wellisch2012
wellisch2012
Abstract
Gifted children who do not achieve often have problems with motivation and
socioemotional adjustment and may also have learning disabilities. This article
examines factors such as attachment difficulties and maternal depression as these
may contribute to underachievement. The article reviews past and current practices
of gifted identification and argues that schools have an important role in the early
identification of socioemotional problems and learning difficulties, as these can create
barriers to learning and achievement. Although Gagné did include underachievers
in his Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent, he has also argued that only
achieving children should be included in academic talent development programs, and
he supports a separate pathway for gifted underachievers. This article demonstrates
that such a pathway can be achieved through an inclusive model for gifted achievers and
underachievers. A model involves the early triaging of children through identification
of giftedness, socioemotional problems, and learning difficulties.
Keywords
gifted, model, intervention, underachievers, socioemotional adjustment.
Gagné (2011) argued in his recent lead article that only highly achieving children
should be included in academic talent development programs and that limiting eligi-
bility to high achievers would not discriminate against children from minority racial or
cultural backgrounds. Gagné’s lead article drew 40 commentaries. Balogh (2011), for
example, stated that Gagné’s program does not solve the problem of equity in talent
development and thought that the answer was to be found in an early and broadened
1
Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia
Corresponding Author:
Mimi Wellisch, 11 Marguerite Avenue, Mt Riverview, NSW 2774, Australia
Email: [email protected]
identification strategy. Dracup (2011) commented that the suggested program is use-
less in identifying those with abilities who have not yet achieved. Some other com-
mentaries identified that Gagné’s argument may also discriminate against gifted
underachievers where race and culture were not involved, a group of children who
could conceivably achieve, provided they received appropriate and timely support.
The term underachievers refers to children who have ability and yet do not achieve to
their potential, or, according to Gagné (1985), who are “gifted intellectually, but not
talented academically.” Giftedness is broadly defined as a genetically inherited poten-
tial or the ability to reach high levels of achievement in a variety of pursuits, preceded
by early characteristic signs (Howe, Davidson, & Sloboda, 1998).
Wellisch and Brown (2011) commented that gifted underachievers do not necessar-
ily hail from low socioeconomic or ethnic minorities but are nevertheless disadvan-
taged by learning or socioemotional problems, which may be a result of childhood
stress and trauma. They argued that Gagné had offered no pathway or model that
would enable talent development for these children, “despite evidence that emotion
and cognition are intertwined in human mental function (Adolphs, Tranel, & Damasio,
2003; LeDoux, 1996; Phelps, 2006; Vygotsky, 1987)” (p. 118). In his response to
Wellisch and Brown’s commentary, Gagné (2011) confirmed his position that gifted
underachievers do not fit into a talent development program and that another avenue
to address their special needs is required:
Except for rare cases where underachievement has its source in intense boredom
caused by the slow-paced regular curriculum, we cannot expect that gifted
underachievers will miraculously become high achievers when placed in an
ATD program . . . The solution seems to be, as Wellisch & Brown [italics in
original] suggest, the availability of ‘an alternative pathway for underachievers’
(p. 115) . . . gifted underachievers, whether or not they belong to minority or
low SES groups, need a special alternative pathway, distinct from the highly
challenging course offered in ATD programs. I will leave to experts the task of
engineering that pathway. (p. 145)
This article takes up Gagné’s invitation not only by outlining a unique pathway for
gifted underachievers but also by proposing an inclusive model of identification and
progression for all gifted children.
When Gagné (1985) first proposed his Differentiated Model of Giftedness and
Talent (DMGT), it was immediately recognized internationally for the inclusion of
underachievers, who were placed in the giftedness component of the DMGT. Although
Gagné has continued to update his model since that time (e.g., Gagné, 1995; 2004;
2009), there has been no attempt to add components that address the needs of this
subgroup of gifted children. Underachievers have remained stationary within the
DMGT, without pathways of possible progression, unable to move beyond being iden-
tified as naturally gifted. Underachievers are therefore destined to remain underserved
in schools that follow Gagné’s ideas, because, as Ford, Grantham, and Whiting (2008)
stated “(w)hen one makes giftedness synonymous with achievement, gifted under-
achievers will be neither recruited nor retained” (p. 300).
This article will first review the literature on underachievers and then examine
gifted children’s socioemotional adjustment and its association with attachment,
maternal depression, and motivation. Then we will take a brief look at past and current
procedures to identify giftedness. Finally, we will examine important additions to
identification procedures and the necessary interventions and planned progressions
within the proposed new model that together can make a positive difference to the
lives and prospects of underachievers.
Underachievement
Terman and Oden (1959) found two factors that divided high achievers from other
gifted participants in their study: drive to achieve (e.g., persistence and motivation)
and all-round socioemotional adjustment. However, a closer look at underachieve-
ment presents a complex set of causes (Dai, Moon, & Feldhusen, 1998). These include
social and economic influences (Freeman, 1992), race (Baker, 2011), culture
(Freeman, 2011; Sternberg, 2007), twice exceptionality (e.g., where a child is gifted
and has a disability; Silverman, 2009), lack of motivation due to socioemotional prob-
lems (Reis & Renzulli, 2004), lack of interest, and absence of educational challenge,
engagement and, support (Reis & Renzulli, 2009). Additional factors such as gender
have also influenced level of achievement (Gross, 1993). Thus, there is a wide range
of causes and factors influencing achievement.
Betts and Neihart (1988) suggested that there are six recognizable character types,
or profiles, of gifted children, including that of a potential school dropout, presenting
as angry, depressed, withdrawn, or acting out and being defensive. Betts and Neihart
also described the characteristics of a twice-exceptional child who may try to avoid
failures, who may be stubborn, impatient, disruptive, confused, stressed, frustrated,
who may feel discouraged, rejected, helpless, and isolated, and who may have sloppy
handwriting.
Lovett and Lewandowski (2006) challenged the “clinical lore” of a large, hidden
population of twice-exceptional children whose gifts and disabilities mask each other
and who could benefit uniquely from targeted interventions, at the same time stating
that they have no doubt of the existence of these children (p. 525). Nevertheless,
research findings such as Barnard-Brak, Johnsen, and Pond’s (2009) study can verify
the existence of these children where they are least expected. Barnard-Brak et al.
found evidence indicating that approximately 9% of a special education population
could have IQs in the 90th percentile, evidence that gifted children may well remain
unidentified in classrooms among chronologically similarly aged peers. Such waste of
potential to society has not been calculated, nor have the later costs, such as the reduced
level of happiness in at least some of these individuals (Seligman, 2002). As already
outlined, these and other underachievers may present with socioemotional adjustment
problems.
As has been outlined, maternal depression and attachment problems have been
linked with learning disabilities, socioemotional adjustment, and underachievement.
These gifted children are at risk of losing motivation, disengaging from the educative
process, and becoming early school leavers (Cloud, 2007). The loss of their potential
contribution to society has not been calculated. Cloud has called for a new model to
address the threat to the Nation’s precious, prodigious intellectual resource. This new
model should include early intervention for socioemotional problems (Beissner, 2008),
as the latter can affect motivation, an important factor in achievement.
Motivation
According to Reis and Renzulli (2009), there is no noncognitive trait more influential
on high levels of performance than effort or motivation. In fact, Renzulli (1978) con-
sidered high level of task commitment, associated with motivation, to be so signifi-
cant that he listed it as one of the three identifying factors in his 3-ring definition of
giftedness (the other two are high levels of creativity, and above average ability).
Another similar observation made by a number of scholars is that gifted children are
naturally motivated to influence their environment to ensure that their never-ending
need for information and challenges are met (Winner, 2000, Perry & Szalavitz, 2006,
Sternberg, 2005).
The experiences of trauma or great injustice have also been shown to be motivating
catalysts for some high achievers (Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1993;
Piechowski, 1997). However, ongoing family dysfunction and socioemotional prob-
lems are better known for their negative effects, interfering with motivation, planning,
attention, memory, and high achievement (Adelman & Taylor, 2000), resulting in
severely disrupting children’s learning and achievement (Reis & Renzulli, 2004).
Deficits in achievement motivation (or achievement-related thoughts, feelings, and
actions), according to a review by Dai et al. (1998), are thought to be associated with
a variety of issues, including unrealistic self-expectations, harsh self-criticism, low
self-confidence, a tendency to harbor self-defeating beliefs, and a lack in integration
of goals and personal standards. These self-defeating attributes and attitudes appear to
be related to poor socioemotional adjustment.
The three key factors in achievement for gifted children, then, can be summarized
as good socioemotional adjustment (Terman & Oden, 1959), positive achievement
motivation (Dai et al., 1998), and high ability (Renzulli, 1978), three essential ingredi-
ents to becoming an accomplished intellectually gifted person. Therefore, gifted chil-
dren who are considered to have high ability and fail to achieve (underachievers) can
be expected to struggle with adjustment and motivation and an eventual loss of ability.
Children who are gifted and underachieve as a result of learning disabilities typically
have high abilities as well as disabilities. To help these children progress, we need to
know which of the key achievement factors—adjustment, motivation, and/or ability—
need addressing, and these answers can be found through assessment.
Full Scale IQ in gifted children from a mean of 128.7 for the WISC–Third Edition
(WISC-III) validity study to IQ 123.5 for the WISC-IV gifted sample (Flanagan, &
Kaufman, 2004). Therefore, if the WISC-IV IQ test is used to identify gifted children,
consideration should be given to include children with a Full Scale IQ > 125 in gifted
programs.
It has long been argued that identification through the use of IQ tests alone fails to
identify many gifted children, for example, when they have nonacademic abilities
(Delisle, 2003; Winner, 2000). It has, therefore, been generally agreed that a diverse
range of identification strategies are likely to better capture a more equitable proportion
of gifted children (Merrick & Targett, 2004; VanTassel-Baska, 2000). Although this
strategy is an improvement on the onetime almost exclusive use of IQ tests for identifi-
cation, there is still no guarantee that underachieving gifted children are more easily
identified through the expanded use of methods such as class grades, or teacher, parent,
self-, and peer nominations. There is also general consensus that these children require,
along with twice-exceptional children, additional effort and careful selection of assess-
ment tools to ensure that a true measure of their abilities is obtained. The identification
process has, therefore, been expanded to include objective alternative achievement mea-
sures and subjective information gathered from peers, parents, and teachers (Merrick &
Targett, 2004). Such information may now also include portfolios of children’s work or
creative endeavors, achievement tests, and classroom work samples.
In the meantime, educators/administrators have suffered a loss of faith in IQ tests,
and there has been a trend in Australia to replace them with brief and less rigorous
screening tools (Merrick & Targett, 2004), such as rating scales and interviews, often
assessed by nonpsychologists, resulting in a lack of objective evidence of giftedness.
For example, in a recent personal comment to the first author, an employee of the
Catholic Education Office in Sydney advised that IQ tests were not in use to identify
gifted children within the Catholic Education system. When asked how gifted children
were then identified, the first author was told that teachers were very experienced and
could recognize a gifted child among other children. In contrast to such conviction,
research indicates that teachers continue to be quite poor identifiers of gifted children
(Moon & Brighton, 2008; Neumeister, Adams, Pierce, Cassady, & Dixon, 2007).
Lovett and Lewandowski (2006), who reviewed previous and current modes of
identification, commented that
[A]t the present time, IQ tests and comparable batteries of cognitive abilities
that yield general ability indices appear to be the most acceptable primary mea-
sures of giftedness, even though access to a gifted support program may be
based on a comprehensive evaluation integrating multiple sources of informa-
tion. (p. 524)
Lovett and Lewandowski also added that they did not believe such evaluation
should include imprecise measures such as creativity tests, isolated high scores on
specific subtests, or peer nomination. The reduced use of IQ tests and the failure to
replace them with equally objective and reliable measures have increased the likeli-
hood of underachievers remaining unidentified and increased the risk of gifted chil-
dren dropping out of school (Ford et al., 2008; Renzulli & Park, 2002).
As has been outlined, identification can be difficult for a variety of reasons. An
addition to these is the possible effect of early attachment problems associated with
maternal depression on gifted children’s motivation and behaviors (Murray et al.,
1999; Prior & Glaser, 2006; Wellisch, 2010). Recent findings indicate that gifted chil-
dren are more likely to be securely attached than other children (Wellisch, Brown,
Taylor, et al., 2011); however, problems associated with maternal depression and inse-
cure attachment such as learning difficulties and socioemotional problems can never-
theless be present (Wellisch, Brown, & Knight, 2011) and can, therefore, obscure
giftedness and contribute to the difficulty in the identification of giftedness.
As stated earlier, underachievers can be found in any group of children and can
include children from a minority or low socioeconomic group, including “invisible
gifted children,” who may show no signs of giftedness (Merrotsy, 2008). These under-
achievers may also present with an unusual ability profile, for example, very able in
some skills, with deficits in important other abilities. They may exhibit socioemotional
adjustment problems, attention problems, and may lack in adequate achievement-
related thoughts, feelings, and actions. As gifted underachievers can be difficult to
recognize in a group of children, all children should ideally be assessed for adjustment
and ability at the point of school entry. Assessment of all children would establish a
baseline reference point for teachers who can then measure subsequent early learning
gains and enable early identification of giftedness, learning disabilities, and socioemo-
tional problems and disorders. This early intervention strategy would provide solid
information and ensure evidence-based and appropriate educational placements, pro-
visions, and interventions.
Identification of the gifted and learning disabled (GLD). Identification of children who
are gifted and have learning disabilities, according to Lovett and Lewandowski (2006),
should be based on absolute low achievement in an academic skill-area subject, for
example, in the bottom 10% of the distribution of the particular skill, “(w)hen a stu-
dent has an IQ score in the gifted range (i.e., a standard score above 130) and signifi-
cantly below average achievement (i.e., a standard score below 85)” (p. 524). The
importance of this strategy is to demonstrate that the child has substantial abilities and
disabilities above and below the average when compared with peers of the same age.
The Neale Analysis of Reading Ability (Neale, 1999) and the Wide Range
Achievement Test 4 (Wilkinson & Robertson, 2006) can be used to identify academic
abilities and learning disabilities. The addition of these assessments would help iden-
tify and place children who need to be accelerated, especially those who can already
read well at school entry, and identify children who have a potential to achieve but lack
the enabling skills. Rogers (2011) has also used the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of
Cognitive Abilities to identify specific learning disabilities (Woodcock, Mather, &
McGrew, 2001) to identify these children.
