Managing Pet Owners’ Guilt and Grief in Veterinary Euthanasia Encounters
Managing Pet Owners’ Guilt and Grief in Veterinary Euthanasia Encounters
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
Encounters
Patricia Morris1
Abstract
Through examining the emotion-laden encounters between veterinarians
and bereaved pet owners, this study focuses attention on a group of medical
professionals who manage the emotions of their clients in light of opposing
contextual goals. While negotiating possible outcomes for animal patients,
veterinary emotion work is designed to assuage guilt and grief to facili-
tate timely and rational decisions. However, after clients make the difficult
decision to euthanize their pet, veterinary emotion work is geared toward
creating “safe” emotional space for grieving clients. This study illustrates that
veterinarians have a growing commitment to comforting the owners of eutha-
nized animals and to validating their feelings of grief, pain, and sorrow. On a
broader, theoretical level, this study also applies and extends concepts developed
in previous sociological analyses of emotion management and human–animal
relationships.
Keywords
emotion management in the workplace, euthanasia, socioemotional economy,
sociozoologic scale, human-animal relationships, veterinary medicine
1
Drury University, Springfield, MO, USA
Corresponding Author:
Patricia Morris, Drury University, Behavioral Science Department, 900 North Benton Avenue,
Springfield, MO 65802
Email: [email protected]
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
338 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Introduction
Euthanasia is different for today’s vets. Today people have varying relation-
ships with their pets, but very often they will see their pets as members of their
family. . . . This is kind of a new paradigm with regard to euthanasia. The old
way of doing things was when a person wanted to euthanize their dog, you
would take it in the back and it gets euthanized. . . . The client was really not
involved in the process at all. We are really getting away from that, but there
are still veterinarians [who] do that. There are still some veterinarians where it
is a business thing. You go up front and pay the money and the dog goes in the
back and gets euthanized. It is not warm at all. Most people now, however,
want to be more intimately involved with euthanasia. They want it to be a nice
experience.
—Forty-four-year-old male, veterinary professor discusses
euthanasia in a lecture to his seniors
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 339
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
340 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 341
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
342 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 343
rapport known to facilitate authentic data collection (Warren and Karner 2005).
First, I spent a great deal of time with participants, and we shared both the frustra-
tion of difficult cases and the joy of heroic life-saving events. Observing as many
cases as possible on day, evening, and overnight shifts helped me to build rapport
and established my genuine interest in their work. While no employee refused to
allow me to observe them at work, I was unable to schedule formal interviews
with every veterinarian employed at the hospitals because of time constraints.
Given my participants’ demanding schedules, a few of the interviews took place
in break rooms or hallways during slow shifts. In most cases, however, the inter-
views took place while participants were off duty in isolated areas of the hospital,
such as the library or private office. I conducted the majority of interviews for
approximately one hour but some lasted for as long as three hours.
In addition to spending many hours with participants, I built rapport by shar-
ing in the daily “grind” of the profession (Arluke and Sanders 1996). My duties
included cleaning exam tables, bathing animals, monitoring equipment, deliv-
ering charts across the hospital, and assisting in noncritical procedures such as
restraining patients or taking their temperatures. In fact, I once sustained a
semiserious injury while helping to control a very agitated feline patient.
Admittedly, I wore short sleeves proudly for a week after that incident with
hopes that others would notice my wounds and interpret the injury as a sign of
my commitment to the project. Over time, I became especially good at an
unpopular task known as “expressing” anal glands. Without going into much
detail, suffice it to say the oily secretions one must gently coax out of the swol-
len gland have a very disagreeable odor to humans. Thankfully, the sometimes
painful and pungent tasks helped build rapport with participants and facilitated
discussion of emotionally uncomfortable work-related experiences.
Humor also greatly aided my transition from “outsider” status to accep-
tance in the private workspace of veterinarians. Participants publically ana-
lyzed my character through the telling and retelling of two particularly
embarrassing events that happened to me early in my fieldwork. The first
story involved a horrific accident that occurred while transporting a large,
wheeled container packed full of animal cadavers to the crematorium. When
tipping back the wheeled container, I lost my balance and slipped, causing the
bodies to spill out on top of me. Told with increasingly exaggerated detail and
humorous glee by many participants, this story, which recounted lifeless ani-
mals falling on my face, always elicited a hearty laugh. The second comedic
event involved a devilishly planned practical joke in which I was asked to
feel inside the rectum of a Bullmastiff for foreign objects. This dog had ren-
dered himself unable to defecate by consuming an unsupervised birthday
cake which had been artfully decorated with pieces from a Civil War chess
set. As I stuck my fingers into his rectum, I managed to grab a missing rook
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
344 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
which helped the patient release a sudden gush of black diarrhea all over my
face and clothes. Anticipating this explosion, the entire clinic erupted in
laughter and cheers.
My willingness to laugh at these emotionally troubling and embarrassing
situations reinforced my reputation as someone who can withstand the diffi-
cult parts of the job and who can be trusted with sensitive “insider” informa-
tion. Similar to Sanders’s (2004) experience as a researcher among sex
workers, in every setting I became the source of jokes, gags, and funny sto-
ries aimed at testing my willingness to “play along.” For example, partici-
pants often playfully implied that I was macabre or morbid because of my
research interest in euthanasia. Interns particularly enjoyed making me the
target of humor by asking me technical questions in front of clients so that
they could later take mischievous pleasure laughing at my awkward and
unscientific responses. Although such teasing may seem alienating, it made
me feel welcome. Workplace researchers repeatedly note humor as a key part
of socializing newcomers, relieving tension, and building a sense of group
camaraderie (Francis 1994). As such, participating in playful repartee and
good-humored teasing not only reinforced my acceptance as an insider but
also helped participants become more comfortable with me (and I with them).
Eventually, participants came to see me as a confidant, and I became
increasingly certain that I could trust their descriptions and see their actions
as genuine. As time passed, participants brought up sensitive matters without
my prodding and sometimes went out of their way to find me in the hospital
to share stories they thought relevant to the project. For example, after notic-
ing my interest in the euthanasia-related cards, letters, and gifts displayed
around their offices, participants often saved them to share with me during
our interviews. Although I did not do an official content analysis of these
cards and letters, I transcribed some of the key content in my fieldnotes.
While my fieldwork was often emotionally upsetting and physically exhaust-
ing, my use of ethnographic methods gave me direct access to the private and
emotionally charged interactions between veterinarians and their clients.
Results
Pet Owners’ Emotions: Guilt and Grief
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 345
transportation and farm animals (Bryant and Snizek 1976). Early pet owners
assumed the duties of caring for their pets’ health and well-being and, when
it came to ending the lives of suffering or unwanted animals, they “put them
down” at home. Over time, however, pet owners began to regard home meth-
ods such as shooting or drowning as inconvenient, distasteful and, in some
cases, cruel or inhumane (Grier 2006). Concerned with how best to alleviate
(or simply evaluate) their animals’ suffering, pet owners progressively turned
to veterinarians for assistance, not only in managing their pets’ health but
also in attending to their deaths.
As euthanasia became an increasingly common task for veterinarians, the
procedure typically took place in the backrooms of clinics, far from the view
of pet owners. As late as 1981, articles in veterinary journals strongly discour-
aged practitioners from allowing client access to the death of their pets (e.g.,
Bustad, Hines, and Leathers 1981). Apprehensive that grieving clients would
become an additional time-consuming burden, veterinarians also expressed
concern that witnessing the death of beloved companions might be emotion-
ally disturbing for clients. While some veterinarians continue to ban pet own-
ers from euthanasia procedures, many criticize such policy as old-fashioned
and inconsiderate of clients’ wishes and expectations. In fact, surveys confirm
that the majority of pet owners strongly believe veterinarians should allow
clients the option to remain with their animals during euthanasia procedures
(Adams, Bonnett, and Meek 2000; Martin et al. 2004). Thus, many veterinar-
ians today interact with emotionally distraught clients leading up to and after
they have made the difficult decision to end the life of their pet.
Unlike the typical “unemotional” veterinary consultation, euthanasia-
related conversation between the veterinarian and client are often marked by
at least some degree of emotional distress (Swabe 1994). Although I wit-
nessed some pet owners make seemingly callous decisions regarding the
death of their animals, I more often observed emotionally distraught owners
who request to hold their companion animals during the euthanasia and spend
time with their bodies after they die. This pattern held true not only for own-
ers of dogs and cats but also of birds, mice, ferrets, hamsters, and even an
iguana. Veterinarians classified pet owners’ troubling emotions into two main
categories: those associated with grief (e.g., sadness, distress) and those asso-
ciated with guilt (e.g., doubt, regret).
For many pet owners, the death of a companion animal can feel similar to
the loss of a human relationship (Gerwolls and Labott 1994). In fact, research-
ers have found similar grief reactions among people mourning the death of
animals and those experiencing a human loss (Carmack 1985; Quackenbush
1985). Wrobel and Dye’s (2003) study of adults whose pet recently died
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
346 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 347
difficult choice: “This man is suffering from serious grief at the thought of
losing his cat, but he also feels guilty because he let her get outside the house.
. . . You can’t waffle on something like this. . . . It is inhumane to leave this
animal the way it is, and it can be agonizing on us when they won’t make a
decision!” Participants often felt their clients’ emotions (particularly guilt and
grief) had the potential to impair their ability to make timely medical deci-
sions.
Circumstances in which guilt is not easily managed are often particularly
frustrating for the veterinarian as demonstrated by the case of Daisy, a Golden
Retriever, who swallowed a toothpick accidentally left inside her owner’s ham-
burger. When the toothpick perforated her gastrointestinal tract, she developed
a debilitating case of peritonitis. After Daisy’s first surgery to correct the dam-
age, she relapsed. Because of the severity of her condition, the intern assigned
to her case strongly recommended euthanasia. However, Daisy’s owner
responded, “I can’t kill her. It is my fault she ate the toothpick. I have to give
her another chance.” Unable to change his client’s mind, the intern reluctantly
agrees to another surgery. After three weeks of intensive care and two major
surgeries, the peritonitis returns. Despite a spiraling bill upwards of 10,000 dol-
lars and the surgeon’s prediction that Daisy would likely not survive another
surgery, the client wants to try again. At this point, a supervisor calls the intern
into her office to discuss the mounting bill and the owner’s refusal to euthanize.
The intern, clearly frustrated with the situation, loudly exclaims, “What do you
want me to do? