Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology 2nd Edition Harry Reis all chapter instant download
Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology 2nd Edition Harry Reis all chapter instant download
https://ebookfinal.com/download/handbook-of-research-methods-in-
personality-psychology-1st-edition-richard-w-robins/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/research-methods-for-social-
psychology-2nd-edition-dana-s-dunn/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/handbook-of-research-methods-in-
experimental-psychology-stephen-f-davis/
Handbook of Child Psychology Social Emotional and
Personality Development 6th Edition William Damon
https://ebookfinal.com/download/handbook-of-child-psychology-social-
emotional-and-personality-development-6th-edition-william-damon/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/relationships-well-being-and-
behaviour-selected-works-of-harry-reis-1st-edition-reis/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/principles-and-methods-of-social-
research-2nd-edition-william-d-crano/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/research-in-psychology-methods-and-
design-7th-edition-edition-goodwin/
https://ebookfinal.com/download/research-methods-and-statistics-in-
psychology-5th-edition-hugh-coolican/
Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality
Psychology 2nd Edition Harry Reis Digital Instant
Download
Author(s): Harry Reis, Charles M. Judd
ISBN(s): 9782013024730, 2013024738
Edition: 2
File Details: PDF, 22.25 MB
Year: 2014
Language: english
Handbook of Research Methods in
Social and Personality Psychology
Second Edition
This indispensable sourcebook covers conceptual and practical issues in
research design in the field of social and personality psychology. Key
experts address specific methods and areas of research, contributing to a
comprehensive overview of contemporary practice. This updated and
expanded second edition offers current commentary on social and
personality psychology, reflecting the rapid development of this dynamic
area of research over the past decade. With the help of this up-to-date text,
both seasoned and beginning social psychologists will be able to explore the
various tools and methods available to them in their research as they craft
experiments and imagine new methodological possibilities.
HARRY T. REIS is Professor of Psychology in the Department of Clinical
and Social Sciences, University of Rochester. He is the coauthor of An Atlas
of Interpersonal Situations and the coeditor of The Encyclopedia of Human
Relationships.
CHARLES M. JUDD is College Professor of Distinction in the Department
of Psychology and Neuroscience at the University of Colorado at Boulder.
He is the author of Data Analysis: A Model Comparison Approach and
Research Methods in Social Relations.
Handbook of Research Methods in
Social and Personality Psychology
Second Edition
Edited by
Harry T. Reis
University of Rochester
Charles M. Judd
University of Colorado at Boulder
32 Avenue of the Americas, New York, NY 10013-2473, USA
www.cambridge.org
Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9781107600751
© Cambridge University Press 2000, 2014
This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the
provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of
any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge
University Press.
Author Index
Subject Index
Contributors
John A. Bargh
Yale University
Veronica Benet-Martínez
Pompeu Fabra University
Elliot T. Berkman
University of Oregon
Jim Blascovich
University of California
Marilynn B. Brewer
University of New South Wales
Heining Cham
Fordham University
Tanya L. Chartrand
Duke University
Robert B. Cialdini
Arizona State University
William D. Crano
Claremont Graduate University
William A. Cunningham
University of Toronto
Rick Dale
University of California
Jan De Houwer
Ghent University
Alice H. Eagly
Northwestern University
J. Mark Eddy
University of Washington
Craig K. Enders
Arizona State University
Leandre R. Fabrigar
Queen's University
Susan T. Fiske
Princeton University
Shelly L. Gable
University of California
Bertram Gawronski
University of Texas at Austin
Kevin J. Grimm
University of California
K. Paige Harden
University of Texas at Austin
Richard E. Heyman
New York University
Oliver P. John
University of California
Blair T. Johnson
University of Connecticut
Charles M. Judd
University of Colorado at Boulder
Deborah A. Kashy
Michigan State University
David A. Kenny
University of Connecticut
Norbert L. Kerr
Michigan State University
Nuri Kim
Stanford University
Jon A. Krosnick
Stanford University
Paul J. Lavrakas
Northern Arizona University
Matthew D. Lieberman
University of California
Kristen A. Lindquist
University of North Carolina
Todd D. Little
Texas Tech University
Yu Liu
Arizona State University
Michael F. Lorber
New York University
Michael R. Maniaci
University of Rochester
Kerry L. Marsh
University of Connecticut
Gina L. Mazza
Arizona State University
Gary H. McClelland
University of Colorado
Dominique Muller
Pierre Mendes France University at Grenoble, University Institute of
France
Karen S. Quigley
Northeastern University and Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial (Bedford)
VA Hospital
Harry T. Reis
University of Rochester
Mijke Rhemtulla
University of Amsterdam
Michael J. Richardson
University of Cincinnati
Ronald D. Rogge
University of Rochester
Alexander M. Schoemann
East Carolina University
Eliot R. Smith
Indiana University
R. Scott Tindale
Loyola University Chicago
Eric Turkheimer
University of Virginia
Penny S. Visser
University of Chicago
Duane T. Wegener
The Ohio State University
Stephen G. West
Arizona State University
Tessa V. West
New York University
Keith F. Widaman
University of California
Vincent Y. Yzerbyt
Université catholique de Louvain at Louvain-la-Neuve
Introduction to the Second Edition
When we put together the first edition of this Handbook, published in 2000,
we scarcely could have imagined the pace with which methodological
innovation would occur in social and personality psychology. To be sure,
we hoped that the field's relentless pursuit of ever-more creative and precise
methods would continue – a pursuit that the book was intended to
encourage. Our expectation was that a new edition would be needed
somewhere in the far distant future. A mere 13 years later, that time has
come. Social-personality psychologists have advanced the frontiers of
methodology at a far faster rate than we anticipated, so much so that the
prior volume of this Handbook no longer did justice to the diverse
approaches and methods that define the field's cutting-edge research. With
these advances in mind, we set out to provide under a single cover a
compendium of the most important and influential research methods of
contemporary social-personality psychology.
Our goal for this volume is the same as it was for the prior edition: to
inform and inspire young researchers to broaden their research practices in
order to ask and answer deeper, more finely grained questions about social
life. One sometimes hears that methodological innovation provides little
more than an incremental gain on what is already known. In our opinion,
this view is short-sighted. As Greenwald (2012) observed, the great
majority of Nobel Prizes in the sciences have been awarded for
methodological advances rather than for theoretical contributions. This, he
reasons, is because of the synergy between methodology and theory:
Existing theories point to the need for new methods, which then suggest
questions that could not have been envisioned, much less investigated, with
older methods. In this way, new methods open the door to better
understanding of phenomena.
Social-personality psychologists have always been quick to capitalize on
new methods and technical innovations to further their exploration of the
processes that govern social behavior. Although the field continues to be
criticized for overrelying on laboratory experiments conducted with college
student samples, we believe that this criticism is short-sighted. As this
volume illustrates, social-personality psychologists conduct research using
diverse approaches, ranging from neuroscientific methods to observational
coding of live interaction, from implicit assessments to everyday experience
studies, and from priming outside of awareness to population-based
surveys. Furthermore, the Internet has made possible access to diverse and
specialized samples, an opportunity that social-personality psychologists
have eagerly embraced. Add to this the sophisticated insights afforded by
new or improved statistical innovations such as dyadic data analysis,
mediation analysis, and multilevel models, and it is readily apparent that
our theories are built on a rich, complex, and mature empirical foundation.
We suspect that our receptivity to innovation is one reason for the
growing popularity and influence of social-personality psychology.
Membership in the Society for Personality and Social Psychology has more
than doubled since 2000, and social-personality psychologists are now often
found in schools of business, medicine, and law. The influence of our work
extends well beyond the field's traditional borders, so much so that Yang
and Chiu (2009), in an analysis of citation patterns in APA journals,
identified social psychology as being positioned at the center of the
psychological sciences. We believe that this influence is at least partly
attributable to our leadership in championing methodological innovation.
For example, Baron and Kenny's classic paper on moderation and
mediation, published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology in
1986, is the most cited article of all time in scientific psychology, with more
than 34,000 citations at the time of this writing.
Changes in the field's methodology do not occur in a vacuum, of course.