Assessing other disabilities. Some gifted children meet the criteria for Aspergers dis-
order. Rogers (2011) has successfully used the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADIR-R;
Le Couteur, Lord, & Rutter, 2003) and BASC-2 (Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004) to
identify children with autism spectrum disorders. Finally, either the CBCL (Achen-
bach & Rescorla, 2001) or the Conners Rating Scales (Conners, 1997) can be used to
assess attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
IQ testing for intellectual giftedness. IQ tests such as the Wechsler tests or the Stan-
ford–Binet 5 (Roid, 2003) should be used to identify giftedness. In addition to assess-
ing a child’s IQ, the WISC-IV (Wechsler, 2003) has also been useful in correctly
identifying twice-exceptional children who were all found to have a large discrepancy
between index scores (Rogers, 2011). For the culturally diverse, Ford et al. (2008)
suggested the use of nonverbal tests of intelligence that are less culturally loaded than
traditional tests, such as the Naglieri Nonverbal Ability Test (NNAT; Naglieri, 1997),
Universal Nonverbal Intelligence Test (Bracken & McCallum, 1998), and Raven’s
Progressive Matrices (Raven, Raven, & Court, 2003). The successful use of a spe-
cially designed process of dynamic assessment has also been demonstrated, for exam-
ple, with the Coolabah Dynamic Assessment Method (CDAM), which creates a
nonthreatening and culturally unbiased identification process that seeks to optimize
cognitive performance (Chaffey, Bailey, & Vine, 2003). In summary, a range of objec-
tive IQ and other ability measures should be used, selected on the basis of the needs of
children.
Identification as Prevention
The researchers who interviewed mothers of gifted children mentioned earlier
(Wellisch, Brown, & Knight, 2011) found that peer misunderstandings involved the
gifted child being perceived as different from other children and could, in some cases,
difference in whether a child will continue to develop his or her giftedness as these
may vary according to individual differences, contexts, and environments. However,
the opportunity must be made available. Currently, as acknowledged by Gagné
(2011), there is no pathway between the potentially gifted underachiever and entry
into talent development. The proposed inclusive model aims to bridge the gap
between initial giftedness and its development. It proposes the strengthening of gifted
identification, the establishment of gifted early intervention, monitoring, and further
progression. These measures relate directly to children’s socioemotional status, moti-
vation, skills, and abilities so that gifted children can be guided onto a supportive
pathway toward their optimum development.
Initial Screening
Characteristics of giftedness. The characteristics of gifted children may be the first
sign of giftedness. These characteristics can differ from child to child. However, Fra-
sier and Passow (1994) identified the following 10 core gifted characteristics in chil-
dren from diverse backgrounds, although a gifted child may not exhibit all of these (all
italics in this section have been added for emphasis): motivation, intense unusual
interest, highly expressive communication skill, effective problem solving ability,
excellent memory, inquiry (curiosity), quick grasp or insight, uses logic and reason-
ing, imagination or creativity, and ability to convey and pick up humor. These charac-
teristics are similar to characteristics listed for highly and profoundly gifted children
(Rogers & Silverman, 1997; Appendix). High sensitivity has also been listed as an
early sign of giftedness (Rogers & Silverman, 1997; Silverman, 1993, 1998). Finally,
Clark (1992) also alerts us to a particular sign of precocity: “We have long known that
gifted children read early” (italics added; p. 99).
Gifted underachiever characteristics. A Senate inquiry into the education of gifted
children received submissions and gathered information on a variety of issues, includ-
ing the characteristics of underachievers (Commonwealth of Australia, 2000). The
characteristics included psychosomatic and psychological symptoms, such as stom-
achaches, headaches, depression, mental confusion, self-harm, poor self-esteem, sleep
disorders, nightmares, eczema caused by stress, and behaviors associated with inse-
cure attachment and ADHD.
Wellisch and Brown’s (2012) analysis of mothers’ narratives from an earlier study
identified that gifted children with borderline or clinical internalizing scores on the
CBCL were more likely to be easily frustrated, to be introverted, to have perfectionis-
tic tendencies, and the boys were less likely to be interested in sport than their peers.
These children were also more likely to have a learning disability.
Together, the mix of gifted and underachiever characteristics can provide the first
signs of giftedness in children. Gifted children are a heterogeneous group. For
example, one child may have most of the characteristics associated with giftedness,
whereas another may not share her characteristics of excellent memory and motiva-
tion, and instead exhibit underachiever characteristics associated with ADHD, such
After Identification
The model suggested here includes pathways for gifted children who have socio-
emotional problems, children who have learning or other disorders, and children
with a combination of these (Figure 1). Once giftedness is established, the adjust-
ment and disorder assessments and checklists can act in a triaging capacity to fur-
ther identify gifted children who have adjustment issues and are in need of
therapeutic support or social-skills training, and those children who are in need of
educational remediation.
possibly also with characteristics associated with underachievement, for example, the
socioemotional and other problems mentioned earlier. Following this initial observa-
tion, children should then undergo ability assessments and assessment for twice
exceptionality.
Twice-Exceptional Children
Planned interventions should be documented for children who have been identified as
gifted with either disorders or learning difficulties. Individual education and therapeutic
plans should be tailored to each child, taking into account frequency and level of severity
of problems and disorders. The plans should be regularly reassessed, adjusted as necessary,
and strategies reconsidered if outcomes fail to meet expectations within the planned time
frame.
Parents should be invited to take part in the process and be informed regularly on
the progress of interventions. They should also have access to information and, where
available, access to parenting programs for gifted children with socioemotional prob-
lems. Although there are currently no evidence-based parenting programs for behav-
ioral issues in gifted children, a promising program for parents of gifted children has
been piloted in conjunction with the evidence-based Triple-P Parenting Program
(Morawska & Sanders, 2009). Rogers (2011) has also reported the inclusion of parent-
ing programs in her current longitudinal study. The Triple-P Parenting intervention has
demonstrated over 40% improvement in child behavior over the short term and 30%
over the long term (Sanders et al., 2004).
The “invisible gifted children,” and other underachievers who may have gaps in
their knowledge, low self-esteem, and poor planning skills, should be grouped together
at least initially, or until they are able to cope in a more competitive context (Bailey,
2004). There should be an initial emphasis on the child’s strengths, and eventually a
move toward addressing weaknesses, and educational strategies should include access
to gifted program provisions through alternative learning styles (Rogers, 2011).
Educational provisions. Children who have only socioemotional problems and no
learning disabilities should be considered for extension, subject or whole-year accel-
eration, and mentoring. This strategy may not only address their educational needs but
also their socioemotional problems when these are related to frustration with the edu-
cational process, or when social problems have arisen due to the gap between their
advanced mental abilities and the average mental abilities of same-aged peers. As
most children with socioemotional problems have some difficulties relating to others,
social-skills training should be part of any intervention strategy.
Children who have learning disabilities and socioemotional problems, however,
should not be considered for whole-year acceleration until their problems have been
adequately addressed. Their strengths should nevertheless be supported, for example,
through extension programs and subject acceleration. Such educational strategies can
help to ensure that children’s self-esteem is maintained and that they remain educa-
tionally challenged and engaged while their problems are being addressed.
Remedial educational intervention. Children who are gifted, well adjusted, and have a
learning disorder can participate in gifted education programs as long as their disabil-
ity is in an unrelated area, the disability can be simultaneously addressed, and the
program does not place undue pressure to perform beyond their capability. If, for
example, the learning disability is in mathematical concepts, there is no reason that
subject acceleration should not be considered in the child’s strong English ability,
along with coaching to help fill gaps in learning as a result of the acceleration, with
simultaneous remedial support provided for the learning disability.
Children with internalizing or externalizing problems and disorders. Children with con-
duct problems, anxiety, or depression should be referred for therapeutic intervention.
CBT has been shown to reduce anxiety and depression and can address a negative
thinking style that leads to low self-esteem, behavior problems, and poor motivation.
Conduct problems require cooperation between school and parents, and parenting pro-
grams that address behavior management should be accessible to parents of these
gifted children. Family counseling may also be needed, especially when siblings
become affected, and social-skills training should be offered to the children while they
simultaneously access gifted programs considered appropriate for their interests and
level of motivation, and abilities.
Conclusion
This article has outlined how gifted children who have learning and other disabilities
and/or socioemotional problems can participate in gifted programs. The proposed
Inclusive Identification and Progression Model sets out a pathway that can support
children’s giftedness as well as address their problems. Availability of remedial edu-
cational and therapeutic strategies in schools can ensure the protection of gifted chil-
dren’s self-esteem and help maintain educational challenge and engagement while
their problems are being addressed.
Appendix.
Trait, aptitude or behavior How it may look Roger’s researched list
Reasoning Ability to make generalizations and use 99.3% reason well
Logical approaches to metaphors and analogies; can thing
figuring out solutions. things through in a logical manner;
critical thinker; ability to think things
through and come up with a plausible
answer.
Imagination/creativity Shows exceptional ingenuity in 93.4% have a vivid
Produces many ideas; highly using everyday materials; is keenly imagination
original. observant; has wild, seemingly silly
ideas; fluent, flexible producer of
ideas; highly curious.
Humor Keen sense of humor that may be 95.9% have an
Conveys and picks up on gentle or hostile; large accumulation excellent sense of
humor well. of information about emotions; humor
capacity for seeing the (the, ed.)
unusual; uncommon emotional depth;
openness to experiences; sensory
awareness.
