1986-009-DLD
1986-009-DLD
FINAL REPORT
by
1
DEVELOPMENT OF INTENSIVE POND FARMING TECHNIQUES FOR THE MUD CRAB
SCYLLA SERRATA (FORSKAL) IN NORTHERN AUSTRALIA.
INTRODUCTION
A large demand for crab meat on both Australian and
S.E.Asian markets has provided incentive for the development of a
mud crab Scylla serrata farming industry in tropical Australia.
Cannibalism of newly-moulted. softshelled crabs however
constitutes a major constraint to profitable pond farming.
The main aim of the project was to observe moult-related
cannibalism of mud crabs and define causative factors of this
phenomenon and then to develop pond design and management
techniques which alleviated this problem.
The project was initiated in September 1986 when
construction of Sea Hatcheries Ltd.' commercial multi-species
hatchery at Mourilyan Harbour. North Queensland (fig. 1)
commenced. Facilities for larval rearing had been established by
December 1986 and the first attempt to produce juvenile crabs was
carried out.
By mid year of 1987 the main hatchery building was completed
and a controlled environment laboratory was made available for
clearwater behavioural studies. Experimental tank systems for
replicated trials were set up in this room and time-lapse video
equipment. for continual surveillance. was purchased and
installed.
Initial behavioural studies had used wild caught juvenile
mud crabs. however the numbers collected were small and their
availability was erratic which made experimental design
difficult.
2
After many, largely unsuccessful. attempts at rearing larvae
in order to obtain juvenile crabs for the projected behavioural
trials, it was decided to postpone the project for six months
(between 16.10.87 - 31.3.88) while a more sophisticated rearing
system was constructed.
LARVAL REARING
.L.. Broodstock Maintenance
Over the course of the project eighty-eight (88) mature
female mud crabs (mean C.W. = 15.5 + 1.1cm) were collected from
Mourilyan Harbour and Conn Creek, Hinchinbrook channel (fig. 2)
by set pot or dilly. A histogram comparing the size range of
mature female mud crabs collected in northern (this study) and
southern Queensland (Heasman,1980) is presented in figure 3. This
shows that female mud crabs reach maturity at a smaller size
3
�
�
,
38
0
�
u rt
0
r
�
m
,
, m
0
rtr
�rt
�m ~rt
� I
� m
�u
m�
�-
ru
rt
m
Figure 2. Mourilyan Harbour and Hinchinbrook Channel,
North Queensland. Collection sites for mature
female mud crabs Scylla serrata.
50
Heasman, 1980
45 Moreton Bay
Southern Queensland
40 x C.W. - 16.65 cm
(n=339)
35
F
R 30
E
Q
u 25
E
N
C 20
V
J.5
J.0
- 0
20
Present stu dy
J.8 Mourilyan Harbour
Tropical Queensland
J.6
x C.W. - 15.5 cm
J.4 (n=ll8)
F
R
E J.2
Q
u J.0
E
N
C 8
V
6
0
> > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > > >
J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. J. 1 J. 1 J. 2 2 2 2
. . . . . . . . . . . . .
2 2 3 3 3 4 4 4 5 5 5 5 6 6 6 ? ? 7 8 8 8 8 9 9 9 0 0 0 J.
. . . .
6 9 2 5 8 J. 4 7 0 3 6 9 2 5 8 J. 4 ? 0 3 6 9 2 5 8 J. 4 ? 0
. . . . . . . . . . . .
SIZE CLASS <cM)
(carapace width) in tropical Australia.
Initially the crabs were maintained individually in buoyant
clothes baskets held within floating Barramundi (Lates
calcarifer) broodstock cages in Mourilyan Harbour. This system
proved inappropriate as servicing (feeding and cleaning) was
difficult and water quality parameters such as salinity.
temperature and turbidity were variable. Consequently a land
based crab broodstock holding facility was established.
This consisted of a 2.4m diameter (5000 litre) above-ground
fibreglass tank (plate 1) which had a perforated false bottom
fitted to allow water circulation through a sand substrate.
Approximately 20cm depth of calcareous beach sand was supplied as
substrate to permit crab burying as well as provide biofilter
media for reconditioning of sea water.
No more than seventeen (17) adult fem�le mud crabs were held
communally in the broodstock tank at any one time and all had the
propodus of each cheliped clipped to reduce damage sustained
during any antagonistic encounters.
Crabs were fed once daily a mixed diet of trawled crabs
(Portunid .e.E..�J. prawns (Penaeus £E...:...) and pilchards (Sardiops
neopilchardus) and any uneaten food was removed the following
morning.
The temperature in the tank closely followed that of ambient
seawater as heating facilities were not available and fresh
seawater from the harbour was exchanged daily. A screen of 70%
shade cloth was placed over the tank to prevent excessive algal
blooms.
4
.6.1.. Induction of Spawning
Methods used to induce female crabs to spawn (extrude eggs)
included bilateral and unilateral eyestalk ablation (Heasman and
Fielder, 1983) and environmental stimulation. Table 1 shows the
effect of the three husbandry techniques on induction of spawning.
Both physical eyestalk removal techniques induced crabs to
spawn (60% and 70% for bilateral and unilateral ablation
respectively) however bilateral ablation caused high mortality
(22.5%) of crabs in the 24 hours following eyestalk removal. This
was most likely due to excessive loss of haemolymph and other
stress related factors.
Bilateral
Eyestalk 40 24 12.4 + 4.4
Ablation (60%)
Unilateral
Eyestalk 10 7 21.9 + 13 1
Ablation (70%)
Eyestalks 38 7 27.4 + 29
Intact (18.4%)
Tank
Environment
-----------------------------------------------------------
5
On the other hand, crabs subjected to unilateral eyestalk
ablation showed a low mortality level (10%) equivalent to that of
intact crabs held communally. Similar results have been reported
for Penaeus monodon (Liao and Chen, 1982) where mortality for
bilaterally ablated prawns was higher than for those unilaterally
ablated and bilateral ablation was therefore not considered a
practical procedure for induction of spawning in this species.
The mean time taken for intact crabs, held in the broodstock
tank, to spawn naturally was 27.4 + 29 days (n = 7). Eyestalk
ablation markedly reduced this time and increased the percentage
of crabs which extruded eggs. Plate 2 shows a berried female mud
crab holding approximately 1 million eggs.
3: Hatching
Although broods of eggs were obtained in a shorter period of
time when crabs were subjected to eyestalk ablation, the quality
of first stage zoea larvae (Z 1) obtained at hatch was variable.
Broods extruded after bilateral ablation produced very poor Z1
with only 25% of the hatches providing larvae suitable for
culture i.e.. vigorous and feeding at the water surface. Hatching
was often extended over a 24 hour period resulting in prezoea or
Z1 larvae which lacked vigour and fell out of the water column.
Total mortaltiy occured within 24 hours of hatch.
In contrast, broods extruded after unilateral ablation or
natural spawning showed strong hatching synchrony (0600-0800 hrs)
at 29 ± 1°c and produced good quality larvae in 71% of··. the cases.
Samples of larvae from these hatches were used in larval rearing
trials.
6
Plate 1 Fibreglass broodstock holding tank. The tank is
4.5 sq.m and has afalse bottom covered in 20 cm
depth of calcareous sand.
7
1-•
, ..t:,., (..,r, t:J'.i -.J (:Cl 1...0 C.�
1::::;::1 13;) ($) ,:s::i C5:I ,::s:i ($1
-��-�.........--.1.,..--L--.J
i.Ci
r.::
!J!
:- �-;
n .<
n ·1:i
m :-:.
!fi n
Ul !li
...h !M-:
\l:
!M.:o
<
�:J
,-,
'-'
tn ffO!--:
::'�t��
!3:)•
I�•� !·•--
u;0·:a = r.::r
::c
lf!
1 n::
nJ -1;
!t::t. [ !l! ·1
-1 D.1
II' c. i-'• r-T
!.:I: i c. !l !
I' :J
,.r.)
.,r
!Citi
;\
"..
"' t
t..
!M ...� �!r·r-J ..!l1
-r r :-r <
t�i [] - m !J.:
..§
M: ,:
: IT!
1-4- ,-::r,
er:, !.n �-
II :.::i
•.!.JI llfll •.,�. Cl
o:::i cb
�-..i
o:�
persisted during the latter larval stages (Z4, Z5).
Exuvia entrapment at the first metamorphosis from Z5
Megalopa caused excessive mortality. Affected animals died during
ecdysis after being unable to free their appendages and eyes from
the exuvia or alternatively some individuals shed their exuvia
but lacked vigour and died soon after. Similar symptoms have been
experienced for larvae of Macrobrachium rosenbergii and Palaemon
serratus larvae and juvenile lobsters, Homarus §.E..:.. (Brock, 1982).
It is thought that this is largely due to inadequate larval
nutrition.
Thesurvival rate from Z1 to C1 was very low and averaged
approximately 0.5%. This is in the order of survival rates (4-9%)
claimed by Japanese technicians for culture of Scylla (Cowan,
1984) but is much lower than the 26% survival recorded by Heasman
and Fielder, (1983) indicating that site variation may contribute
significantly to the success of mud crab larval rearing.
Improvement of larval nutrition needs further investigation
before commercial numbers of juvenile mud crabs will be reared.
8
addition, non-replicated continual surveillance trials which
utilized time-lapse video recording, enhanced data obtained for
the above trials with crab behavioural observations.
9
F" i (;i !...I. i·- E·! ��:j ,; i�·::1 5;. j_ in u. 1 -��!. t. E· d l:::! f:.-:� -73. C r--1 ·::::- )/ ;::. L t-:: I! 1 ( i·-1 .=:1 -::;;. f:.:.·, r·i c:, n 2-. cl e �-:. j_ i;} rt !-.••
,..! yI
Ciu_j_ nn
c.-tnd r:-:tE:J.c!E�f:-� J_C)/':3) :1
c..
b
. . . .•... .·:.
.....
.. . .....
· .. -��
. .. ..··'·.... - ·.,
. ·�.. ..: :· ...
..
.. . .... .. ..
• • '
i
60
"\\'i)� OU( "t"tt>E ltJ •nbe. 0� "t"l\)£:: UJ
"'M'j WG-1-\-1.
M
I 50
N
s
A 40
C
'I
I
u
I 30
T
y
/' 20
H
0
u
R 1.0
0 ........_________________________________,
0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 1. 2 2 ·2 2 2
1. 2 3 4 5 6 ? 8 9 0 1. 2 3 4 5 6 ? 8 9 0 1. 2 3 4
0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0
0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 -0 0 0 0 0 0
HOURLY INTERUAL
emmulating a pool of water typically found beneath a rock or leaf
in the mangrove zone at low tide.
Two (2) wild caught C6 mud crabs (av.C.W.= 21 + 1.45mm) were
used in each trial and were fed twice daily on minced crab
(Portunid .§.E...:..). The trial was repeated three (3) times and each
trial was run until one of the crabs moulted (5-6 days). Closed
circuit television and time-lapse video recording were used to
continually observe crab behaviour.
The state of the water level appeared to have a marked
effect on juvenile mud crab behaviour (fig. 6). The number of
antagonistic contacts and time spent actually moving over the
bottom and foraging for food was much greater at "high tide". At
"low tide" the crabs were mostly inactive i.e. either buried in
the sand in both subtidal and intertidal zones or occupying an
artificial shelter.
Three (3) crabs were observed to moult. Two (2) of these
crabs moulted at high tide between 0507 and 0607 hrs. whereas the
third crab moulted at low tide at 1500 hrs.
All crabs moulted in the subtidal zone and were positioned
on the sand substrate. They did not occupy the artificial
shelters which were large enough to accomodate a moulting crab.
All of the newly moulted (softshelled) crabs were attacked
and consumed by their hardshelled conspecifics, almost
immediately following ecdysis. It appeared that the hardshelled
crabs were able to detect the presence of a moulted crab by a
chemical stimulus. On several occasions the hardshelled crab,
which had been buried and had the sight path blocked by a
shelter, became active and made its way directly to the
10
softshelled crab. The chemical involved was most likely moulting
hormone (Fielder, pers. comm.).
From these results obtained by small-scale tank trials, it
appears that the imposition of a simulated tidal system is not an
effective method for reducing cannibalism of softshelled juvenile
mud crabs.
Other factors against this system of management include:
(a) subadult (70-150 mm C.W.) and adult (150-200 mm C.W.)
crabs, which represent the marketable product, migrate into the
intertidal zone at high tide but mostly retreat back to the
subtidal zone with the falling tide (Hill et.al.,1982). Therefore
stocking density would increase at low tide and hardshelled as
well as softshelled cannibalism could account for major
mortality.
- - . - (b) - good-·- quality brackish/sea water would be·· necessary
constantly for water replacement
(c) High pumping costs.
11
predators. Heasman (1980) showed that recently moulted crabs are
able to flee from or repel (by threat postures) other hardshelled
crabs within several hours (1 - 2% of the intermoult duration) of
moulting.
This trial was set up to determine if juvenile mud crabs had
preferred intermoult and moulting sites when provided with
naturally occurring substrates.
Results
Hardshelled crabs significantly (P < 0.001) preferred coral
rubble and detritus substrates to sand as sites for normal
intermoult behaviour during the period of observation (tables
2 & 3).
12
Table 2. Total number of juvenile mud crabs found occupying the
coral rubble or sand for 6 observations during 0800 and
1800 for 7 days. Three (3) treatment replicates with 3
crabs per rep.
TREATMENT
RUBBLE SAND
DAYS (total number crabs) (total number crabs)
Rep 1 Rep 2 Rep 3 Rep 1 Rep 2 Rep 3
1 18 17 18 0 1 0
2 17 13 18 1 5 0
3 17 12 17 1 6 1
4 15 7 9 3 11 9
5 13 13 10 5 5 8
6 14 13 8 4 5 10
7 9 13 11 9 5 7
Subtotal 103 88 91 23 38 35
TOTAL 282 *** 96
1 17 16 17 1 2 1
2 15 13 16 3 5 2
3 16 18 13 2 0 5
4 15 17 13 3 1 5
5 17 17 17 1 1 1
6 16 18 13 2 0 5
---------------------------------------------------------
7 15 14 15 3 4 3
Subtotal 111 113 104 15 13 22
TOTAL 328 *** 50
*** Significant at 0.1% level
When crabs moulted in the coral/sand treatment 83.3% (n= 12)
of the exuvia were found on the sand surface. Thus there appeared
to be a distinct preference for different sites to carry out
intermoult and moulting activity. This was a very desirable
attribute because by simple preference for a substrate the
vulnerable softshelled stage had removed itself from the vicinity
of the predatory hardshelled crabs.
There did not appear to be any preference for the sand
substrate as a moulting site when combined with detritus. Only
56% (n= 9) of the exuvia were found on the sand.
A possible reason for the difference between detritus and
coral rubble may concern the type of cavity available for
seclusion. Although the coral had interstitial cavities suitable
for hardshelled seclusion they may not have been large enough for
ecdysis. thus forcing the crab to leave the coral. On the other
hand, the nature of the detritus (organic debris) was such that
the cavity size was undefined i.e. the substrate could be easily
moved by both hard and softshelled crabs to create larger
cavities if desired.
Although the treatment combinations caused different
behavioural patterns there was no effect on cannibalism at this
stocking density as survival for all treatments was 100%. Further
trials investigating the treatment effect at higher stocking
density were not carried out for the following reasons.
If a rubble system were to be established in a pond
situation several problems could arise.
(a) The rubble would have to provide intersticies large
enough to accomodate subadult and possibly adult crabs. This
13
would only be accomplished with large rocks/bricks which in most
cases would have to be trucked onto site. This would be a costly
procedure.
(b) Siltation, which would reduce the efficiency of the
system by reduction of cavity volume, could be a major problem.
Desilting would require removal. at least in part, of the rubble
which would be labour intensive and therefore expensive. The
frequency of this procedure would depend largely on the quality
(amount of suspended solids) of the incoming exchange water.
(c) Harvesting could present problems as seine netting would
be impossible and ineffective over the rocky bottom; drain
harvesting would again require the removal of the rubble to
capture secluded crabs. Trapping would be the only other
alternative and this can lead to bias sampling as well as
incomplete population harvest.
(d) By providing a wide range of cavity sizes the smaller
crabs may remain within the rubble to moult. thus becoming
vulnerable to larger intermoult crabs also occupying this zone.
The use of organic detritus in a pond situation would not be
possible due to the potential water quality problems. however it
would be possible to provide inert artificial materials which
created a similar environment.
The following trials were based on development of this idea.
14
Artificial Substrate Trials
Introduction
Preliminary observations of large numbers of C1 C4 mud
crabs held in tanks supplied with two (2) types of artificial
material, namely fibreglass fl¥Wire and nylon onion bags, had
shown that both materials provided highly desired habitats.
Juvenile mud crabs spent most of their time secluded
underneath or within folds of the mesh and only left to forage
for food. Antagonism between conspecifics was reduced and this
appeared to be related to the amount of artificial material
present.
A large proportion of the exuviae were found on the mesh and
it seemed that the moulting crabs required a substrate which
they could grasp to facilitate extraction from the exuviae. Many
of the exuviae were firmly attached with the dactyl of .their
perieopods (walking legs) wrapped around the matrix of the mesh.
When C4 crabs were observed in the process of moulting it
also appeared that extraction from the exuvia was accomplished
faster on a mesh substrate than on a bare tank surface.
Although both artificial materials provided similar
substrate qualities the onion bag was chosen for further
investigations for the following reasons.
(a) Onion bags were much cheaper than comparitive areas of
flymesh and they were
(b) readily available.
(c) The open weave of the bags allowed better water
circulation and did not accumulate detritus as quickly as
flymesh.
15
(d) The bags were purchased in a form suitable for
establishment in a pond situation whereas the fl¥Wire required
work such as cutting and folding/stitching to create darkened
cavities.
The tanks were checked twice daily for any exuviae and the
position in which they were found was recorded. The number of
16
surviving crabs was recorded every 2-3 days. The trial was
conducted for 31 days.
The amount of available shelter had a marked effect on
survival of the crabs (table 4). On the whole. the number of
surviving crabs was related to the number of layers of shelter.
Survival was lowest (22%) in the treatment with 1 shelter and
highest (61%) in the treatments with 2 and 4 layers. The number
of mortalities was closely related to the number of moults which
occurred on the bottom level (figure 8). Food was placed onto
this level so it is likely that most of the crabs present there
were foraging and were therefore potential cannibals.
This may explain why survival in treatment 2 was the same as
treatment .4. 71% and 77% of the moults occured above the bottom
layer for treatments 2 and 4 respectively whereas only 55%
occurred above the bottom layer in treatment 3. This may indicate
that the position where moulting occurred was largely
opportunistic. The treatments were not replicated so no real
conclusions about site preference can be drawn.
The data does show however that juvenile crabs occupied
artificial shelter which was present at varying depths throughout
the water column.
17
Figure /. Experimental tank �;�L=m t.he
effect of artificial shelter m1...td
J
7
C
----..::::r-==:::::::::
I.4-
,.
d - j::3__ -
I-
'-;- ..
,_
,_ .
.
._
'
..
.
b
.'
'
' '
a.
1 17 14 82 22
2 24 7 29 61
3 22 11 50 39
4 26 7 27 61
Treatment 2
-I , -,. - -_ - I
,_J, -(
17
\ •: -." - .- . : : -: - · I
7 7
Treatment 3
4
·1 ------. · · · · �. -
•• : •. •' ••••• :
;i 8
10 11
I
Treatment 4 5
..............
.............
7
8
6 7
18
Artificial Shelter/Sand Trial
Introduction
The above trial had shown that crabs would utilise
artificial shelter for both intermoult and moulting behaviour
however the shelter was not tested in conjunction with a natural
substrate.
It was thought that the mud crabs may have had a greater
preference for a natural fluid substrate in which they could
bury, and therefore the inclusion of artificial shelter in a pond
would be pointless if the crabs did not use it.
Two trials were run in concert to determine the effect of
artificial shelter with a sand substrate on crab behaviour and
survival.
Continual Surveillance Trial
Materials and Methods
This trial utilised continuous video observation and was
carried out in a single 100 litre (Nally Pty Ltd) container,
which was established with two levels of artificial shelter (225
and 337 mm depth) and a bottom level of calcareous sand. Each
layer of substrate occupied 1/3 of the container surface area
(plates 3 & 4).
The artificial layers were constructed from plastic
cardboard covered in nylon mesh bag. Excess onion bag was bunched
to create folds in which the crabs could hide. The sand layer was
positioned centrally and was approximately 5 cm deep.
Water was air-lifted from the tank into a coralline
biofilter and reconditioned water was returned to the tank below
the sand layer. This prevented turbulence and allowed undisturbed
19
Plate 3. Surveillance camera used in conjunction with time
lapse video recorder for observation of juvenile
mud crab behaviour.
20
Figure 9. The mean number of Juvenile mud crabs found occupying
the substrate layers per hour (total of 4 by 15 minute
observations). Substrate consisted of 2 layers of onion bag
at 225 <LOW) and 337mm <HIGH> depth respectively and a
bottom sand layer.The tank was stocked with 9 C6 mud crabs
initially and the trial ran for 5 days.
N 20
u
M
B
E
R J.5
0
F
C 1.0
R
A
B
s
5
0
�
\f? :9
J. J. J. J. 1. 1. 1. 1. 2 2 2 2 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1. 1.
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 l. 2 3 0 1. 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 1.
3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3 3
-
0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 � 0 0 0
HOURLY INTERUAL
HIGH SHELTER-++- SAND -e- LOW SHELTER
however 8 exuvia were found during the 7 day acclimation period.
Five (5) moults occurred on the 337 mm depth shelter, 1 on the
225 mm depth layer and 2 on the sand. Mortality did not occur
over the course of the trial.
The trial indicated that artificial shelter was actually
preferred for seclusion over the natural fluid sand substrate.
Inactive crabs also preferred to be as far away as possible from
the feeding site.
21
Plate 5. Experimental 100 litre tank. Substrate provided
consisted of vertically suspended artificial
onion bags and a bottom sand substrate.
crabs were placed into each tank. Crabs were fed as in the
previous trial.
Observations of any exuvia and their position were noted
twice daily. A population census was taken approximately every
third day. The trial was run for 23 days.
Results
The results showed that addition of the vertically suspended
bags significantly (P<0.001) increased survival of the juvenile
mud crabs (table 5).
As a consequence of increased survival, the biomass after 23
days was also significantly (P<0.01) greater in the plus bag
treatment. There was no difference between the mean weight of
individual surviving crabs however the range in the plus bag
treatment was larger and both replicates of this treatment
-comprised Cg-·cra-bs:- -There were no C9· crabs-·present -±n · the---mmd -
only treatment as that these larger crabs were consumed, as they
attempted to moult, by smaller conspecifics.
This is contrary to the result obtained by Holland et.al.
(1971) when blue crabs, Callinectes sapidus were held
communally in a tank with a sand bottom only. They found that
crabs became larger as the population became smaller (due to
cannibalism) and their trial was terminated when the population
was comprised of one large crab.
These authors did report however that a lessening of
cannibalism (as was expected) was not observed as the population
in each aquarium was diminished. This was also observed in our
trial (figure 10).
22
Fi(;JLtr... t:::· :l() ., -rf1E� E1f-f<::-. :<ct �.J•;· "L!!t.:= r-::1·-·e.·=-1::::nce c1f \/�:-:.1 1---t.j.c:.::·:�11·-y-- �::;1...1.�::;pencJi::·:-:1cl t::i.:;:·•. g
shelter □n growth (success of moulting) and surv1va1 of
juvenile mud crabs. 25 mud crabs ranging from L4 to C7 were
stocked initially. The trial ran for 23 days.
50.00
M
E
A
N
N 40c00
0
M
0
u 30.00
L
T
....,
C
2() e (}Q
T
A 10c00
, ___ *
----
L ---- =
I --¾---------$-�
T
V
'
0:00-+-tit"----------,----------.---------.--------,
,
0= (H) 12 ... i){) 18= i)(i 24: ()(}
Table 6. The number of exuviae found on the sand and the mesh
bags in treatment 2.
SAND MESH TOTAL
Repl 20 19 39
Rep2 20 15 35
TOTAL 40 34
Discussion
Results of the above 2 trials show conclusively that the
addition of vertically suspended artificial shelter greatly
increases juvenile mud crab growth and survival.
23
MEAN FOOD CONSUMED <xBWt)
1-n. i•.•.:. m
n:!�) I-"- 1:��� (,�,) t•� IC,,.H'j (Jl"'N .....� 0:) •.,Q n.i�
;-,.:. :i
::r:·:· m
�-
1::L ::::!
m
n !:u _,!
·:
::i .....
,.C! :,
C
..J:::: ::::
'""r'
_,
-:; 11
:...i.
!
J
: I
-,
__:
CL !.i1
U'l !--
-,-, 1T.!
C C:L
o_ :�:
[1.ir·l-
..,( 1:
!.11
···<
r-=·
() '���' i,-,.. 1-• t·J
Q OJ t•,J r.:r- C:•
t,,._.1
,:) () 0 0
l.11 i.J" ,_,,
1' +
+ ,, ·I·
<
I
,. ·I-
,, /T
,, i
+
,'i' !'.IJ
+ + ,,
,,I
I-'
,' -1...1., :!:L!::i
+ /+
()
::i:: ()
rn
J,-..j
,, //
G"I
:1: ,/ +
1'
ll
-..j
Ci
·+:
'C)
"1 /'/
1 + l �ji -h
:1•,
I·•.) ,,:; .....
3: / m Cl
r- --� 0..
t:t i::::i
1'
()
1/
('") ,,
;1J
l�
1;1:1 ,,.,/
.......
J.Ci I
,'
....... ,.
+ ,I
(,•J
I'
i:::::i
()
,'/(
/ ..ffj,
+ 1,
,
·-·
,· o. rn
,I +
fll1
!-,•I+
,I
·'1'
_,,
J.t
-h ··!<
••••••i••,1:::
The crabs showed a strong preference for the artificial
shelter, especially in the shallower regions, for normal
intermoult activity.
Both hardshelled and softshelled cannibalism was
significantly reduced by providing sites (cavities) for seclusion
in which crabs could either carry out moulting or escape to after
having already moulted. The crabs were able to remain here until
their new shell hardened and they could continue normal
intermoult activity.
Provision of the artificial shelter markedly reduced contact
and therefore antagonism between conspecifics which resulted in
fewer crabs losing limbs. This is very beneficial as generally
loss of appendages results in smaller increments of moult
(smaller sized animals) and may prolong intermoult duration
_______ . dep_ending _on the _ number o .f . 1 imbs __ requiring regener.afi.on
(Hartnoll, 1982).
Feeding Trials
Several trials were set up to determine the quantity of food
consumed by juvenile crabs and the effect of feeding frequency on
survival.
24
L Individually contained crabs
The first trial investigated food consumption of individual
juvenile crabs. Fifteen (15) wild caught juvenile mud crabs
ranging in size from 0.007 - 3.679g were held individually in 1
litre containers of sea water (salinity = 31.5ppt; Temp =25 °c).
Each crab was presented a known quantity of 20% agar bound prawn
pellet at 4 hourly intervals. The food was placed on a 53 um mesh
screen to facilitate retrieval. After 4 hours any remaining
pellet was removed from the container, drained for 1 minute on
blotting paper and weighed (Mettler AE 166 balance). The amount
of food consumed was calculated by difference. Preliminary trials
had shown that pellet weight did not change after 4 hours
immersion.
The feeding trays were cleaned and a new cube of food placed
into the crab container. Water was exchanged daily and the trial
ran for 6 days after a 7 day acclimation period.
Results
More food was consumed during 1000 - 2200 hrs however some
food was consumed during all 4 hourly observations (figure 11).
Food consumption (dry wt. food/wet wt.crab * 100) was highly
variable from day to day and between crabs. The mean amount of
food consumed by 15 crabs over the 6 day period was 27.2 + 22.6
%Biomass/day.
There was a distinct reduction in the quantity of food
consumed as the crabs became larger (figure 12). Smaller mud
crabs (C2 - C5) usually moult every 5 - 6 days (at 29 - 30 °c)
whereas the larger crabs in the trial (C7) moult approximately
every 14 days. Therefore the demand for food by the smaller crabs
25
may be greater due to the shorter intermoult duration.
An optimal feeding strategy would be to supply food
constantly because although a feeding peak was observed, some
food was consumed during every 4 hour period. Also because the
amount of food consumed varied daily, a strict feeding regime
could not be set. Better management would involve constant
observation of the amount of food present in a pond and altering
the quantities supplied accordingly.
26
quality was maintained. Prior to each feeding session any
remaining food and waste was siphoned from the tanks.
The experimental design was completely randomized with 4
replicates of each treatment. Experimental containers were 0.11
m 2 and each received reconditioned water from a central coralline
biofilter.
Each tank was stocked with 10 hatchery reared
(C.W.=15.9mm) mud crabs. An equal amount of bunched artificial
material was provided for shelter.
Daily observations of the number of remaining crabs and any
newly moulted crabs were carried out. The weight of the newly
moulted crabs was determined and the amount of food given to each
tank was adjusted accordingly. The trial ran for 28 days. Results
of the trial were subjected to ANOVA to determine any treatment
significance.
Results
Table 7 shows that frequency of feeding had no effect on
survival or growth of the mud crabs. This indicates that the
minced crab had a similar quality after 2.5 and 9 hours
immersion. The food was usually noticed to be present but in
diminishing quantities as the day progressed.
27
Table 7. The effect of 2 frequencies of fee2ing on survival of 10
juvenile (C5) crabs held in 0.11 m tanks. Each tank was
fed a total of 60% biomass per day for 28 days.
Frequency of Feeding
2 times/day 6 times/day
Rl R2 R3 R4 Rl R2 R3 R4
Survivors 9 3 4 8 8 5 4 6
-----------------------·
mean
-----------------------------------
6.0 n.s mean 5.0
= =
Tot. Wt(g) 31.5 12.4 17.8 30.2 25.6 22.2 17.1 28.9
crabs
mean = 23.0 n.s mean = 23.5
28
Therefore in order to overcome potential quality problems
with pelletised foods the most practical feeding scheme would be
one of frequent application of small quantities of food.
29
tank was observed regularly (every 1- 2 hours) and provided when
necessary. Feeding commenced at 0730-0800 and continued to 2 400
hrs.
The tank water was aerated continually and at least 100% of
the volume was exchanged daily. Four (4) 2 00 Watt aquarium
heaters were used during the cooler winter months in an attempt
to raise the water temperature. Temperature ranged from 21.5 to
0
2 9.5 C.
At 3-4 day intervals the tank bottom was siphoned to remove
waste. At approximately 14 day intervals the tank was completely
drained and the crabs were measured (C.W.mm) and weighed to
determine biomass increase. The tank was checked twice daily for
any exuviae. The trial ran for 1 2 3 days.
30
Figure 13. The µbrL�11� survival and number of □ 2
□
juvenile mud crabs grown for 123 days in rc�_,} ��.11]·=·__:,:· s,,,-.,· \,��--.!.i;'.�._•, ! -� ·_!.•--�
.r·• .• �:. �
•
. :.
1
:·,�· ·- • ·•1�..::
. '._:
1 1
100. OO-r----'"'I
s
u
R 8(J .. ()(>
V
I
V
A
L
60.00
-t4aa:!
-!tfl!!ili
itl+W
N '
u ,w#
M 40.00 #"
,-,
-lf't
'.i ..
E �:l!:i
,H+!i-#
�..\Hlf,. ·
•·
.uHt+
2t). (H)
+
#--!ttit-
T ..,.rii".,i..\HfT
s *+
o. oo,-+1----------,�-------........---------,--------�
0.00 40.00 8(): (1{) 120.00 160.00
DAYS
SURVIVAL
'l.
+ TOTAL NUMBER OF MOULTS
metamorphosis). This size crab represents the marketable Asian
product (Baliao et.al.,1981). An example of this sized crab is
shown in plate 7. The first C13 crab of 120mm (300g) was obtained
in 118 days (182 days from metamorphosis).
Figure 14 shows the growth rate of hatchery reared crabs in
this study was greater than that recorded by Heasman (1980). The
mean temperature experienced in both trials was similar i.e.
26.4 + 2.1 and 27.0 ± 0.5 °c respectively.
The intermoult duration for successive instars was similar
in both trials however the moult increment in the present study
was larger in the later stages (Cg - C12>- In fact the carapace
width of a C 12 crab in this trial was equivalent to that of a C13
crab in Heasmans trial.
The high sustained moult increments (30%) of the latter
stages in the present trial may be explained by differences in
feeding strategy and amount of food consumed. In our trial food
was supplied ad libitum in small quantities from 0800 to 2400
each day. The food was generally consumed within 1 2 hours
after which time more fresh food was provided. An average of 10%
(wet wt. food : total biomass) was consumed each day.
In Heasmans trial however food was provided in excess once
daily and uneaten food was removed about 12 hours later. The
mean amount of food consumed per day was 3.3% total body weight.
This 3-fold reduction in consumption when fed once daily to
excess (in this case prawn, fish and mussel) may indicate that
the quality of the food had deteriorated rapidly leading to
smaller consumtion rates. A deficiency in either quality or
quantity of food depresses the rate of crustacean growth by
31
Plate 6. Small-scale growout tank (4.5 sq.m). 40 onion
bags were suspended vertically through the water
column.
150.00
I
N
s
T 120.00
A
C
A
R 90.00
A
p
A
C
E 6(} c: (}()
D
T
''
u 30.00
m
m
X PRESENT TRIAL
+ HEASMAN (1980)
either reducing the increment or lengthening the intermoult
(Hartnoll, 198 2 ). In this case with mud crabs it appeared that
only moult increment was affected.
Therefore by providing high quality food constantly,
consumption and consequently growth rates can be increased.
Table 8 shows that production (5580 kg/ha) in the present
trial was approximately 6 times greater than that of mud crabs
grown in experimental earthern ponds at SEAFDEC (Baliao et.al.,
1981). Crabs were able to be held at much higher initial stocking
density (7. 2 crabs/m 2 ) with relatively low mortality (36.4%) when
provided with artificial shelter whereas crabs grown in earthern
ponds at 2 crabs/m 2 sustained major mortality (69%) with
subsequent low net production (948 kg/ha).
The relative growth increment (g/day) of the SEAFDEC crabs
was greater than that of crabs grown in our trial. It is most
likely that the higher temperatures experienced in the earthern
ponds (25 34 °c) contributed to this by shortening the
intermoult duration.
Autotomy (the sacrificial loss of limbs during antagonistic
encounters) was experienced throughout the growout period and
increased as crabs became larger (figure 15). Walking and
swimming legs were autotomized more often than chelipeds. At the
termination of the trial 14.3% of the crabs were missing one
cheliped and 2 9.0% were missing other limbs.
The 14.3% missing chelipeds would not be suitable for market
as approximately 60% of the edible flesh is contained within the
claws. These crabs would have to be on grown to the next instar
when the claws would be regenerated (smaller than the original)
32
Table 8. Growth and productivity of mud crabs grown in tanks with
artificial substrate (present study) and in experimental
earthern ponds at SEAFDEC (Baliao et.al., 1981).
A Present study
B & C - Treatments at SEAFDEC
S.D. - Stocking Density (crabs/ha.)
Figure 15. The number(%) of juvenile mud crabs missini
appendages during 123 days of continued growout in a 4.5 m
fibreglass tank supplied with ar�ificial shelter. Crabs were
stocked initially at 7.2 crabs/m�.
Cl)
�
100
(C 90
�
u 80
� 70
0 60
I:ii:l
c.,
50
(C 40
foot
30
u 20
� 10
I:ii:l
� 0
1 14 24 38 67 77 100 108 123
DAYS IN GROWOUT TANK
or discarded if this was not economically viable.
At the end of the growout trial the population consisted of
crabs ranging in size (C.W.) from 67.8 - 1 2 0.0 mm or C10 - C13-
Therefore a proportion of the population had not reached a
marketable size. Recent research by Aiken and Waddy (1988) on
lobsters Homarus americanus has shown that they also display a
wide variation in growth rate when held communally in tanks.
When the lobsters were sorted into size groups and restocked
2
at 2 5/m at 60 day intervals the results showed that each group
reflected the growth pattern of the original population. That is
some of the lobsters in each group grew very rapidly while others
continued to grow slowly. This resulted in the faster growing
lobsters of the smaller group surpassing the slower growers of
the larger lobster group. This growth pattern was caused by
social pressure. That is, "growth rate for any individual is
determined by genetic potential and the relative size of other
individuals in the tank."
The size variability of the mud crabs may have been caused
by similar social pressure. Therefore the potential to increase
growth rates, by regular grading and restocking of ponds may
exist. This must be investigated further.
CONCLUSIONS
33
( 2) The mud crabs preferred the artificial material to sand for
both intermoult and moulting behaviour.
(6) Juvenile mud crabs consumed food during both day and night
and food should therefore be provided continually each day.
(8) The amount of food consumed by individual crabs each day was
variable and therefore regular observation of consumption rate
(using retrievable food trays) should be carried out and
adjustments made accordingly.
34
(9) It may be possible to achieve greater productivity in an
earthen pond with suspended artificial substrate. In the present
growout trial a sand or mud substrate was not provided. In a pond
situation this substrate would provide a further dimension for
seclusion as well as promote generation of natural benthic flora
and fauna. Supplementation of a formulated diet with naturally
occurring prey organisms may enhance overall nutrition which
could lead to faster growth rates.
35
REFERENCES
36
Quinn,R.G. and D.R.Fielder.1978. A laboratory beach system for
prolonged maintenance of sand crabs, Mictvris
(Latreille,1806) and Scopimera (De Haan, 1833) (Decapoda,
Brachyura). Crustaceana 34(3):310-313.
37
Table 5. The effect of vertically suspended artificial bag
shelter on survival and growth of juvenile mud crabs.
Two replicates per treatment. An initial stocking
density of 25 crabs. The trial was run for 23 days.
No. Total Mean Crab Cumm. % Crabs
Surv. Biomass Crab Size Number Missing
Crabs (g) Weight Range Moults Limbs
(g) (g)
NO BAGS
REP 1 4 24.3 6.07 3.2-7.4 16 50
+2.0 C 7-C 8
REP 2 6 23.0 3.83 2.2-5.9 23 50
-----------------------------------------------------------------
+1.4 C5-C8
MEAN 5.0 23.7 4.95 19.5 50
PLUS BAGS
REP 1 15 76.0 5.07 2.1-11.1 39 20
+2.6 C1C9
REP 2 17 85.9 5.05 0.7-13.6 35 29.4
±4.0 C5-C9
MEAN 16.0 81.0 5.06 37.0 24.7
*** ** n.s * *
*** - significant at 0.1% level
** II
1 • 0% II
* II
5 • 0% II
38