dp10065
dp10065
Kelvin KC Seah
July 2016
Forschungsinstitut
zur Zukunft der Arbeit
Institute for the Study
of Labor
The Impact of Immigrant Peers on
Native Students’ Academic Achievement in
Countries Where Parents of Immigrants
Are Relatively Skilled
Kelvin KC Seah
National University of Singapore
and IZA
IZA
Phone: +49-228-3894-0
Fax: +49-228-3894-180
E-mail: [email protected]
Any opinions expressed here are those of the author(s) and not those of IZA. Research published in
this series may include views on policy, but the institute itself takes no institutional policy positions.
The IZA research network is committed to the IZA Guiding Principles of Research Integrity.
The Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA) in Bonn is a local and virtual international research center
and a place of communication between science, politics and business. IZA is an independent nonprofit
organization supported by Deutsche Post Foundation. The center is associated with the University of
Bonn and offers a stimulating research environment through its international network, workshops and
conferences, data service, project support, research visits and doctoral program. IZA engages in (i)
original and internationally competitive research in all fields of labor economics, (ii) development of
policy concepts, and (iii) dissemination of research results and concepts to the interested public.
IZA Discussion Papers often represent preliminary work and are circulated to encourage discussion.
Citation of such a paper should account for its provisional character. A revised version may be
available directly from the author.
IZA Discussion Paper No. 10065
July 2016
ABSTRACT
This study examines how exposure to immigrant students affects the academic achievement
of native students in the three largest immigrant-receiving countries – United States,
Australia, and Canada. Using a large cross-country dataset, variation in the share of
immigrant children between different grade levels within schools is exploited to identify the
impact of immigrant peers. I find that exposure to immigrant children has dissimilar effects on
native students’ achievements across the three countries. While exposure has a positive
impact on Australian natives, it has a negative impact on Canadian natives. Exposure has no
effect on U.S. natives. More importantly, I find that institutional factors, such as the way in
which countries organise their educational systems, have a crucial bearing on how immigrant
students affect their peers.
Corresponding author:
Kelvin KC Seah
Department of Economics
National University of Singapore
AS2 #06-02, 1 Arts Link
Singapore 117570
E-mail: [email protected]
1 INTRODUCTION
1
Exceptions are Neymotin (2009), Ohinata and van Ours (2013), and
Hermansen and Birkelund (2015), who find some patchy evidence that
exposure to immigrant children have positive impacts on native students’
educational outcomes.
2
Zealand2)3. Yet, one would expect immigrant peer effects to be quite different,
possibly even positive, given that the school-going immigrant population in
these countries tend to be from more privileged socioeconomic backgrounds.
Because the immigrant children who are at the focus of existing studies
typically come from less-privileged backgrounds, peer socioeconomic
composition and school immigrant concentration are likely to be negatively
correlated in these studies (Jensen and Rasmussen, 2011). Since most studies
in the immigrant peer effects literature ignore modelling explicitly the effects
2
Unlike in most other high immigration countries, the average educational
level of immigrants in Canada, Australia, and New Zealand stand out as being
much higher than that of natives. See, for example, Antecol et al. (2003).
3
Though Friesen and Krauth (2011) provides some indication on the possible
effects of immigrant peers in a country where the migrant population tends to
be relatively skilled, the results are not generalisable. There are 2 reasons for
this. Firstly, the study focuses only on the effects of 2 specific groups of
immigrant students – those that speak Chinese and those that speak Punjabi at
home. This hardly covers the universe of immigrant students in their country
of study (Canada). Secondly, the evidence is based on student experience in
British Columbia and may not be representative of student experience in the
other Canadian provinces.
3
which are due to the socioeconomic backgrounds of immigrant schooling
peers, the estimated effects documented in these studies will also include any
effects which are due to the unfavourable socioeconomic characteristics of
migrant peers. Since the estimates capture a “total treatment effect”, the
findings may not be generalisable to those countries where immigrant children
are, on average, from more-privileged socioeconomic backgrounds.
Given the above considerations, the objective of this study is to fill the
gap in the literature by examining whether the negative immigrant peer effects
documented in much of the literature still persists when immigrant students
have relatively higher-skilled parents than native students.
(2) How does student achievement vary with the share of immigrant peers?
(4) Do institutional factors, such as the way in which countries organise their
educational systems, have an influence on the peer effects exerted by
immigrants?
While there have been a small number of previous studies which have
tried to investigate whether academic achievement and the share of immigrant
peers varies non-linearly4, none have attempted to entirely relax the linearity
4
These include Gould et al. (2009), Schneeweis (2013), Szulkin and Jonsson
(2007), and Hardoy and Schøne (2013). Gould et al. (2009) and Schneeweis
(2013) allow for a quadratic relationship between the grade share of
4
assumption by using non-parametric regression methods to recover the
underlying functional relationship between these variables. The present study
will be the first to model any possible non-linearity this way. Existing studies
also rarely investigate how and why immigrant peer effects arise. With the
exception of Hardoy and Schøne (2013) and Ohinata and van Ours (2013), no
other study I know of has attempted to investigate the mechanisms behind
these effects. The present research is among the few which attempts to study
whether the peer effects are possibly generated by differences in the language
ability of immigrants and educational attainment of their parents. The final
research question adds to the literature most appreciably. To my knowledge,
no study has yet examined the relationship between immigrant peer effects
and educational institutions. Yet, the fact that educational policies and
institutions have been found to affect the relative academic achievement of
immigrants (Schneeweis, 2011; Cobb-Clark et al., 2012) implies that it may be
possible for institutional factors to influence the way in which immigrant
students affect others.
In this study, we are interested in examining the peer effects only from
first-generation immigrant students. Given that differences in attributes (e.g.
differences in language ability and/or host-country-specific human capital) are
likely to be sharper between native students and first-generation immigrant
immigrants and student outcomes. On the other hand, Szulkin and Jonsson
(2007) and Hardoy and Schøne (2013) first create dummy variables for the
shares of immigrant peers by grouping these into arbitrarily-sized bins (e.g. 0-
5%, 5-10% immigrants and so on) before regressing student outcomes on
these dummies to allow for non-linearities. Both approaches have their
disadvantages. The former approach restricts the possible functional
relationships analysed to only quadratic and linear forms while the conclusions
yielded by the latter approach are sensitive to the base category specified and
to the size of the bins used to group the migrant shares.
5
students than between native students and later generations of immigrant
students, these students constitute a particularly interesting group to study.
Hence, throughout the rest of this paper, immigrants refer to first-generation
migrants – that is, all students not born in the country where the test was
conducted5. Note also that, throughout the paper, the level of skills possessed
by parents is proxied by their educational attainment.
5
I do not further restrict first-generation migrants to those who have at least 1
foreign-born parent because foreign-born children born abroad of 2 native-
born parents may also have missed out on certain educational experiences
specific to the country during their time of absence. Hence, they are also of
interest to this study. In any case, the results do not differ substantively when
the definition of first-generation migrant students is restricted to those who
were born-abroad and who had at least 1 foreign-born parent.
6
adjacent grades within schools to identify the causal impact of immigrant
peers on natives’ academic achievement.
7
better than native students, this channel allows for immigrants to have a
positive impact on peers’ learning experiences.
The results from this study indicate that exposure to immigrant students
does have an effect on the academic achievement of peers. In particular,
results from both non-parametric regressions and school fixed effects
estimations suggest that while exposure to immigrant peers has a positive
impact on the Math achievement of Australian natives, it has a negative
impact on the Math achievement of Canadian natives. There is no evidence
that exposure to immigrant peers has an effect on the academic achievement of
U.S. natives. Interestingly, the result for Canada suggests that even if
immigrant students have more educated parents than natives, this does not
guarantee that immigrants will have non-adverse impacts on the academic
achievements of peers. Additional tests to uncover the mechanisms behind the
respective peer effects reveal that the peer effects of migrants are more
adverse when they are non-native speakers of the test language and when they
8
have less-educated parents. Hence, within countries, an improvement in the
quality of immigrant children (as measured by language proficiency and
parental education) will likely mitigate any negative effects or enhance any
positive effects which these students may have on the educational outcomes of
natives.
The findings from this study will be useful for education policy-makers
because it enables them to know whether country immigration policies (e.g.
policies aimed at increasing the educational attainments of incoming migrants)
and educational institutions have an influence on the way migrant students
affect their peers.
2 DATA
The data used in this paper is from the Third International Mathematics
and Science Study (TIMSS 1995)6. TIMSS 1995 is an international study
6
Available online from: http://www.iea.nl/data.html. This study was renamed
the “Trends in International Mathematics and Science Study” after 1995.
9
which was conducted across more than 40 countries in 1995. The aim of the
study is to assess the Mathematics and Science achievements of students in 5
grade levels (3rd, 4th, 7th, and 8th grades and the grade constituting the final year
of secondary education) of the participating countries.
7
The other waves of TIMSS (i.e.1999, 2003, 2007, and 2011 waves) assess
the academic achievements of students belonging to only 2 grade levels (4th
and 8th grade). Students in the 4th and 8th grades are typically not drawn from
the same school, making within-school type comparisons infeasible.