Acknowledgment
We wish to thank Viggo Knackstredt for suggestions and assistance in relation to the design of
the model and the tabulation for the table for this article.
Funding
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
article.
References
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & pro-
files. Burlington: University of Vermont.
Adelman, H. S., & Taylor, L. (2000). Moving prevention from the fringes into the fabric of
school improvement. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation, 11, 7-36.
Amend, E. R., & Beljan, P. (2009). The antecedents of when normal behaviors of gifted children
are misinterpreted as pathological. Gifted Education International, 25, 131-143.
Bailey, S. (2004). Gifted and talented education: Professional development package for teach-
ers module 6. Sydney, Australia: University of New South Wales.
Baker, J. (2011). Stability of racial differences in gifted education: The case for stereotype
threat. Talent Development and Excellence, 3, 27-28.
Balogh, L. (2011).Theory and practice in one conception. Talent Development and Excellence,
3, 29-32.
Barnard-Brak, L., Johnsen, S. K., & Pond, A. (2009). The incidence of potentially gifted stu-
dents within a special education population. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Beissner, S. R. (2008). Unintended consequences of no child left behind mandates on gifted
students. Forum on Public Policy, 2. Retrieved from http://forumonpublicpolicy.com/sum-
mer08papers/archivesummer08/beisser.pdf
Betts, G. T., & Neihart, M. (1988). Profiles of the gifted and talented. Gifted Child Quarterly,
32, 248-253.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment (Vol. 1). London, England: Hogarth Press
& Institute of Psycho-Analysis.
Bracken, B. A., & McCallum, R. S. (1998). Universal nonverbal intelligence test. Itasca, IL:
Riverside Publishing.
Chaffey, G. W., Bailey, S. B., & Vine, K. W. (2003). Identifying high academic potential in Aus-
tralian aboriginal children using dynamic testing. Australasian Journal of Gifted Education,
12(1), 42-55.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (1998). Maternal depressive disorder and contextual
risk: Contributions to the development of attachment insecurity and behavior problems in
toddlerhood. Developmental Psychopathology, 10, 283-300.
Clark, B. (1992). Growing up gifted (4th ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Clark, B. (2008). Growing up gifted: Developing the potential of children at home and at school
(7th ed.). Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Cloud, J. (2007, August 16). Are we failing our geniuses? Time, 170(9). Retrieved from http://
www.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171,1653653,00.html
Colangelo, N., Assouline, S. G., Marron, M. A., Castellano, J. A., Clinkenbears, P. R., Rogers,
K., & Smith, D. (2010). Guidelines for developing an academic acceleration policy. Journal
of Advanced Academics, 21, 180-203.
Commonwealth of Australia. (2000). Senate employment, workplace relations, small business
and education references committee terms of reference: The education of gifted children.
Canberra: Parliament of Australia.
Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners’ Rating Scales-revised. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health
Systems.
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. S. (1993, January 25-27). Family influences on
the development of giftedness. In G. R. Rock (Ed.). The origins and development of high
ability (pp. 187-206). Symposium conducted at the meeting of Ciba Foundation, London,
England.
Dai, D. Y., Moon, S. M., & Feldhusen, J. F. (1998). Achievement motivation and gifted students:
A social cognitive perspective. Educational Psychologist, 55(2/3), 45-63.
Delisle, J. (2003). To be or to do: Is a gifted child born or developed? Roeper Review, 26, 12-13.
Dracup, T. (2011). An alternative English perspective on equity in gifted education. Talent
Development and Excellence, 3, 45-50.
Eddles-Hirsch, K., Vialle, W., Rogers, K. B., & McCormick, J. (2010). Just challenge those
high-ability learners and they’ll be all right! Journal of Advanced Academics, 22, 106-128.
Ericsson, K. A., Prietula, M. J., & Cokely, E. T. (2007). The making of an expert. Harvard Busi-
ness Review, 85(11), 115-121.
Flanagan, D. P., & Kaufman, A. S. (2004). Essentials of WISC-IV assessment. Hoboken, NJ:
John Wiley.
Ford, D. Y., Grantham, T. C., & Whiting, G. W. (2008). Culturally and linguistically diverse
students in gifted education: Recruitment and retention issues. Exceptional Children, 74,
289-306.
Fraser, D. (2010). Creativity, mood disorders and the aesthetic. Gifted Education International,
27, 84-96.
Frasier, M. M., & Passow, A. H. (1994). Toward a new paradigm for identifying talent potential
[Research Monograph 94112]. Storrs: University of Connecticut.
Freeman, J. (1992). Quality education: The development of competence. Geneva, Switzerland:
UNESCO.
Freeman, J. (2005). Permission to be gifted: How conceptions of giftedness can change lives.
In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (2nd ed., pp. 80-97).
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Freeman, J. (2011). A wish for the gifted and talented. Talent Development and Excellence, 3,
57-58.
Gagné, F. (1985). Giftedness and talent: Reexamining a reexamination of the definitions. Gifted
Child Quarterly, 29, 103-112.
Gagné, F. (1995). From giftedness to talent: A developmental model and its impact on the lan-
guage of the field. Roeper review, 18, 103-111.
Gagné, F. (2004). Transforming gifts into talents: The DMGT as a developmental theory. High
Ability Studies, 15, 119-147.
Gagné, F. (2009). Building gifts into talents: Brief overview of the DMGT 2.0. A day with
Professor Gagné—Working with Gagné’s Differentiated Model of Giftedness and Talent
(2008). Seminar, James Ruse Agricultural High School, Carlingford, 2 April.
Gagné, F. (2011). Academic talent development and the equity issue in gifted education. Talent
Development and Excellence, 3, 3-22.
Gross, M. U. M. (1993). Exceptionally gifted children. London, England: Routledge.
Hay, I., Elias, G., & Booker, B. (n.d.). Schooling issues digest-students with learning difficulties
in relation to literacy and numeracy (DEST). Retrieved from http://www.dest.gov.au/sec-
tors/school_education/publications_resources/schooling_issues_digest/schooling_issues_
digest_learning_difficulties.htm
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1998). Innate talents: Reality or myth.
Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 399-442.
Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-
R). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
Leschied, A. W., Chiodo, D., Whitehead, P. C., & Hurley, D. (2005). The relationship between
maternal depression and child outcomes in a child welfare sample: Implications for treat-
ment and policy. Child & Family Social Work, 10, 281-291.
Lovecky, D. V. (1995). Highly gifted children and peer relationships. Counseling and Guidance
Newsletter National Association for Gifted Children (NAGC), 5(3), 2, 6-7.
Lovett, B. J., & Lewandowski, L. J. (2006). Gifted students with learning disabilities: Who are
they? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 39, 515-527.
McMahon, C. A., Barnett, B., Kowalenko, N. M., & Tennant, C. C. (2006). Maternal attachment
state of mind moderates the impact of post natal depression on infant attachment. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 660-669.
Merrick, C., & Targett, R. (2004). Gifted and talented education: Professional development
package for teachers module 2. Sydney, Australia: University of New South Wales.
Merrotsy, P. (2008). The Wii Gaay project. Education Research Journal, 1, 1-19.
Moon, T. R., & Brighton, C. M. (2008). Primary teachers’ conceptions of giftedness. Journal for
the Education of the Gifted, 31, 447-480.
Morawska, A., & Sanders, M. (2009). An evaluation of a behavioural parenting intervention for
parents of gifted children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 463-470.
Morelock, M. J. (1992). Giftedness: The view from within. Understanding Our Gifted, 4(3),
11-15.
Munro, J. (2002). Understanding & identifying gifted learning disabled students. Australian
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 7, 20-30.
Murray, L., Sinclair, D., Cooper P., Ducournay, P., Turner, P., & Stein, A. (1999). The socio-
emotional development of 5-year-old children of postnatally depressed mothers. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 1259-1271.
Naglieri, J. A. (1997). Naglieri nonverbal ability test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora-
tion.
Neale, M. D. (1999). Neale analysis of reading ability (3rd ed.). Melbourne: Australian Council
for Educational Research Limited.
Neihart, M. (1999). The impact of giftedness on psychological well-being: What does the
empirical literature say? Roeper Review, 22, 10-17.
Neihart, M., Reis, S. M., Robinson, N. M., & Moon, S. M. (2002). Social and emotional issues
facing gifted and talented students: What have we learned and what should we do now? In
M. Neihart, S. M. Reis, N. M. Robinson, & S. M. Moon (Eds.), The social and emotional
development of gifted children. What do we know? (pp. 267-290). Washington, DC: Pru-
frock Press.
Neumeister, K. L. S., Adams, C. M., Pierce, R. L., Cassady, J. C., & Dixon, F. A. (2007). Fourth-
grade teachers’ perceptions of giftedness: Implications for identifying and servicing diverse
gifted students. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 30, 479-499.
NSW Department of Education and Training. (2007). Early entry to primary schools. Darling-
hurst, Australia: Author.
Perry, B. D., & Szalavitz, M. (2006). The boy who was raised as a dog: And other stories from a
child psychiatrist’s notebook: What traumatized children can teach us about loss, love, and
healing. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Peterson, J. S., & Ray, K. E. (2006). Bullying and the gifted: Victims, perpetrators, prevalence,
and affects. Gifted Child Quarterly, 50, 148-168.
Piechowski, M. M. (1997, August). Emotional giftedness: An expanded view. Paper presented at the
12th World Conference of the World Council for Gifted and Talented Children, Seattle, WA.
Prior, V., & Glaser, D. (2006). Understanding attachment and attachment disorder: Theory,
evidence and practice. London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2003, updated 2004). Manual for Raven’s Progressive
Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Reis, S. M., Burns, D. E., & Renzulli, J. S. (1992). Curriculum compacting: The complete
guide to modifying the regular curriculum for high ability students. Mansfield Center, CT:
Creative Learning Press.
Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and emotional develop-
ment of gifted and talented students: Good news and future possibilities. Psychology in the
Schools, 41, 119-130.
Reis, S. M., & Renzulli, J. S. (2009). Myth 1: The gifted and talented constitute one single
homogenous group and giftedness is a way of being that stays in the person over time and
experience. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53, 233-235.
Renzulli, J. S. (1978). What makes giftedness? Reexamining a definition. Phi Delta Kappan,
60(3), 180-184.
Renzulli, J. S., & Park, S. (2002). Giftedness and high school dropouts: Personal, family, and
school-related factors. Storrs: University of Connecticut.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). BASC-2: Behavior Assessment System for Chil-
dren (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Rogers, K. B. (2002). Re-forming gifted education: Matching the program to the child. Scotts-
dale, AZ: Great Potential Press.
Rogers, K. B. (2011). Thinking smart about twice exceptional learners: Steps for finding them
and strategies for catering to the appropriately. In C. Wormald & W. Vialle (Eds.), Dual
exceptionality (pp. 57-70). Wollongong, Australia: AAEGT.
Rogers, K. B., & Silverman, L. K. (1997, November). Personal, medical, social and psycho-
logical factors in 160 + IQ children. Paper presented at the National Association for Gifted
Children 44th Annual Convention, Little Rock, AK.
Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (5th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Sanders, M. R., Pidgeon, A. M., Gravestock, F., Connors, M. D., Brown, S., & Young, R. W.
(2004). Does parental attributional retraining and anger management enhance the effects
of the Triple P-Positive Parenting Program with parents at risk of child maltreatment?
Behavior Therapy, 35, 513-535.
Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Authentic happines: Using the new positive psychology to realize
your potential for lasting fulfilment. Sydney, Australia: Random House.
Silverman, L. K. (1993). The moral sensitivity of gifted children and the evolution of society.
Roeper Review, 17, 110-116.
Silverman, L. K. (1997). The construct of asynchronous development. Peabody Journal of Edu-
cation, 72, 36-58.
Silverman, L. K. (1998). Through the lens of giftedness. Roeper Review, 20, 204-211.
Silverman, L. K. (2009). The two-edged sword of compensation: How the gifted cope with
learning disabilities. Gifted Education International, 25, 115-130.
Sternberg, R. J. (2005). The theory of successful intelligence. Interamerican Journal of Psy-
chology, 39, 189-202.
Sternberg, R. J. (2007). Cultural dimensions of giftedness and talent. Roeper Review, 29, 160-
165.
Stoeger, H., & Ziegler, A. (2010). How fine motor skills influence the assessment of high abili-
ties and underachievement in math. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 34, 195-219.
Stoeger, H., Ziegler, A., & Martzog, P. (2008). Deficits in fine motor skill as an important factor
in the identification of gifted underachievers in primary school. Psychology Science Quar-
terly, 50, 134-146.
Terman, L. M. (1925). Genetic studies of genius. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University.
Terman, L. M., & Oden, M. H. (1959). Genetic studies of genius: The gifted group at mid-life.
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
VanTassel-Baska, J. (2000). The on-going dilemma of effective identification practices in gifted
education. Communicator, 31, 39-41.
VanTassel-Baska, J. (2005). Domain-specific giftedness. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson
(Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness (2nd ed., pp. 358-376). New York, NY: Cambridge Uni-
versity Press.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Har-
court Assessment.
Wellisch, M. (2010). Communicating love or fear: The role of attachment styles in pathways to
giftedness. Roeper Review, 32, 116-126.
Wellisch, M., & Brown, J. (2011). Where are the underachievers in the DMTG’s academic talent
development? Talent Development and Excellence, 3, 115-117.
Wellisch, M., & Brown, J. (2012). The many faces of a gifted personality: Characteristics along
a complex gifted spectrum. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wellisch, M., Brown, J., & Knight, R. (2011). Gifted and misunderstood: Mothers’ narratives
of their children’s socio-emotional adjustment and educational challenges. Manuscript sub-
mitted for publication.
Wellisch, M., Brown, J., Taylor, A., Knight, R., Berresford, L., Campbell, L., & Cohen, A.
(2011). Secure attachment style and high IQ: Is attachment a protective factor? Australasian
Journal of Gifted Education, 20(2), 23-33.
Wilkinson, G. S., & Robertson, G. J. (2006). Wide Range Achievement Test 4 professional man-
ual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources
Winner, E. (2000). The origins and ends of giftedness. American Psychologist, 55, 159-169.
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., Mather, N., & Schrank, F. A. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III
tests of cognitive abilities examiner’s manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Jac Brown is a senior lecturer teaching in the area of clinical psychology at Macquarie
University, Australia.