Introduction to Language
Introduction to Language
- Lectures -
● Scientific Understanding:
While myths offer fascinating narratives, science lacks definitive answers
regarding the origin of language. Researchers suggest early spoken language
emerged 50,000 to 100,000 years ago, but no direct evidence or artifacts clarify
its evolution.
● Religious Theories:
Many religions propose divine origins of language. In Christianity, Adam named
creatures in Eden, while Hinduism credits Sarasvati, wife of Brahma, with
language's gift. Ancient experiments, like Psammetichus's isolation of infants,
sought to identify the divine language but yielded dubious results.
● Scientific Skepticism:
Experiments isolating infants, such as those by Psammetichus and King James
IV, failed to prove the existence of a divine or "original" language. Instead, they
underscored that language acquisition depends on exposure to speech.
● "Bow-Wow" Theory:
This theory suggests language arose from imitating natural sounds, like
"caw-caw" for birds. Such onomatopoeic origins are evident in words like "buzz"
and "hiss," though it’s insufficient to explain abstract concepts or silent objects.
● "Pooh-Pooh" Theory:
Emotional exclamations like "Ouch!" or "Wow!" might have contributed to early
speech development. However, their distinct production (intakes of breath)
contrasts with normal speech patterns, making them an unlikely source.
● Melodic Beginnings:
Charles Darwin proposed that music preceded language as a means to "charm"
others. Observations of infants’ early melodic interactions support this theory,
but it’s unclear why humans moved beyond melody to complex language.
● "Yo-He-Ho" Theory:
Early humans may have used coordinated grunts and chants during collective
physical activities, embedding language within a social framework. However, this
theory doesn’t explain how specific sounds originated.
The Physical Adaptation Source
● Anatomical Changes:
Human evolution involved physiological changes aiding speech, such as:
○ Upright posture allowing controlled breathing.
○ Smaller, even teeth and intricate lips aiding sound production.
○ A uniquely shaped tongue enabling varied sounds.
○ A lower-positioned larynx and resonant pharynx enhancing vocal range.
These adaptations highlight speech's evolutionary benefits, despite risks
like choking.
● Cognitive Overlap:
Tool-making and speech likely share neural circuits, as both involve intricate
motor control. Studies on ancient stonecutting reveal brain activity patterns
similar to those involved in speech, suggesting a shared evolutionary pathway.
● Manual Communication:
Gesture likely preceded vocal language, as evidenced by chimpanzee signals and
early human infants’ gestures. Over time, vocalization and gestures combined,
with humans developing articulated speech alongside expressive hand
movements.
Human language is distinct from all other forms of communication in the animal
kingdom due to a set of unique properties. These features allow humans to convey
complex, abstract, and infinite ideas. Below is a deeper dive into each property:
1. Reflexivity
● Definition: Reflexivity refers to the ability to use language to think, talk, and
analyze language itself. Humans can reflect on the structure, usage, and
effectiveness of their communication.
● Examples:
○ We can discuss grammatical rules (e.g., "Should I use 'who' or 'whom'?").
○ We critique language use (e.g., "I wish he wouldn't use so many technical
terms").
○ We can even create new words to describe language phenomena (e.g.,
"texting" for sending messages via phone).
● Comparison with Animals: Other creatures do not reflect on their signals or
communication. A dog barking doesn’t analyze its bark or suggest modifications
to another dog’s bark to improve clarity or tone. Reflexivity is uniquely human
because it enables metalinguistic awareness—the ability to use language as a tool
for self-awareness and learning.
2. Displacement
● Definition: Displacement is the ability to refer to things that are not immediately
present in the here and now. This includes discussing past events, future plans,
hypothetical situations, or imaginary concepts.
● Examples:
○ Humans can talk about historical events, like "The signing of the
Declaration of Independence," or plan for the future, like "Next year, I’ll
travel to Italy."
○ Humans also discuss abstract or mythical entities like Santa Claus, fairies,
or heaven.
● Comparison with Animals:
○ Animal communication is typically restricted to the immediate
environment. For example, when a dog growls, it indicates a present threat
("Stay away now").