10
TIMSS 1995 employed a two-stage sampling design. In the first stage,
schools were sampled with a probability proportional to school size. In the
second stage, intact classes of students were randomly selected from within
the sampled school. Generally, within each school, one classroom would be
randomly selected from the 7th grade and another would be randomly selected
from the 8th grade. Because each respondent provides information on his/her
nativity status (whether born abroad or born in the country of test), one is able
to identify whether a particular observation is an immigrant student. This
information allows one to construct the key variable of interest – a variable
measuring the share of immigrant students in the 7th and the 8th grade of each
school8. In all subsequent analyses, I define immigrant students as all those
born abroad.
8
The share of immigrant children in each grade (both 7th and 8th) of a school is
calculated as follows: First, each student is weighted by the product of the
class weighting factor (inverse of the probability of selection of the classroom
within a school), the student weighting factor (inverse of the probability of
selection of a student within a classroom), and the student weighting
adjustment (adjustment to account for non-participating students in the
selected classroom). Next, for each grade in a particular school, the proportion
of immigrant students to all students is computed, applying the
abovementioned weight. All results are similar if I had instead restricted my
sample to only those schools with one class per grade. In this case, the share of
immigrant children in the 7th grade class would be akin to the share of
immigrant children in the 7th grade of the school. Likewise, the share of
immigrant children in the 8th grade class would be akin to the share of
immigrant children in the 8th grade of the school. The disadvantage of the
latter analysis, though, is that very few schools (and hence, student
observations) would remain in the sample, leading to large standard errors in
the impact estimates.
11
2.1 Sample
12
students would have obtained if they had completed an assessment consisting
of all the items in the 8 test booklets. They are derived using multiple
imputation methods. Throughout this paper, test scores are normalised within
each country and subject, to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 so that
the values presented can be interpreted as fractions of a standard deviation.
13
exposure to immigrant peers. However, there is no reason a priori to believe
why a linear relationship should hold. In this section, I relax the linearity
assumption entirely and, instead, let the data characterise the functional
relationship underlying the variables.
9
In Figures 1 and 2, grade effects are partialed out semi-parametrically prior to
estimation. The bandwidth is set to 0.4 for all regressions. I chose the smallest
bandwidth that provided me with a relatively smooth curve.
14
between adjacent grades of each school are used to identify the peer effects of
immigrants, the relationships depicted in this panel account for the non-
random sorting of students between schools.
10
Throughout most of the distribution, the relationship between the grade
share of immigrants and the grade average Math test scores is negative. The
part of the curve to the left of -0.2 and to the right of 0.2 (of the horizontal
axis) can, for practical purposes, be ignored since the difference in the shares
of immigrants across the 2 grades in the vast majority of schools in the sample
does not exceed 0.2 in absolute value. As such, the regression function is
likely to be poorly estimated at both tails of the distribution where the absolute
difference in the share of immigrants exceeds 0.2.
15
are displayed in Figure 2. The estimated relationships are very similar to those
found in Figure 1. Accordingly, results for Math achievements apply as well to
Science.
3 EMPIRICAL STRATEGY
16
unobserved determinants of student achievement may be correlated with the
share of immigrant students in schools and classes. As such, OLS estimators
of the achievement effect of immigrant peers are likely to be biased.
17
prior to estimation. To do this, equation (1) is first averaged over all students
in each school, 𝑠, to yield:
𝑦̅𝑠 = ̅̅̅
𝑿′𝑠 𝛽 + 𝛿𝐼̅𝑠 + 𝛼𝑠 + 𝜀̅𝑠 (2)
18
The empirical strategy used will allow one to capture causal effects as
long as parents do not react to a high concentration of immigrant students in a
particular grade by moving their children to another school. The estimates
would be biased otherwise. The direction of the bias will depend on the types
of native students who move. If the natives who move are the higher-
achieving ones, then our estimates will be subject to a negative bias (i.e. we
will overestimate the negative effects, or alternatively, underestimate the
positive effects of immigrant students). On the other hand, if the natives who
move are the lower-achieving ones, then our estimates will be positively
biased.
4 RESULTS
19
Table 3 presents results from a variety of regressions showing the
estimated effect of the share of immigrant grade peers on the Math and
Science achievements of native Australian, Canadian, and U.S. students11. For
each subject, 3 columns of results are displayed. The first column shows
estimates from the most basic specification: an OLS specification that controls
only for grade fixed effects. The second column shows the estimates when
school fixed effects are included in the regressions. By including school fixed
effects, the coefficient on the immigrant share variable is identified on the
basis of variation in the exposure to immigrant peers across adjacent grades
within schools. Hence, the results in the second column are free from any bias
due to the non-random sorting of students across schools. Finally, the third
column shows estimates from the most comprehensive specification. This
specification controls for student12 and family background13 characteristics
11
The full set of coefficient estimates for the regressions in Table 3 can be
found in Appendix Tables A1 and A2.
12
Included in the set of student characteristics are dummy variables for the
student’s sex (male or female) and whether the student speaks the test
language frequently at home (this dummy is set to 1 if the student reports
always or almost always speaking the test language at home). It also includes a
continuous variable indicating the student’s age (measured in months).
13
Included in the set of family background characteristics are dummy
variables for the highest level of education attained by the student’s mother
and father, the number of books in the student’s home, and whether the
student lives with both parents. It also includes a continuous variable
indicating the total number of people living in the student’s home.
Observations may fall into 1 of 6 parental education categories: Father
(Mother) had at most primary education or less; Father (Mother) had some
secondary education; Father (Mother) completed secondary education; Father
(Mother) had some / completed vocational education; Father (Mother) had
20
(which may be correlated with both the share of immigrant grade peers and
subject test scores) in addition to grade and school fixed effects.
In all analyses, I present standard errors which are clustered at the school
level to correct for arbitrary correlations in individual error terms within
schools14.
21
deviation leads to a 0.084 standard deviation improvement in test scores15. So
the estimated effects, while non-zero, are arguably small.
For the U.S., the coefficient estimate in column (1) indicates that native
Math achievement is negatively related to the share of immigrant peers in the
grade. However, the estimate in column (2) reveals that this association is
15
An alternative approach is to consider that the school-grade at the 50th
percentile of the immigrant share distribution had 7.4% immigrants while that
at the 75th percentile had 14.6% immigrants. This immigrant share difference
is 7.2 percentage points or 0.81 (=7.2/8.9) standard deviations. The estimate
implies that an increase in the share of immigrants which is sufficient to move
a school-grade from the 50th to the 75th percentile of the immigrant share
distribution would only result in a 0.068 (=0.81×0.084) standard deviation
improvement in Math test score.
22
actually an artefact of the non-random sorting behaviour of students across
schools. Once school differences are accounted for, the statistically significant
relationship between the variables disappears and the coefficient falls to
essentially zero. This result is unaltered when controls for individual and
family background characteristics are added (column (3)).
The above analysis suggests that while exposure to immigrant peers has
a positive impact on the Math achievement of Australian students, it has a
negative impact on the Math achievement of Canadian students. There is no
evidence that exposure to immigrant peers has an effect on the Math
achievement of U.S. students. These results are consistent with those from
non-parametric regressions.
16
None of the conclusions described for Table 3 changes even when log-linear
or log-log specifications are used.
23
4.1 Mechanisms Explaining Immigrant Peer Effects
24
then the impact from immigrant peers with less-educated parents should be
more negative (or less positive) than the impact from immigrant peers with
more-educated parents. This is indeed what I find. Panel A of Table 4 shows
that for all 3 countries, the share of immigrant peers with less-educated parents
has a larger negative impact on natives’ Math and Science test scores than the
share of immigrant peers with more-educated parents (though effects are only
precisely estimated for Australia). Noteworthy are the findings for Australia.
For Math, exposure to immigrant peers with more-educated parents has a
statistically significant positive impact on the achievement of Australian
natives while for Science, exposure to immigrant peers with less-educated
parents has a statistically significant negative impact on their achievement.
17
A student is classified as a native speaker of the test language if he/she
reports always or almost always speaking the language of test at home. He/she
is classified as a non-native speaker of the test language otherwise.
25
test scores is sizeable and statistically significantly negative at the 5% level,
the estimated effect of the share of native language speaking immigrant peers
is small and non-significant. This provides some support for the idea that the
overall negative peer effects documented for Canada are partly driven by the
language deficiencies of immigrant students. Little can be said for
achievements in the U.S. (and for Science achievement in Canada) because the
coefficients on the variables of interest do not reach statistical significance in
any of these estimations.
26
peer effect. Rather, it is the un-modelled difference in attributes possessed by
the two groups of students (i.e. native speaking and non-native speaking
immigrant students) which contributes spuriously to this result. Hence,
attaching a causal interpretation to the above result would be erroneous.
5 DISCUSSION
18
Both countries have a skills-based admissions policy where admission is
based on economic criteria such as educational attainment, occupational
demand, language ability, and age.
27
One potential explanation lies in the fact that how immigrant students
perform relative to natives (and therefore how immigrant students affect peers’
behaviours) is not only influenced by their socioeconomic characteristics but
also by institutional factors such as the way in which educational systems are
organised (Schneeweis, 2011; Cobb-Clark et al., 2012). This means that even
if immigrant students possess better socioeconomic characteristics than native
students, any positive influence that this may have on peer effects may still be
undone if countries adopt education policies or organise their educational
systems in ways that hinder the educational progress of migrants. Because
educational systems are inherently differently organised across countries, the
impacts from exposure to immigrant students can be different in different
countries even if the relative socioeconomic characteristics of immigrant
students in these countries are identical.
19
Data from TIMSS 1995, accessed from: https://nces.ed.gov/timss/idetimss/.
20
School principals were posed with the following question: “How much
influence do teachers of a subject have in determining curriculum?” They
could choose among 4 options: “none”, “a little”, “some”, or “a lot”.
28
such influence21. The extent to which schools are given the autonomy over
curriculum setting is important, because as pointed out by the OECD (2012),
“schools with more autonomy over curricular decisions may be better able to
cater to the particular needs of immigrant students”. They are therefore better
able to present immigrants with more educational opportunities. Because of
the lower autonomy that Canadian schools possess over curricular decisions,
schools there may not be as able as schools in either Australia or the U.S. to
cater to the specific needs of immigrant students. Hence, the peer effects
exerted by immigrants may be worse in Canada than in the other two
countries.
21
School principals were asked: “How much influence does the Principal/Head
of School have in determining curriculum?” Again, they could choose from 4
options: “none”, “a little”, “some”, or “a lot”.
22
In the U.S, the average age at arrival of 7th and 8th grade immigrant children
is 6.276 years.
23
In the U.S., 56.5% of 7th and 8th grade immigrant children report that they
either “always” or “almost always” speak the test language at home.
29
strong predictors of immigrant students’ academic performance (see Cobb-
Clark et al. (2012) and OECD (2012)), the older ages at which immigrant
children arrive in Canada (compared to Australia and the U.S.) and the lower
tendency for Canadian immigrant students to speak the test language at home
(compared to Australian and American immigrant students) may, to some
extent, account for the more adverse effects that these children have on peers’
educational achievements.
Where 𝑦𝑖𝑔𝑠𝑐 denotes the academic performance of native student 𝑖 from grade
𝑔 of school 𝑠 in country 𝑐. 𝑿𝑖𝑔𝑠𝑐 denotes a vector of individual and family
background characteristics for the student. 𝐼𝑔𝑠𝑐 denotes the share of immigrant
children in grade 𝑔 of school 𝑠 in country 𝑐. 𝑅𝑐 is a measure of the institution
of interest for country 𝑐 and so 𝐼𝑔𝑠𝑐 × 𝑅𝑐 represents an interaction between the
24
Institutional variables which vary at the country level cannot be included as
main explanatory variables in specification (4) because they would be
perfectly collinear with the country fixed effects.
30
grade share of immigrants and the institutional characteristic. 𝛼𝑐 , 𝛼𝑠 , and 𝛼𝑔
capture unobserved country, school, and grade heterogeneity respectively and
are accounted for in the regressions through a set of country, school, and grade
dummy variables. The coefficient of interest here is 𝛾 for it captures how the
effect of the share of immigrant peers on student achievement changes as the
institutional feature of interest varies across countries. If the estimate of this
coefficient is negative, then this indicates that the influence of the share of
immigrants on student achievement becomes smaller (i.e. becomes more
negative) as the indicator measuring the institutional feature increases. The
converse is true if the estimated coefficient is positive25.
25
More precisely, the marginal effect of the grade share of immigrant peers on
𝜕𝑦
student achievement is given by = 𝛿 + 𝛾𝑅 (subscripts dropped for
𝜕𝐼
31
Equation (4) is estimated separately for each institutional indicator of
interest. Table 5 reports the results from these estimations. For brevity, only
estimates of the coefficient on the interaction term between the grade share of
immigrant peers and each institutional indicator of interest (i.e. estimates of
the parameter 𝛾 in equation (4)) are reported. These tell us directly how the
effect of the share of immigrant peers on native students’ test scores changes
as the institutional indicator of interest is increased. Statistically significant
negative coefficients indicate that the peer effects of immigrants become
smaller (more negative) as the institutional indicator is increased. Statistically
significant positive coefficients indicate the opposite.
32
they provide a possible explanation for the apparently conflicting findings
within the literature. As noted, while most studies have found immigrants to
have either adverse or no effects on natives’ achievements, a small number do
actually find beneficial effects. This analysis shows that the way immigrant
students affect their peers is ultimately influenced as well by the educational
institutions under which they operate. This is a novel finding in the literature
on immigrant peer effects.
6 CONCLUSION
The results from this study indicate that immigrant students do have an
effect on native peers’ academic achievement. However, the effects of
immigrants are not the same across the 3 countries. While immigrant students
in Australia affect the academic achievements of natives positively, immigrant
students in Canada affect the academic achievements of natives adversely.
However, in both cases, it is only the impacts on natives’ Math achievements
which are statistically significant. The impacts on natives’ Science
achievements, though qualitatively similar, do not reach statistical
significance. There is no evidence that immigrants in the U.S. affect the
academic achievements of natives.
33
The results from Canada demonstrate that even if immigrant children
have better-educated parents than natives, this does not guarantee that they
will have non-adverse effects on native peers’ academic outcomes. How
immigrants affect peers’ academic achievement depends also, and perhaps
more importantly, on the way a country’s educational system is organised.
However, within countries, improvements in immigrant quality (as measured
by language ability and parental education) may lessen the negative effects or
increase the positive effects that immigrants have on the educational
achievements of peers.
34
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
35
REFERENCES
Friesen, Jane and Krauth, Brian. (2011) Ethnic Enclaves in the Classroom.
Labour Economics, 18(5), 656-663.
Gould, Eric D., Lavy, Victor, and Paserman, Daniele M. (2009) Does
Immigration Affect the Long-Term Educational Outcomes of Natives? Quasi-
Experimental Evidence. The Economic Journal, 119(540), 1243-1269.
Hardoy, Inés and Schøne, Pal. (2013) Does the Clustering of Immigrant Peers
Affect the School Performance of Natives?. Journal of Human Capital, 7(1),
1-25.
Hermansen, Are Skeie and Birkelund, Gunn Elisabeth. (2015) The Impact of
Immigrant Classmates on Educational Outcomes. Social Forces, 94(2), 615-
646.
36
International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement.
(n.d) Third International Mathematics and Science Study [Online] Available
from: http://www.iea.nl/timss_1995.html [Accessed 17 Nov 2012].
Jensen, Peter and Rasmussen, Astrid Wurtz. (2011) The Effect of Immigrant
Concentration in Schools on Native and Immigrant Children’s Reading and
Math Skills. Economics of Education Review, 30(6), 1503-1515.
Ohinata, Asako and van Ours, Jan C. (2013) How Immigrant Children Affect
the Academic Achievement of Native Dutch Children. The Economic Journal,
123(570), F308-F331.
37
Schnepf, Sylke V. (2006) How Different are Immigrants? A Cross-Country
and Cross-Survey Analysis of Educational Achievement. In: Parsons, Craig A.
and Smeeding, Timothy M. (eds.) Immigration and the Transformation of
Europe. Cambridge, United Kingdom, Cambridge University Press. pp. 200-
234.
38
Figure 1: Non-Parametric Estimates of the Effect of School-Grade Immigrant Concentration on Natives’ Math Test Scores
Panel A: Relationship between Native Students' Math Test Scores and Share of Immigrant Children at the School-Grade Level
2
Average Math Test Score of School-Grade (Natives)
1
1
0
0
-1
-1
-1
-2
-2
-2
-3
0 .2 .4 .6 .8 0 .2 .4 .6 .8 0 .2 .4 .6
Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade
bandwidth = .4 bandwidth = .4 bandwidth = .4
Panel B: Relationship between Differences in the Share of Immigrant Children between 2 Grades of a School and Differences in Native Students' Math Test Scores
2
2
1
1
0
0
-1
-1
-2
-1
Notes: Data from the Third International Mathematics and Science Study of 1995. Sample weights used in all computations. Grade effects are partialed out semi-
39
parametrically prior to estimation. The bandwidth is set to 0.4 for all regressions.
Figure 2: Non-Parametric Estimates of the Effect of School-Grade Immigrant Concentration on Natives’ Science Test Scores
Panel A: Relationship between Native Students' Science Test Scores and Share of Immigrant Children at the School-Grade Level
2
Average Science Test Score of School-Grade (Natives)
1
0
0
-1
-1
-1
-2
-2
-2
-3
-3
-3
0 .2 .4 .6 .8 0 .2 .4 .6 .8 0 .2 .4 .6
Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade Share of Immigrant Children in School-Grade
bandwidth = .4 bandwidth = .4 bandwidth = .4
Panel B: Relationship between Differences in the Share of Immigrant Children between 2 Grades of a School and Differences in Native Students' Science Test Scores
2
2
1
1
1
0
0
0
-1
-1
-2
-1
Notes: Data from the Third International Mathematics and Science Study of 1995. Sample weights used in all computations. Grade effects are partialed out semi-
parametrically prior to estimation. The bandwidth is set to 0.4 for all regressions. 40
Table 1: Parental Education of Foreign-Born and Native-Born Children, by Country
Notes: Data from the Third International Mathematics and Science Study of 1995. Sample weights used in all computations so
that mean values are nationally representative. Values are proportions. * indicates that the mean value is significantly different
at the 5% level between native and immigrant students.
41
Table 2: Summary Statistics of Variables Used in Estimations (for the Sample of Native Students)
Notes: Unweighted means and standard deviations of all variables included in the estimations, for the sample of native students.
42
Table 3: Estimated Effect of the Share of Immigrant Peers on Native Students' Test Scores
Math Science
OLS OLS (w/School FE) OLS OLS (w/School FE)
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)
Controls for Controls for
Student Student
No Controls No Controls No Controls No Controls
& Family & Family
Characteristics Characteristics
Australia
Share of Immigrant Students in Grade 0.910** 1.095*** 0.935** 0.440 0.563 0.263
(0.383) (0.390) (0.375) (0.382) (0.404) (0.397)
Observations 11,177 11,177 7,488 11,177 11,177 7,488
R-Squared 0.025 0.323 0.385 0.026 0.280 0.371
Canada
Share of Immigrant Students in Grade -0.435** -0.494* -0.557* -0.226 -0.411 -0.205
(0.206) (0.284) (0.289) (0.221) (0.253) (0.264)
Observations 14,906 14,906 9,882 14,906 14,906 9,882
R-Squared 0.041 0.270 0.346 0.026 0.285 0.375
U.S.
Share of Immigrant Students in Grade -1.669*** -0.006 -0.035 -2.612*** -0.280 -0.089
(0.510) (0.530) (0.466) (0.497) (0.413) (0.369)
Observations 9,836 9,836 7,855 9,836 9,836 7,855
R-Squared 0.037 0.404 0.459 0.065 0.412 0.473
Notes: Grade fixed effects are included in all regressions. The student and family background variables control for students’ sex, age, whether or not they speak the test
language frequently at home, household size, mothers’ and fathers’ highest level of education, number of books at home, and whether or not they live with both parents.
Standard errors in parentheses are cluster-robust standard errors that allow for correlation in individual error terms within schools. *** p-value<0.01, ** p-value<0.05, * p-
value<0.1.
43
Table 4: Estimated Effects of the Shares of Immigrant Peers in the High Skill Group and in the Low Skill Group on Native Students' Test Scores
Math Science
Skill/Adaptation Measured Share of Immigrant Peers in High Share of Immigrant Peers in Low Share of Immigrant Peers in High Share of Immigrant Peers in Low
by: Skill/Adapted Group Skill/Adapted Group Skill/Adapted Group Skill/Adapted Group
Panel A
Australia 1.225*** -0.662 0.564 -1.296**
(0.401) (0.558) (0.385) (0.639)
Canada Parental Education -0.242 -1.117 0.024 -0.847
(0.302) (0.723) (0.282) (0.560)
U.S. 0.211 -0.948 0.166 -0.656
(0.614) (0.970) (0.426) (0.757)
Panel B
Australia 0.214 1.925*** -0.103 0.784
(0.422) (0.724) (0.397) (0.644)
Canada Language Use -0.157 -0.806** -0.243 -0.128
(0.424) (0.351) (0.413) (0.316)
U.S. -0.170 0.072 -0.109 -0.070
(0.683) (0.738) (0.577) (0.602)
Notes: This table presents estimates from various regressions, showing the impact of immigrant peers in the high skill/adapted group and the impact of immigrant peers in
the low skill/adapted group. Skill/adaptation is measured by: (1) parental education and (2) language use. Immigrant students are defined as low skilled/adapted when skill
is measured by parental education and if neither of the student’s parents have an education above the secondary level. When adaptation is measured by language use,
immigrant students are low skilled/adapted if they are non-native speakers of the test language. In each case, the number of observations from Australia, Canada, and the
U.S. is 7,488, 9,882, and 7,855 respectively. All regressions control for the grade, sex, age, and household size of students, the number of books the student has at home,
whether the student frequently speaks the test language at home, whether the student lives with both parents, and the highest education attained by both parents. All
regressions also include school fixed effects. Standard errors in parentheses are cluster-robust standard errors that allow for correlation in individual error terms within
schools. *** p-value<0.01, ** p-value<0.05, * p-value<0.1.
44
Table 5: Estimated Impacts of Various Institutional Arrangements on the Peer Effects Exerted by Immigrants
Math Science
Institutional Indicator 1
Share of Students with Teachers who Possess Influence in Curriculum Setting 3.712*** 1.488
(1.315) (1.267)
Full Set of Controls? Yes Yes
Observations 25,225 25,225
R-Squared 0.387 0.399
Institutional Indicator 2
Share of Students with Principals who Possess Influence in Curriculum Setting 7.829*** 3.098
(2.841) (2.711)
Full Set of Controls? Yes Yes
Observations 25,225 25,225
R-Squared 0.387 0.399
Institutional Indicator 3
Average Age at Arrival of Immigrant Students -1.582*** -0.721
(0.508) (0.526)
Full Set of Controls? Yes Yes
Observations 25,225 25,225
R-Squared 0.387 0.399
Institutional Indicator 4
Share of Immigrant Students who Speak the Test Language Frequently at Home 14.012*** 6.403
(4.513) (4.670)
Full Set of Controls? Yes Yes
Observations 25,225 25,225
R-Squared 0.387 0.399
Notes: The coefficients in this table represent estimates of the parameter in equation (4). Each is derived from a separate regression. Negative coefficients imply that the peer effects of
immigrants become smaller (more negative) as the institutional indicator is increased. Positive coefficients imply that the peer effects of immigrants become larger (more positive) as the
institutional indicator is increased. All regressions control for the grade, sex, age, and household size of students, the number of books the student has at home, whether the student
frequently speaks the test language at home, whether the student lives with both parents, and the highest education attained by both parents. All regressions also include country and school
fixed effects. Standard errors in parentheses are cluster-robust standard errors that allow for correlation in individual error terms within schools. *** p-value<0.01, ** p-value<0.05, * p-
value<0.1.
45
Appendix Table A1: Full Set of Coefficient Estimates for Table 3 Regressions – Math Achievement
46
Mother Completed University Education – – 0.381*** – – 0.275*** – – 0.156**
(0.086) (0.049) (0.072)
Father had Some Secondary Education – – 0.156*** – – 0.179*** – – -0.071
(0.059) (0.044) (0.055)
Father Completed Secondary Education – – 0.159** – – 0.234*** – – -0.030
(0.061) (0.045) (0.052)
Father had Vocational Education – – 0.290*** – – 0.330*** – – 0.001
(0.058) (0.048) (0.056)
Father had Some University Education – – 0.205*** – – 0.293*** – – 0.042
(0.069) (0.046) (0.057)
Father Completed University Education – – 0.377*** – – 0.408*** – – 0.091
(0.062) (0.044) (0.058)
Has 11-25 Books at Home – – 0.223** – – 0.067 – – 0.082*
(0.089) (0.055) (0.042)
Has 26-100 Books at Home – – 0.497*** – – 0.288*** – – 0.319***
(0.081) (0.054) (0.040)
Has 101-200 Books at Home – – 0.644*** – – 0.397*** – – 0.446***
(0.081) (0.054) (0.041)
Has more than 200 Books at Home – – 0.762*** – – 0.417*** – – 0.506***
(0.080) (0.054) (0.043)
Observations 11,177 11,177 7,488 14,906 14,906 9,882 9,836 9,836 7,855
R-Squared 0.025 0.323 0.385 0.041 0.270 0.346 0.037 0.404 0.459
Notes: Grade fixed effects are included in all regressions. Standard errors in parentheses are cluster-robust standard errors that allow for correlation in individual error terms within schools. ***
denotes a coefficient significant at the 1% level, ** denotes a coefficient significant at the 5% level, * denotes a coefficient significant at the 10% level.
47
Appendix Table A2: Full Set of Coefficient Estimates for Table 3 Regressions – Science Achievement
48
Mother Completed University Education – – 0.444*** – – 0.337*** – – 0.035
(0.086) (0.045) (0.070)
Father had Some Secondary Education – – 0.157*** – – 0.081* – – 0.043
(0.060) (0.042) (0.055)
Father Completed Secondary Education – – 0.143** – – 0.113*** – – 0.084
(0.062) (0.043) (0.051)
Father had Vocational Education – – 0.318*** – – 0.283*** – – 0.116**
(0.057) (0.045) (0.053)
Father had Some University Education – – 0.225*** – – 0.154*** – – 0.144**
(0.073) (0.045) (0.056)
Father Completed University Education – – 0.372*** – – 0.295*** – – 0.194***
(0.062) (0.043) (0.057)
Has 11-25 Books at Home – – 0.141 – – 0.048 – – 0.115**
(0.093) (0.051) (0.045)
Has 26-100 Books at Home – – 0.398*** – – 0.279*** – – 0.343***
(0.087) (0.050) (0.042)
Has 101-200 Books at Home – – 0.566*** – – 0.460*** – – 0.477***
(0.084) (0.052) (0.045)
Has more than 200 Books at Home – – 0.723*** – – 0.534*** – – 0.553***
(0.087) (0.051) (0.046)
Observations 11,177 11,177 7,488 14,906 14,906 9,882 9,836 9,836 7,855
R-Squared 0.026 0.280 0.371 0.026 0.285 0.375 0.065 0.412 0.473
Notes: Grade fixed effects are included in all regressions. Standard errors in parentheses are cluster-robust standard errors that allow for correlation in individual error terms within schools. ***
denotes a coefficient significant at the 1% level, ** denotes a coefficient significant at the 5% level, * denotes a coefficient significant at the 10% level.
49