–false questions or multiple-choice ques-
tions as an evaluation of their understanding.
Speaking: Students act out a dialogue, interview or discussion on topics
related to the text.
Writing: Students write messages, postcards, letters, etc. on topics related to
the text.
It is assumed that these class activities will help learners to deepen their under-
standing of the spoken text. They represent the suggested mechanism and
field-based views of the present research for addressing problems of listening
comprehension and presumably similar problems in similar teaching–learning
contexts.
In particular, the findings of the research imply that teachers should design
classroom activities which train their students to pay attention to the overall
message rather than listen to every single word in order to come to better under-
standing of the text. Learners need also to be aware of the factors which contrib-
ute to their difficulties in listening, such as the unfamiliar or difficult items in a
long spoken text. It is suggested, therefore, that teachers introduce intelligent
guesswork as an important strategy to help their students infer unfamiliar
vocabulary from the context, encouraging them to use their background knowl-
edge to arrive at better interpretation of the unfamiliar items. Similarly, difficult
grammatical structures must be unlocked through a process of inference, expla-
nation, and practice. In addition, teachers should design classroom tasks to teach
listening comprehension rather than test it. Besides asking students comprehen-
sion questions after listening to the spoken text, teachers should also ask them to
draw upon a range of tasks to do while listening. In this way students can
provide and revise their interpretations as the text builds up and students listen
carefully both to the text itself and to the suggested interpretation of it.
It should be remembered that when designing these activities we need to give
clear instructions and offer a specific purpose for the task. In addition, we need to
consider the degree of interest and involvement they generate as uninteresting
activities may not develop learners’ listening comprehension.
In short, the study helps us to define students’ perceptions of listening
comprehension problems and establish realistic measures for treatment which
would be incorporated in the design of listening courses and textbooks.
However, this study has not examined teachers’ views of teaching listening
Listening Comprehension Problems 151
should also study the impact of listening on the development of other skills, such
as speaking, reading, and writing as well as how to incorporate listening with
these skills.
Correspondence
Any correspondence should be directed to Professor Ali Saud Hasan, Faculty
of Education, Damascus University, Damascus, Syria.
References
Anderson, A. and Lynch, T. (1988) Listening. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Baltova, I. (1994) The impact of video on the comprehension skills of core French students.
The Canadian Modern Language Review 50, 507–31.
Berne, J.E. (1995) How does varying pre-listening activities affect second language
listening comprehension? Hispania 78, 316–29.
Berne, J.E. (1998) Examining the relationship between L2 listening research, pedagogical
theory, and practice. Foreign Language Annals 31 (2), 169–90.
Boyle, J.P. (1984) Factors affecting listening comprehension. ELT Journal 38 (1), 34–8.
Brown, G. (1992) Listening to Spoken English. London: Longman Press.
Brown, G. and Yule, G. (1983) Teaching the Spoken Language. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
Byrnes, H. (1984) The role of listening comprehension: A theoretical base. Foreign
Language Annals 17, 29–31.
Call, M. (1985) Auditory short-term memory, listening comprehension, and the input
hypothesis. TESOL Quarterly 19, 765–81.
Chiang, C.S. and Dunkel, P. (1992)The effect of speech modification, prior knowledge and
listening proficiency on EFL lecture learning. TESOL Quarterly 26, 345–74.
Devine, T.G. (1967) Listening. Review of Educational Research 37, 152–58.
Devine, T.G. (1978) Listening: What do we know about fifty years of research and
theorizing? Journal of Reading 21, 296–304.
Duker, S. (1964) Listening. Review of Educational Research 35, 156–63.
Dunkel, P. (1991) Listening in the native and second/foreign language: Toward an
integration of research and practice. TESOL Quarterly 25, 431–57.
Færch, C. and Kasper, G. (eds) (1986) The role of comprehension in second language
learning. Applied Linguistics 7, 257–74.
Field, J. (1998) Skills and strategies: Towards a new methodology for listening. ELT Journal
52 (2), 110–18.
Flowerdew, J. (ed.) (1994) Academic Listening: Research Perspectives. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Flowerdew, J. and Miller, L. (1996) Student perceptions, problems and strategies in
second language lecture comprehension RELC Journal 23 (2), 60–80.
Goh, C. (1997) Metacognitive awareness and second language listeners. ELT Journal 51 (4),
361–9.
Grant, L.H. (1997) Listening to English in the 21st century: The need for learner strategies.
152 Language, Culture and Curriculum
In C. Zaher (ed.) Proceedingsof the Third EFL Skills Conference: New Directions in Listening
(pp. 10–18). The Center for Adult and Continuing Education, The American University
in Cairo, 3–5 December 1996.