○ Bees exhibit a limited form of displacement by dancing to communicate
the location of a nectar source, but this displacement is restricted to
recently discovered nectar. They cannot communicate about past nectar
sources or potential future ones. Humans, however, can move freely across
time and space in their language.
3. Arbitrariness
4. Cultural Transmission
These six properties enable human language to be open-ended, infinitely creative, and
capable of expressing abstract, hypothetical, and complex ideas. While some animals
exhibit limited versions of these traits (e.g., displacement in bees), no animal
communication system possesses all these features to the extent seen in human
language. This unique combination makes language a cornerstone of human cognition,
culture, and social interaction.
Introduction to Grammar
Traditional Grammar
Traditional grammar, rooted in the study of Latin and Greek, provides a foundational
framework for understanding language structure. Early grammatical descriptions of
Latin, later applied to English, shaped how we classify words into grammatical
categories, or parts of speech. This tradition introduced terms like noun, verb, and
adjective.
● Nouns are words used to name people, places, objects, qualities, or abstract
ideas (e.g., boy, school, love). Proper nouns, like Cathy or Rome, are capitalized.
● Articles (a, an, the) classify or identify nouns. For example, a banana introduces
an item, while the banana refers to a known item.
● Adjectives describe nouns, adding details such as size or quality (e.g., large
objects, strange experiences).
● Verbs denote actions or states (e.g., go, talk, be). They also express relationships,
as in Jessica is ill.
● Adverbs provide additional context for actions or adjectives, describing how,
when, or to what degree (e.g., slowly, very, yesterday).
● Prepositions indicate relationships involving time, place, or manner (e.g., at
five o’clock, on the table).
● Pronouns stand in for nouns or noun phrases, making sentences less repetitive
(e.g., she, it, themselves).
● Conjunctions connect clauses or ideas, showing relationships between them
(e.g., and, because, when).
The parts of speech framework from traditional grammar remains central to linguistic
studies and language education today.
Grammatical Agreement
Languages operate with rules ensuring that components of a sentence match or "agree"
with each other. This agreement involves several grammatical categories:
● Number: Verbs and pronouns adjust to reflect singular or plural subjects (e.g.,
she runs vs. they run).
● Person: Pronouns and verbs reflect distinctions between the speaker (first
person), the listener (second person), and others (third person). For example, I
am (first person singular) contrasts with he is (third person singular).
● Tense: Verbs are modified to indicate when an action occurs, such as present
(loves) versus past (loved).
● Gender: English relies on natural gender, distinguishing male (he), female
(she), and neutral (it). Other languages, like Spanish, follow grammatical
gender, classifying nouns as masculine or feminine, even when unrelated to
biological sex.
For instance, in the sentence Cathy loves her dog, we see agreement between the
singular noun Cathy and the verb loves. The pronoun her also reflects gender-based
agreement with Cathy. These rules ensure clarity and coherence in communication.
English employs natural gender, basing distinctions on biological sex. Pronouns like
he, she, and they are used to reflect male, female, or neutral references, respectively.
However, other languages, such as Spanish, utilize grammatical gender, classifying
nouns into categories like masculine (el sol, “the sun”) or feminine (la luna, “the
moon”), regardless of the noun’s actual characteristics. German goes further,
incorporating a neuter category (das Feuer, “the fire”). These gender classifications
influence the forms of accompanying words, such as articles and adjectives, to maintain
grammatical agreement. Importantly, grammatical gender does not imply biological sex;
for instance, the French noun le livre (“the book”) is masculine despite having no
inherent gender.
Languages differ in how they arrange subjects (S), verbs (V), and objects (O) in
sentences. English follows a Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) pattern, as in The cat chased
the mouse. However, other languages use alternative orders:
● SOV: Common in Japanese (e.g., The cat the mouse chased).
● VSO: Found in Scottish Gaelic (e.g., Chased the cat the mouse).
● VOS: Seen in Malagasy, spoken in Madagascar.
These word-order patterns form the basis of language typology, the study of
grammatical similarities and classifications among languages. Understanding these
patterns is crucial for second-language learners, especially when their native language
uses different structures.
Grammar helps us comprehend the rules underlying language use and the differences
between languages. For instance, English places adjectives before nouns (red wine),
while other languages reverse this order (wine red). Such insights are essential for
teaching and learning languages effectively.