Two important developments have been the rapid increase in digital
technology and miniaturization, which have led directly to implicit
methods, fMRI, and portable devices for recording details of everyday
behavior, as well as in the accessibility of the Internet, which has opened
the door to a broader pool of research participants. Other kinds of changes
have also been influential. For example, the past decade has seen
impressive gains in statistical methods. Although many of these methods
are computationally complex, they encourage researchers to ask far more
intricate and revealing questions than could be asked with t-tests, ANOVAs,
and correlations. These changes notwithstanding, careful readers will note
that our approach to the research process is still grounded in the basics: a
concern for internal validity, an appreciation for the complexity of
generalizability, and the realization that the most useful and accurate
insights will come from programs of research that incorporate multiple,
diverse methods.
An easy way to see the rapid pace of methodological innovation in social-
personality methods is to compare this edition of the Handbook to its
predecessor. The roster of chapters in the current edition represents an
extensive revision from the earlier volume. Twelve chapters are entirely
new to this volume, discussing topics whose particulars or importance have
emerged since publication of the prior volume. These include treatments of
field research, implicit methods, methods for social neuroscience and
behavior genetics, research on the Internet, methods for studying emotion
and dynamical systems, multilevel models, advanced psychometrics,
missing data, and mediation and moderation. An additional introductory
chapter presents a compelling picture of why we do research. Readers of the
first edition will notice that six chapters have been dropped, not because of
diminished relevance but rather because there was no way to include them
and still have the space necessary to describe newer methods. The
remaining chapters have been, in most cases, thoroughly revamped to
reflect recent developments in method or application. We believe that the
result depicts state-of-the-art methods in social-personality psychology, at
least (we feel compelled to point out) for today.
When the two of us entered the field, in the 1970s, a young social-
personality psychologist could be considered well trained after taking two
courses in statistics and measurement and one in methods. Fortunately, that
is no longer the case; methodological training in most graduate programs is
far more extensive and continues for the duration of one's career. Although
some may see this as a daunting challenge, we prefer to see it as a sign of
the health and vigor of our discipline. Social-personality psychologists are
dedicated to obtaining the most enlightening, accurate, and useful
understanding of the social world in which we live. Taking advantage of
methodological innovation to imagine and address newer, more informative
questions is the surest way we know to continue the progress of the past
few decades. We hope this volume serves as a springboard for the next
generation of theoretical advances in social and personality psychology.
Our every expectation is that the methodological advances in the years
since the first edition of this volume will only continue to accumulate in the
years ahead. We have little doubt that the future promises more appropriate
and sophisticated models of data, greater attention to process and
mechanisms, increased insights from neuroscientific explorations, greater
attention to data from diverse samples and settings, and increased insights
in the measurement of automatic responses. And we have no doubt that
there are further advances lurking down the road that will come with some
surprise. Accordingly, in another dozen years (or perhaps sooner), we
suspect it will be time for a third edition of this volume. One certain
prediction that we make is that we will not be the editors of that edition. But
we trust that others will realize the excitement of witnessing the
methodological vitality of the field by preparing that next edition.
Throughout this volume we have loved providing witness to the advances in
research methods mentioned earlier in this paragraph.
References
Greenwald, A. G. (2012). There is nothing so theoretical as a good method.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(2), 99–108.
Yang, Y. J., & Chiu, C. Y. (2009). Mapping the structure and dynamics of
psychological knowledge: Forty years of APA journal citations (1970–
2009). Review of General Psychology, 13(4), 349–356.
Introduction to the First Edition
Harry T. Reis and Charles M. Judd
Reference
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant
validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin,
56, 81–105.
Chapter one Scratch an Itch with a Brick
Why We Do Research
Susan T. Fiske
Intellectual Puzzles
If science starts with an itch, a discrepancy, or a discontent, we build or use
a theory to test explanations. We may detect gaps in existing theory, and
this is the platonic ideal for science, as many chapters in this volume
illustrate.
Alternatively, researchers may pit two theories against each other,
sometimes supporting one to the exclusion of the other, but more often
determining the conditions under which each is true. For example, in close
relationships research, one might pit attachment theories (Shaver &
Mikulincer, 2010) against interdependence theories (Rusbult, Drigotas, &
Verette, 1994), but in fact both can operate simultaneously, one at an
individual-difference level and the other at a situational level. Still, to the
extent that two theories make distinct predictions, the suspense often
captures a researcher's (and a reader's) imagination.
Some researchers commit to a meta-perspective, such as evolutionary or
functional explanations, and apply them to the problem at hand, building
support for that perspective. For example, an evolutionary approach might
argue that people mistrust out-groups because it has often been adaptive to
stick with your own kind (Neuberg & Cottrell, 2008), and specific research
questions follow from these prin- ciples.
Another intellectual strategy borrows a neighboring field's theories and
methods, applying them to social and personality phenomena. For example,
social cognition research originally began by applying nonsocial models of
attention, memory, and inference to social settings, discovering where
common principles did and did not apply (see Fiske & Taylor, 2013 for
more specific examples). For instance, attention is captured by novel social
stimuli, just as by novel nonsocial stimuli (Taylor & Fiske, 1978; McArthur
& Post, 1977). However, attention is also captured by information about
another's intention (Jones & Davis, 1965), so uniquely social principles
sometimes apply to other people, versus things, as objects of perception. So,
borrowing from an adjoining field can illuminate what is unique about
personality and social approaches.
Still another intellectual strategy of research ideas is going back in time
to the earliest psychological writings. Some reread Aristotle (e.g., regarding
social animals; Aronson, 2004); some like the French National Archives
(e.g., regarding emotion theory; Zajonc, 1985). Myself, I like William
James (Fiske, 1992).
Scientists also construct theories from scratch, sometimes going from the
top down with a metaphor that seems to capture an important reality, such
as depicting willpower as a muscle that can get fatigued (Baumeister &
Alquist, 2009). Sometimes theories follow from the bottom up, beginning
with data, where a systematic program of research consistently yields
particular patterns that demand a systematic explanation. For example,
neural responses to face perception suggest that trustworthiness is the first
and primary dimension that emerges, and theory then describes why that
might be the case (Oosterhof & Todorov, 2008). All these then are
intellectual motivators of research.
Personal Experiences
We don't often admit this outside the family, but psychological scientists do
often get ideas from personal experience. We are after all part of our own
subject matter. Informal sources of formal theory are legitimate, as long as
the informal insights are then stated in a systematic and testable form
(Fiske, 2004b). Not all theory has to be expressed in mathematical form –
indeed, in social and personality psychology, most is not – but it does have
to be logical, parsimonious, and falsifiable, unlike common sense. That is,
even theory that derives from personal experience has to be accountable to
empirical tests.
Being keenly interested in human behavior gives us an advantage in
drawing ideas from experience. As trained social observers, we notice
behavioral patterns that others miss. Indeed, McGuire (1973) exhorted
graduate students to observe the real, not just what others have said or what
the sanitized data say.
Within this approach, the trick is, as Lee Ross puts it, to “run the
anecdote” (personal communication, October 12, 2011). If a story, a hunch,
or even fiction seems to capture an important human truth, social and
personality psychologists can design studies that simulate that phenomenon,
to see if it survives the transition from imagination to a reality that
replicates reliably. This volume provides instructions for how to do exactly
that.
One caveat: New investigators sometimes fall into the trap of doing me-
search – that is, studying their own thorny psychological issues, their own
in-group's preoccupations, or some intense idiosyncratic experience. The
problem here is that, although highly motivated, one may not be the most
objective judge of an issue that is too close to home. At worst, one may be
too invested in a certain result, and equally bad, one might have no insight
at all. At a minimum, the motivational biases we investigate might also bias
our interpretation of our results (Kahneman, 2011). At best, one has some
relevant insights and an open mind about whether these testable ideas
produce interpretable data. Only then is one really ready to learn something
scientifically new and reliable, as a result of personal experience.
Group Identities
Many of us go into social psychology because it focuses on the variance
explained by situations, and situations can be changed, to benefit people's
well-being. If you think a social problem is caused by context, that is
potentially a social policy issue, but if you think the social problem has
genetic causes, that does not lend itself to easy societal solutions. One
important social issue in today's multicultural, globalizing world is
intergroup relations – by the author's estimates from conference talk titles,
representing the preoccupation of about a quarter to a third of social
psychology. As our field itself becomes more heterogeneous, more of us are
thinking about various phenomena related to ethnic, racial, cultural, gender,
sexual, age, disability, and other diverse identities.
On the principle of “nothing about us without us,” many of the
researchers studying these issues come from the affected groups. This
presents both opportunities and challenges. The opportunities come in our
field's chance finally to represent the underrepresented. Prejudice research,
for example, has gone from merely studying the perpetrators to studying the
targets, and target-perpetrator interaction (e.g., Richeson & Shelton, 2007),
enriching the science of intergroup interaction, as well as the broader field,
with new more widely applicable insights and methods.
The group-identity research faces challenges parallel to the me-search
challenges, in what might be viewed as we-search. Besides the perils of
lacking objectivity, one is also accountable to a larger identity group, whom
one certainly does not wish to alienate with findings that might cast the
group in a poor light. This issue arises even more for outsiders studying
issues relevant to traditionally oppressed groups, for example, men studying
gender and white people studying black experience. Ultimately,
membership is not required to conduct good group-related science, but
insights do derive from lived experience, and collaboration is one solution
to keeping identity-relevant research both sensitive to politics and respectful
to lived experience. However, even in these cross-identity collaborations,
one must consider whether foregrounding one colleague gains credibility
with one audience (e.g., subordinates), and foregrounding the other gains
credibility with another audience (e.g., dominants). Peter Glick and I
considered this issue in our ambivalent sexism research (e.g., Glick &
Fiske, 1996), deciding for this reason, among others (including who
ultimately did more work), to foreground the male member of our
collaborative team.
Worldview Defense
Even more fraught but also honestly inspiring is research conducted to
examine one's own worldview, whether religious, political, or moral. But
ideology and science make uncomfortable bedfellows, so this is an
enterprise to enter only with both eyes wide open. One has to go into it with
the goal of testing cherished assumptions and being willing to find them
wanting. For example, liberals and conservatives emphasize distinct moral
bases of judgment (Haidt, 2007), and the role of each may unsettle both
ends of the spectrum. The inquiry is permissible if one agrees to play by the
rules of science. Fortunately, reviewers and editors keep us honest, with no
axe-grinding permitted in the ideal case.
Comment
Sources of ideas are as varied as scientists, and we can cluster these sources
in various ways. For example, in a classic exhortation to the field, McGuire
(1973) listed creative sources as including: paradoxical incident, analogy,
hypothetico-deductive method, functional analysis, rules of thumb,
conflicting results, accounting for exceptions, and straightening out
complex relationships. I do not disagree, and the interested reader is
referred to that earlier account.
Publish or Perish
We do research partly to get a job. Even if we are hired to teach certain
classes, covering certain areas, we are promoted for research published in
refereed journals, preferably high-impact ones. Quality, not just quantity,
counts here. For example, many tenure, promotion, and award committees
consult the h-index (Harzing, 2007), which calculates an author's number of
citations relative to the number of total publications, thereby balancing
quality and quantity. Journals can also be evaluated this way, to calculate
their impact factor, although many journals now use sheer number of
downloads, as well as citations, to gauge their status. These indices all tend
to converge, which is reassuring for measurement reliability and validity.
Collaborate
Some of the more people-oriented among us do research partly for the
rewards of collaboration. When we team up to do science, synergy arrives,
joy happens, and companionship shares the inevitable tribulations of the
research enterprise. In my humble opinion, cooperation is conducive to
good science.
From these teams, we develop networks to connect for friendship and
consultation through a career's lifetime. Interdisciplinary collaborative
research in particular often creates the leading edge in science; ideas catch
fire when fields rub up against each other, creating the future networks of
our sciences. The more social and behavioral scientists learn about the
strength of weak ties and the importance of support systems, the more we
should seek these linkages in our professional lives. Joint research is one
way to do this.
Teach
We also do research, among other pragmatic reasons, to inform and
motivate our teaching. Contrary to popular belief, teaching and research
complement each other. In teaching ratings, research productivity correlates
with the professor's rated knowledge, commitment, enthusiasm, and
organization (Feldman, 1987). Admittedly, research does not correlate with
rated time spent on teaching, there being only so many hours in a day. But
students are evidently energized by a teacher who researches.
Serve
Exploring the Variety of Random
Documents with Different Content
however their brain works, their pulse beats neither faster nor slower
for the common accidents of life. There is, therefore, something cold
and repulsive in the air that is about them—like that of marble. In a
word, they are modern philosophers; and the modern philosopher is
what the pedant was of old—a being who lives in a world of his own,
and has no correspondence with this. It is not that such persons have
not done you services—you acknowledge it; it is not that they have
said severe things of you—you submit to it as a necessary evil: but it
is the cool manner in which the whole is done that annoys you—the
speculating upon you, as if you were nobody—the regarding you,
with a view to experiment in corpore vili—the principle of dissection
—the determination to spare no blemishes—to cut you down to your
real standard;—in short, the utter absence of the partiality of
friendship, the blind enthusiasm of affection, or the delicacy of
common decency, that whether they ‘hew you as a carcase fit for
hounds, or carve you as a dish fit for the gods,’ the operation on your
feelings and your sense of obligation is just the same; and, whether
they are demons or angels in themselves, you wish them equally at
the devil!
Other persons of worth and sense give way to mere violence of
temperament (with which the understanding has nothing to do)—are
burnt up with a perpetual fury—repel and throw you to a distance by
their restless, whirling motion—so that you dare not go near them, or
feel as uneasy in their company as if you stood on the edge of a
volcano. They have their tempora mollia fandi; but then what a stir
may you not expect the next moment! Nothing is less inviting or less
comfortable than this state of uncertainty and apprehension. Then
there are those who never approach you without the most alarming
advice or information, telling you that you are in a dying way, or that
your affairs are on the point of ruin, by way of disburthening their
consciences; and others, who give you to understand much the same
thing as a good joke, out of sheer impertinence, constitutional
vivacity, and want of something to say. All these, it must be
confessed, are disagreeable people; and you repay their overanxiety
or total forgetfulness of you, by a determination to cut them as
speedily as possible. We meet with instances of persons who
overpower you by a sort of boisterous mirth and rude animal spirits,
with whose ordinary state of excitement it is as impossible to keep up
as with that of any one really intoxicated; and with others who seem
scarce alive—who take no pleasure or interest in any thing—who are
born to exemplify the maxim,
‘Not to admire is all the art I know
To make men happy, or to keep them so,—
I apprehend, with his own countrymen or ours, all the love and
loyalty would come to little, but for their hatred of the army opposed
to them. It is the resistance, ‘the two to kill at a blow,’ that is the
charm, and makes our fingers’-ends tingle. The Greek cause makes
no progress with us for this reason: it is one of pure sympathy, but
our sympathies must arise out of our antipathies; they were devoted
to the Queen to spite the King. We had a wonderful affection for the
Spaniards—the secret of which was that we detested the French. Our
love must begin with hate. It is so far well that the French are
opposed to us in almost every way; for the spirit of contradiction
alone to foreign fopperies and absurdities keeps us within some
bounds of decency and order. When an English lady of quality
introduces a favourite by saying, ‘This is his lordship’s physician, and
my atheist,’ the humour might become epidemic; but we can stop it
at once by saying, ‘That is so like a Frenchwoman!’—The English
excel in the practical and mechanic arts, where mere plodding and
industry are expected and required; but they do not combine
business and pleasure well together. Thus, in the Fine Arts, which
unite the mechanical with the sentimental, they will probably never
succeed; for the one spoils and diverts them from the other. An
Englishman can attend but to one thing at a time. He hates music at
dinner. He can go through any labour or pain with prodigious
fortitude; but he cannot make a pleasure of it, or persuade himself he
is doing a fine thing, when he is not. Again, they are great in original
discoveries, which come upon them by surprise, and which they
leave to others to perfect. It is a question whether, if they foresaw
they were about to make the discovery, at the very point of projection
as it were, they would not turn their backs upon it, and leave it to
shift for itself; or obstinately refuse to take the last step, or give up
the pursuit, in mere dread and nervous apprehension lest they
should not succeed. Poetry is also their undeniable element; for the
essence of poetry is will and passion, ‘and it alone is highly
fantastical.’ French poetry is verbiage or dry detail.
I have thus endeavoured to shew why it is the English fail as a
people in the Fine Arts, because the idea of the end absorbs that of
the means. Hogarth was an exception to this rule; but then every
stroke of his pencil was instinct with genius. As it has been well said,
that ‘we read his works,’ so it might be said he wrote them. Barry is
an instance more to my purpose. No one could argue better about
gusto in painting, and yet no one ever painted with less. His pictures
were dry, coarse, and wanted all that his descriptions of those of
others indicate. For example, he speaks of ‘the dull, dead, watery
look’ of the Medusa’s head of Leonardo, in a manner that conveys an
absolute idea of the character: had he copied it, you would never
have suspected any thing of the kind. His pen grows almost wanton
in praise of Titian’s nymph-like figures. What drabs he has made of
his own sea-nymphs, floating in the Thames, with Dr. Burney at their
head, with his wig on! He is like a person admiring the grace of an
accomplished rope-dancer; place him on the rope himself, and his
head turns;—or he is like Luther’s comparison of Reason to a
drunken man on horseback—‘set him up on one side, and he tumbles
over on the other.’ Why is this? His mind was essentially ardent and
discursive, not sensitive or observant; and though the immediate
object acted as a stimulus to his imagination, it was only as it does to
the poet’s—that is, as a link in the chain of association, as implying
other strong feelings and ideas, and not for its intrinsic beauty or
individual details. He had not the painter’s eye, though he had the
painter’s general knowledge. There is as great a difference in this
respect between our views of things as between the telescope and
microscope. People in general see objects only to distinguish them in
practice and by name—to know that a hat is black, that a chair is not
a table, that John is not James; and there are painters, particularly of
history in England, who look very little farther. They cannot finish
any thing, or go over a head twice: the first coup-d’œil is all they ever
arrive at, nor can they refine on their impressions, soften them down,
or reduce them to their component parts, without losing their spirit.
The inevitable result of this is grossness, and also want of force and
solidity; for, in reality, the parts cannot be separated without injury
from the whole. Such people have no pleasure in the art as such: it is
merely to astonish or to thrive that they follow it; or, if thrown out of
it by accident, they regret it only as a bankrupt tradesman does a
business which was a handsome subsistence to him. Barry did not
live, like Titian, on the taste of colours (there was here, perhaps—and
I will not disguise it—in English painters in general, a defect of
organic susceptibility); they were not a pabulum to his senses; he did
not hold green, blue, red, and yellow for ‘the darlings of his precious
eye.’ They did not, therefore, sink into his mind with all their hidden
harmonies, nor nourish and enrich it with material beauty, though
he knew enough of them to furnish hints for other ideas and to
suggest topics of discourse. If he had had the most enchanting object
in nature before him in his painting-room at the Adelphi, he would
have turned from it, after a moment’s burst of admiration, to talk of
the subject of his next composition, and to scrawl in some new and
vast design, illustrating a series of great events in history, or some
vague moral theory. The art itself was nothing to him, though he
made it the stalking-horse to his ambition and display of intellectual
power in general; and, therefore, he neglected its essential qualities
to daub in huge allegories, or carry on cabals with the Academy, in
which the violence of his will and the extent of his views found
proper food and scope. As a painter, he was tolerable merely as a
draftsman, or in that part of the art which may be best reduced to
rules and precepts, or to positive measurements. There is neither
colouring, nor expression, nor delicacy, nor striking effect in his
pictures at the Adelphi. The group of youths and horses, in the
representation of the Olympic Games, is the best part of them, and
has more of the grace and spirit of a Greek bas-relief than any thing
of the same kind in the French school of painting. Barry was, all his
life, a thorn in the side of Sir Joshua, who was irritated by the temper
and disconcerted by the powers of the man; and who, conscious of
his own superiority in the exercise of his profession, yet looked
askance at Barry’s loftier pretensions and more gigantic scale of art.
But he had no more occasion to be really jealous of him than of an
Irish porter or orator. It was like Imogen’s mistaking the dead body
of Cloten for her lord’s—‘the jovial thigh, the brawns of Hercules’: the
head, which would have detected the cheat, was missing!
I might have gone more into the subject of our apparent
indifference to the pleasure of mere imitation, if I had had to run a
parallel between English and Italian or even Flemish art; but really,
though I find a great deal of what is finical, I find nothing of the
pleasurable in the details of French more than of English art. The
English artist, it is an old and just complaint, can with difficulty be
prevailed upon to finish any part of a picture but the face, even if he
does that any tolerable justice: the French artist bestows equal and
elaborate pains on every part of his picture—the dress, the carpet,
&c.; and it has been objected to the latter method, that it has the
effect of making the face look unfinished; for as this is variable and
in motion, it can never admit of the same minuteness of imitation as
objects of still-life, and must suffer in the comparison, if these have
the utmost possible degree of attention bestowed on them, and do
not fall into their relative place in the composition from their natural
insignificance. But does not this distinction shew generally that the
English have no pleasure in art, unless there is an additional interest
beyond what is borrowed from the eye, and that the French have the
same pleasure in it, provided the mechanical operation is the same—
like the fly that settles equally on the face or dress, and runs over the
whole surface with the same lightness and indifference? The collar of
a coat is out of drawing: this may be and is wrong. But I cannot say
that it gives me the same disturbance as if the nose was awry. A
Frenchman thinks that both are equally out of drawing, and sets
about correcting them both with equal gravity and perseverance. A
part of the back-ground of a picture is left in an unfinished state: this
is a sad eye-sore to the French artist or connoisseur. We English care
little about it: if the head and character are well given, we pass it over
as of small consequence; and if they are failures, it is of even less. A
French painter, after having made you look like a baboon, would go
on finishing the cravat or the buttons of your coat with all the nicety
of a man milliner or button-maker, and the most perfect satisfaction
with himself and his art. This with us would be quite impossible.
‘They are careful after many things: with us, there is one thing
needful’—which is effect. We certainly throw our impressions more
into masses (they are not taken off by pattern, every part alike): there
may be a slowness and repugnance at first; but, afterwards, there is
an impulse, a momentum acquired—one interest absorbing and
being strengthened by several others; and if we gain our principal
object, we can overlook the rest, or at least cannot find time to attend
to them till we have secured this. We have nothing of the petit
maître, of the martinet style about us: we run into the opposite fault.
If we had time, if we had power, there could be no objection to giving
every part with the utmost perfection, as it is given in a looking-glass.
But if we have only a month to do a portrait in, is it not better to give
three weeks to the face and one to the dress, than one week to the
face and three to the dress. How often do we look at the face
compared to the dress? ‘On a good foundation,’ says Sancho Panza, ‘a
good house may be built’; so a good picture should have a good back-
ground, and be finished in every part. It is entitled to this mark of
respect, which is like providing a frame for it, and hanging it in a
good light. I can easily understand how Rubens or Vandyke finished
the back grounds and drapery of their pictures:—they were worth the
trouble; and, besides, it cost them nothing. It was to them no more
than blowing a bubble in the air. One would no doubt have every
thing right—a feather in a cap, or a plant in the foreground—if a
thought or a touch would do it. But to labour on for ever, and labour
to no purpose, is beyond mortal or English patience. Our clumsiness
is one cause of our negligence. Depend upon it, people do with
readiness what they can do well. I rather wonder, therefore, that
Raphael took such pains in finishing his draperies and back grounds,
which he did so indifferently. The expression is like an emanation of
the soul, or like a lamp shining within and illuminating the whole
face and body; and every part, charged with so sacred a trust as the
conveying of this expression (even to the hands and feet), would be
wrought up to the highest perfection. But his inanimate objects must
have cost him some trouble; and yet he laboured them too. In what
he could not do well, he was still determined to do his best; and that
nothing should be wanting in decorum and respect to an art that he
had consecrated to virtue, and to that genius that burnt like a flame
upon its altars! We have nothing that for myself I can compare with
this high and heroic pursuit of art for its own sake. The French fancy
their own pedantic abortions equal to it, thrust them into the Louvre,
‘and with their darkness dare affront that light!’—thus proving
themselves without the germ or the possibility of excellence—the
feeling of it in others. We at least claim some interest in art, by
looking up to its loftiest monuments—retire to a distance, and
reverence the sanctuary, if we cannot enter it.
‘They also serve who only stare and wait.’[40]
Welcome to our website – the ideal destination for book lovers and
knowledge seekers. With a mission to inspire endlessly, we offer a
vast collection of books, ranging from classic literary works to
specialized publications, self-development books, and children's
literature. Each book is a new journey of discovery, expanding
knowledge and enriching the soul of the reade
Our website is not just a platform for buying books, but a bridge
connecting readers to the timeless values of culture and wisdom. With
an elegant, user-friendly interface and an intelligent search system,
we are committed to providing a quick and convenient shopping
experience. Additionally, our special promotions and home delivery
services ensure that you save time and fully enjoy the joy of reading.
ebookfinal.com