Hasan, A.S. (1988) Variation in spoken discourse in and beyond the EFL classroom: A
comparative study. Unpublished PhD thesis, University of Aston.
Hasan, A.S. (1993) Methodology of Teaching English. Damascus: Damascus University.
Hasan, A.S. (1997) Ways of improving teaching listening comprehension to Arabic
speakers in Syria. In C. Zaher (ed.) Proceedings of the Third EFL Skills Conference: New
Directions in Listening (pp. 159–71). The Center for Adult and Continuing Education,
The American University in Cairo, 3–5 December 1996.
Herron C. (1994) An investigation of the effectiveness of using an advanced organizer to
introduce video in the foreign language classroom. The Modern Language Journal 78,
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013
190–98.
Herron, C. and Seay, I. (1991) The effect of authentic aural texts on student listening
comprehension in the foreign language classroom. Foreign Language Annals 24, 487–95.
Higgins, J.M.D. (1995) Facilitating listening in second language classrooms through the
manipulation of temporal variables. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of Kent
at Canterbury.
Higgins, J. (1997) Students’ listening difficulties: The contribution of speech rate. In
Christine Zaher (ed.) Proceedings of the third EFL Skills Conference: New Directions in
Listening. The Center for Adult and Continuing Education, The American University
in Cairo, 3–5 December 1996.
Howard, D. (1983) Cognitive Psychology: Memory, Language and Thought. New York:
Macmillan.
Keller, P. (1960) Major findings in listening in the past ten years. Journal of Communication
10, 29–38.
Krashen, S.D. (1995) The Input Hypothesis: Issues and Implications. NewYork: Longman.
Lee, J.F. and Van Patten, B. (1995) Making Communicative Language Teaching Happen. New
York: McGraw Hill.
Lynch, A. (1988) Grading foreign language listening comprehension materials: The use of
naturally modified interaction. Dissertation, University of Edinburgh.
McNeill, A. (1997) Some formal obstacles to grasping meaning in spoken English. In C.
Zaher (ed.) Proceedings of the Third EFL Skills Conference: New Directions in Listening. The
Center for Adult and Continuing Education, American University in Cairo, 3–5
December 1996.
Mendelsohn, D.J. and Rubin, J. (eds) (1995) A Guide for the Teaching of Second Language
Listening. San Diego, CA: Dominie Press.
Nunan, D. (1991) Language Teaching Methodology. Hertfordshire: Prentice Hall.
O’Malley, J.M. and Chamot, A.U. (1989) Listening comprehension strategies in second
language acquisition. Applied Linguistics 10 (4), 418–37.
Oxford, R.L. (1993) Research update on teaching L2 listening. System 21, 205–11.
Richards, J.C. (1985) The Context of Language Teaching. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Rivers, W.M. (1981) Teaching Foreign Language Skills (2nd edn). Chicago: University of
Chicago Press.
Rixon, S. (1986) Developing Listening Skills. London: Macmillan.
Rost, M. (1994) Listening. London: Longman.
Rubin, J. (1994) A review of second language listening comprehension research. The
Modern Language Journal 78 (2), 199–221.
Secules, T., Herron, C. and Tomasello, M. (1992) The effect of video context on foreign
language learning. Modern Language Journal 76, 480–90.
Sheerin, S. (1987)Listening comprehension: Teaching or testing. ELT Journal 4 (2), 126 – 31.
Thompson, I. and Rubin, J. (1996) Can strategy instruction improve listenin g
comprehension? Foreign Language Annals 29 (3), 331–41.
Ur, P. (1984)Teaching Listening Comprehension. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Vogely, A. (1995) Perceived strategy use during performance on three authentic listening
tasks. The Modern Language Journal 79 (1), 41–56.
Listening Comprehension Problems 153
Vogely, A.J. (1998) Listening comprehension anxiety: Students’ reported sources and
solutions. Foreign Language Annals 31 (1), 67–80.
Voss, B. (1979) Hesitation phenomena as sources of perceptual errors for non-native
speakers. Language and Speech 22, 129–144.
Wenden, A. (1986) What do second language learners know about their language
learning? A second look at retrospective account. Applied Linguistics 7 (2), 186–205.
Yagang, F. (1994) Listening: Problems and solutions. In T. Kral (ed.) Teacher Development:
Making the Right Moves. Washington, DC: English Language Programs Division, USIA.
Downloaded by [University of Hong Kong Libraries] at 02:34 21 September 2013