Moreover, grammar reveals the hierarchical structures underlying sentences, extending
beyond their linear arrangement. These abstract structures, explored further in
advanced studies, highlight the complexity and universality of human language.
Parts of Speech
The parts of speech categorize words based on their roles in a sentence. Below are the
main categories:
1. Nouns: Words that name people, places, things, or ideas (e.g., boy, school, love).
2. Articles: Words that introduce nouns (e.g., a, an, the).
3. Adjectives: Words that describe nouns (e.g., large, strange).
4. Verbs: Words that denote actions or states (e.g., go, talk, be).
5. Adverbs: Words that modify verbs, adjectives, or other adverbs (e.g., slowly,
very).
6. Prepositions: Words that show relationships between nouns and other words,
often regarding time or place (e.g., at, on, with).
7. Pronouns: Words that replace nouns (e.g., he, it, you).
8. Conjunctions: Words that connect clauses or phrases (e.g., and, but, because).
To analyze sentences, consider their structure and how the parts of speech function
together:
1. Linear Order: English follows a Subject-Verb-Object (SVO) word order. For
example:
○ Subject: The noun phrase (NP) performing the action or being described.
○ Verb: The action or state.
○ Object: The noun phrase receiving the action.
○ Adjuncts: Additional information about time, place, or manner (often
prepositional phrases).
3. Constituents: Sentences are made up of smaller units (constituents) that
combine hierarchically. For example:
Roles of Constituents
1. Subjects: Often the "doer" of the verb (e.g., She sings).
2. Objects: Receive the action (e.g., He saw her).
3. Adjuncts: Provide context like where, when, or how (e.g., He sings beautifully).
4. Modifiers: Describe or limit the meaning of other constituents (e.g., A big
house).
Sentence diagrams or tree structures (e.g., Yule’s constituent analysis) visually represent
how constituents combine to form sentences.
Sure! Let’s break down each sentence into its constituents, showing their parts of
speech, grammatical roles (subject, verb, object, adjunct, etc.), and overall structure.
● Parts of Speech:
○ Subject: She
○ Verb: saw
○ Object: that film
○ Adjunct: last night
● Constituents:
○ [S [NP She] [VP [V saw] [NP [Det that] [N film]] [PP [AdvP last
night]]]
● Parts of Speech:
○ I (pronoun, subject)
○ went (verb, past tense)
○ to (preposition)
○ the (article)
○ faculty (noun, object of the preposition)
○ yesterday (adverb, adjunct of time)
○ and (conjunction)
○ got (verb, past tense)
○ a (article)
○ ten (noun, object)
○ at (preposition)
○ the (article)
○ exam (noun, object of the preposition)
● Structure:
○ Clause 1:
■ Subject: I
■ Verb: went
■ Prepositional Phrase: to the faculty
■ Adjunct: yesterday
○ Clause 2:
■ Subject: I
■ Verb: got
■ Object: a ten
■ Prepositional Phrase: at the exam
● Constituents:
● Parts of Speech:
○ Main Clause:
■ Subject: My dad
■ Verb: thinks
■ Object (embedded clause): we are lazy
○ Embedded Clause:
■ Subject: we
■ Verb: are
■ Subject Complement: lazy
● Constituents:
○ [S [NP [Det My] [N dad]] [VP [V thinks] [S [NP we] [VP [V are]
[AdjP lazy]]]]]
● Parts of Speech:
○ Subject: My aunt
○ Verb: does not want
○ Object (infinitive clause): to share the brownie recipe
○ Embedded Clause:
■ Verb: to share
■ Object: the brownie recipe
● Constituents:
○ [S [NP [Det My] [N aunt]] [VP [Aux does not] [V want] [S [VP [V
to share] [NP [Det the] [Adj brownie] [N recipe]]]]]]
● Parts of Speech:
○ I (pronoun, subject)
○ believe (verb, main verb)
○ my (possessive pronoun, determiner)
○ dog (noun, subject of embedded clause)
○ ate (verb, past tense)
○ my (possessive pronoun, determiner)
○ homework (noun, object of embedded clause)
● Structure:
○ Main Clause:
■ Subject: I
■ Verb: believe
■ Object (embedded clause): my dog ate my homework
○ Embedded Clause:
■ Subject: my dog
■ Verb: ate
■ Object: my homework
● Constituents:
○ [S [NP I] [VP [V believe] [S [NP [Det My] [N dog]] [VP [V ate]
[NP [Det my] [N homework]]]]]]
Pragmatics
Key Point:
Pragmatics is the study of how meaning is shaped by context and speaker intention,
rather than just the dictionary definitions of words. It examines how people use
language in real-world situations and how listeners infer meaning beyond the literal
interpretation of words.
Explanation:
When we communicate, we often rely on context to convey meaning effectively.
Pragmatics helps us understand not just what words mean but what speakers intend to
communicate with those words. For example, in a conversation about "war," different
interpretations may arise based on the historical or cultural background of the listener
and speaker. In the story mentioned in the book, American tourists misunderstood a
Scottish boy’s reference to "the war" because they assumed he meant World War II,
while he was actually referring to a much older conflict. This highlights how
communication relies on shared understanding and context.
Key Point:
Pragmatics involves understanding "invisible meaning," where much of what we
communicate is not explicitly stated but is inferred based on shared assumptions and
context.
Explanation:
Often, we understand messages that are not directly conveyed in words. For instance,
consider a sign that reads "Heated Attendant." A literal interpretation might suggest
that the attendant is heated, but through pragmatics, we understand that the sign likely
means a parking lot with an attendant who operates in a heated area. This ability to
extract meaning beyond what is written demonstrates how communication depends on
context and common knowledge shared between speaker and listener. Without such
shared understanding, misunderstandings can arise.
Key Point:
Understanding language involves both physical context (where and when the
communication happens) and linguistic context (the surrounding words or phrases that
provide meaning).
Explanation:
When interpreting language, context plays a crucial role in shaping meaning. Physical
context refers to the immediate environment in which communication takes place, such
as a sign outside a building that reads "Bank," which in that location suggests a financial
institution. However, if the same word is found in the phrase "a steep bank by the river,"
the linguistic context helps us understand that it refers to a geographical feature instead.
The surrounding words, known as co-text, guide our interpretation by narrowing down
the possible meanings of a word or phrase based on how it is used.
Key Point:
Deictic expressions, such as "this," "that," "here," "there," "now," and "then," rely
entirely on context to convey meaning, making them difficult to interpret without
knowing the specific situation in which they are used.
Explanation:
Deixis comes from the Greek word for "pointing," and it refers to words that direct
attention to specific people, places, or times. For example, if someone says, "You'll have
to bring it back tomorrow because she isn’t here today," the meaning of "it,"
"tomorrow," "she," and "here" cannot be understood without knowing the context in
which the sentence is spoken. Deictic expressions help in conversation by anchoring the
meaning to the time, place, and people involved in the discussion, but their
interpretation heavily depends on the listener's familiarity with the situation.
Key Point:
Reference is the act of using language to identify something, while inference is the
listener’s ability to connect what is said with what is meant.
Explanation:
Speakers use various linguistic expressions—such as proper nouns (e.g., "Jennifer"),
descriptive phrases (e.g., "my friend"), or pronouns (e.g., "she")—to refer to people or
things. However, these words alone do not carry fixed meanings; their interpretation
depends on inference. Listeners must use their knowledge and context to connect the
spoken words to real-world entities. For instance, if a waiter says, "The ham sandwich
left without paying," it is understood that the phrase "ham sandwich" refers to a
customer who ordered that dish. This process of making connections beyond literal
meaning is crucial for effective communication.
Key Point:
Anaphora refers to the use of expressions that refer back to something already
mentioned, while cataphora introduces a reference before specifying what it refers to.
Explanation:
When telling a story or providing information, we often introduce new subjects and
then refer back to them later using pronouns or descriptive phrases. For example, in the
sentence, "We saw a funny home video about a boy washing a puppy in a small bath.
The puppy started struggling," the phrase "the puppy" refers back to the previously
mentioned "a puppy," making it an example of anaphora.
In contrast, cataphora introduces a reference using a pronoun before providing the
actual subject. For example, "It was staring at me from the bushes. A large dog had
escaped from the yard." Here, "It" refers to "a large dog," which is introduced later in the
sentence.
Key Point:
Presuppositions are assumptions that speakers make about what their listeners already
know or believe to be true.
Explanation:
Every conversation carries underlying assumptions that speakers and listeners take for
granted. For example, the statement "Your clock isn’t working" presupposes that the
listener owns a clock. Similarly, asking "When did you stop smoking?" presupposes that
the person used to smoke. A key feature of presuppositions is that they remain constant
even if the statement is negated; whether you say "My car is a wreck" or "My car is not a
wreck," the presupposition that you have a car remains unchanged.
Key Point:
Pragmatic markers are short expressions such as "you know," "well," and "I mean" that
help manage the flow of conversation and express attitudes.
Explanation:
Speakers often use certain phrases to indicate their stance, manage conversation, or
clarify what they are saying. For instance, saying "I mean" can signal a clarification or
correction, while "well" may indicate a pause or a shift in thought. These markers help
to create smoother conversations and provide subtle cues to the listener about how the
speaker wants their message to be interpreted.
Key Point:
Effective communication follows principles of cooperation, including being informative,
truthful, relevant, and clear, as outlined by philosopher Paul Grice.
Explanation:
Grice proposed four maxims that guide conversations:
If someone is asked about their sandwich and replies, "A sandwich is a sandwich," they
are implicitly indicating that they have no strong opinion about it, rather than just
stating an obvious fact.
Key Point:
Politeness strategies help speakers maintain social harmony by addressing others'
needs for independence (negative face) and inclusion (positive face).
Explanation:
"Face" refers to a person’s social self-image, which politeness strategies aim to protect.
Negative face respects an individual's autonomy (e.g., "I know you're busy, but..."), while
positive face emphasizes camaraderie and belonging (e.g., "Let’s do this together").
Different cultures prioritize these aspects differently, affecting communication styles.
Here's an expanded explanation of the key topics you mentioned from the provided text:
Deixis
Deixis refers to words and phrases that cannot be fully understood without contextual
information. Derived from the Greek word meaning "pointing," deixis is used to direct
attention to people, places, or times. Deictic expressions include:
● Person deixis: Words like "I," "you," "he," "she," and "they," which identify
specific individuals based on context.
● Spatial deixis: Words such as "here," "there," "this," and "that," pointing to
locations relative to the speaker or listener.
● Temporal deixis: Words like "now," "then," "yesterday," and "tomorrow,"
indicating points in time.
Deictic expressions rely entirely on context. For example, "You’ll have to bring it back
tomorrow because she isn’t here today" contains several deictic terms (e.g., "you," "it,"
"tomorrow") that make no sense without knowing who is speaking, where, or when.
Certainly! Deixis refers to expressions whose meaning relies on the context of the
utterance. Here’s a comprehensive list of the main types of deixis:
1. Person Deixis
● First person: I, we, me, us, my, our (speaker or group including the speaker).
● Second person: you, your (addressee or listener).
● Third person: he, she, it, they, his, her, their (others, not directly involved in
the conversation).
2. Spatial Deixis
● Examples: here, there, this, that, these, those, near, far, above, below, in front of,
behind.
● Here refers to the speaker’s location, while there refers to a place farther from the
speaker.
3. Temporal Deixis
● Examples: now, then, soon, later, today, tomorrow, yesterday, ago, last week,
next month.
4. Discourse Deixis
Refers to parts of the discourse itself (prior or upcoming parts of the conversation/text).
5. Social Deixis
Antecedents
Antecedents are linguistic elements (e.g., nouns or noun phrases) that introduce a
referent in a discourse, to which later expressions (anaphoric references) refer back.
For example:
The relationship between antecedent and anaphoric expressions is vital for maintaining
coherence in discourse. For example:
Occasionally, a cataphoric structure occurs, where the pronoun precedes the antecedent.
Example: "It appeared out of nowhere, a large black bear glaring in my direction."
Presupposition
Presupposition refers to assumptions speakers make about what the listener already
knows or believes. These are implied and persist even when the statement is negated, a
property known as constancy under negation.
● Examples:
○ "Your clock isn’t working" presupposes that you have a clock.
○ "Where did he find the money?" presupposes that he found money.
○ "We regret buying that car" presupposes that we bought a car.
● Test for presuppositions: Negating a statement (e.g., "Your clock is working")
still leaves the presupposition ("You have a clock") intact.
Presuppositions are critical for effective communication, as they shape the shared
knowledge that underpins interaction.
Speech acts refer to the actions performed through utterances, such as requesting,
promising, commanding, or questioning. John Searle categorized speech acts into five
types:
1. Assertives: Statements that describe the world or state facts (e.g., "It’s
raining.").
2. Directives: Attempts to get the listener to do something (e.g., "Close the door.").
3. Commissives: Commit the speaker to a future action (e.g., "I’ll call you
tomorrow.").
4. Expressives: Express emotions or attitudes (e.g., "I’m sorry.").
5. Declarations: Bring about a change in the world by the act of saying something
(e.g., "You’re fired.").
Speech acts can be direct (matching their literal meaning) or indirect (requiring
inference). For example:
The choice of strategy depends on the social dynamics, cultural norms, and the speaker’s
intent.
Speech Maxims
1. Quality: Be truthful; do not say what you believe to be false or lack evidence for.
2. Quantity: Provide the right amount of information—not too much, not too little.
3. Relation: Be relevant to the conversation.
4. Manner: Be clear, brief, and orderly; avoid ambiguity.
● Language Acquisition:
○ Most children acquire spoken language naturally, but deaf children of deaf
parents acquire sign language in a similar way.
○ American Sign Language (ASL) is widely used in the U.S. despite past
discouragement in educational institutions.
○ ASL was historically considered "mere gestures" rather than a real
language.
Expanded:
Children acquire their first language naturally based on their environment. While
spoken language is common, deaf children naturally learn sign language from their deaf
parents. ASL, which serves a large community in the U.S., was once dismissed in
educational settings, limiting its recognition as a legitimate language.
● Differences:
○ Sign language functions as a replacement for speech.
○ Gestures are used alongside speech to aid communication.
○ Examples of gestures include hand motions while explaining actions.
Expanded:
While gestures and sign language both involve hand movements, they serve different
purposes. Gestures typically accompany spoken words to provide additional meaning,
such as mimicking an action, while sign language is an independent communication
system with its own grammar and vocabulary.
Types of Gestures
1. Emblems:
○ Independent, conventional signals (e.g., thumbs up for approval).
○ Socially dependent and culturally specific.
Expanded:
Emblems are fixed gestures that convey meaning without speech, such as the "thumbs
up" gesture indicating positivity. However, their meanings vary across cultures, making
it essential to understand cultural differences.
2. Iconics:
○ Gestures that visually represent speech content.
○ Example: Tracing a square in the air while talking about a box.
Expanded:
Iconic gestures visually reinforce what is spoken, creating a direct link between the
gesture and the concept being discussed, such as mimicking an umbrella while talking
about rain.
3. Deictics:
○ Pointing gestures used for reference.
○ Example: Pointing to a cake while asking if someone wants some.
Expanded:
Deictic gestures help reference objects or spaces either present or remembered, making
communication more efficient and contextual.
4. Beats:
○ Rhythmic hand movements emphasizing speech elements.
○ Used for structuring narratives.
Expanded:
Beat gestures do not carry specific meaning but help emphasize certain parts of speech
or indicate a transition in storytelling.
Types of Sign Languages
Expanded:
Alternate sign languages serve temporary communication needs in settings where
speech is restricted, like silent religious vows or workplace signals.
Expanded:
Primary sign languages are fully developed languages with complex grammar and are
native to specific communities, differing significantly across regions.
● Oralism:
○ Promoted speech and lip-reading skills.
○ Had low success rates and was widely implemented until the 1960s.
Expanded:
Oralism was based on the belief that sign language hindered learning spoken language,
but it often failed to help deaf students acquire intelligible speech or lip-reading
abilities.
● Signed English:
○ Uses signs in English word order for easier learning.
○ Aimed to facilitate communication between deaf and hearing people.
Expanded:
Signed English was developed to bridge communication gaps, making it easier for
hearing parents and teachers to interact with deaf individuals by aligning with English
grammar.
Structure of ASL
Expanded:
Each ASL sign involves specific hand shapes, orientation, movement, and location,
contributing to its meaning. Misinterpretation of these elements can lead to
communication errors.
● Facial Expressions:
○ Essential for meaning in ASL.
○ Can indicate emotional tone or grammatical features.
Expanded:
Facial expressions play a crucial role in ASL by conveying emotion, emphasis, or
grammatical markers, making them integral to sign communication.
● Historical Background:
○ Influenced by French Sign Language (LSF).
○ Evolved in the U.S. through interactions between deaf communities.
Expanded:
ASL originated from French Sign Language brought to the U.S. by educators, blending
with local signing practices to form a unique linguistic system.
Expanded:
NSL serves as an example of how a sign language can naturally evolve when deaf
children are brought together, highlighting the innate human ability to create language.
● Linguistic Features:
○ ASL includes syntax, morphology, and phonology.
○ Comparable to spoken languages in structure.
Expanded:
ASL is a fully developed language with rules governing word formation and sentence
structure, challenging misconceptions that it is merely a collection of gestures.
Exam:
You need to choose one theory and provide arguments supporting it. Here are the major
theories discussed in the book:
○ The belief that language was given to humans by a divine being (e.g.,
biblical story of Adam naming animals).
○ Argument against: No scientific evidence, failed experiments (e.g.,
Psammetichus' experiment).
● The Natural Sound Source ("Bow-Wow" Theory)
○ Early humans imitated natural sounds (e.g., animal sounds like cuckoo).
○ Argument against: Many abstract words have no natural sound
connection.
● The Social Interaction Source ("Yo-He-Ho" Theory)
Example Answer:
"The Tool-Making Source is the most convincing theory because tool-making and
language both require advanced cognitive functions, such as planning and
coordination, which are controlled by the same areas of the brain. Studies of early
humans suggest that tool-making might have stimulated the development of
structured communication."
2. Displacement
Definition:
Example:
● Humans can discuss the past (What I did yesterday), the future (Next year, I’ll
travel), and even imaginary concepts (Unicorns are magical creatures).
Significance:
● This sets human language apart from animal communication, which typically
focuses on the here and now.
3. Informative Signals
Definition:
Examples:
5. Sentence Analysis
○ Noun (N), Verb (V), Adjective (Adj), Adverb (Adv), Pronoun (Pro),
Preposition (Prep), Conjunction (Conj), Article (Art).
2. Basic Sentence Analysis:
○ Agent (who performs the action), Theme (what is affected), Goal (where
action is directed).
6. Deictic Expressions
Definition:
● Words that rely on context to be understood (e.g., here, there, now, you).
Types:
● I wish you’d been there. (Person deixis: I, you; Place deixis: there)
Presupposition:
Presupposition:
Presupposition:
● Paris exists.
● Paris is the capital of France.
● Paris has a lot of museums.
Presupposition:
● John exists.
● John has a home.
● John drove somewhere last night.
Presupposition:
Definitions:
● Anaphora: Refers back to an earlier word (e.g., Laura went to the store. She
bought milk. She refers to Laura).
● Cataphora: Refers forward to something later in the sentence (e.g., When he
arrived, John was surprised – he refers to John).
Example Analysis:
● Laura went to see the new bookstore downtown. She had a lot of fun.
○ Antecedent: Laura
○ Anaphora: She
8. Presupposition
Definition:
● Boris Johnson was a Prime Minister of the UK. → (Presupposes Boris Johnson
held the position.)
● John drove home last night. → (Presupposes John has a home.)
Searle’s Classification:
Here’s the analysis of the speech acts based on whether they are direct or indirect,
along with their classification using Searle's taxonomy of speech acts:
● Direct Speech Act: The speaker explicitly commands the listener to give the
book.
● Searle's Classification: Directive (an attempt to get the listener to do
something).
Types:
Here’s the analysis of whether the speakers use positive or negative politeness in
the examples:
a. Move!
e. I love you!
Maxims:
Example Flouting: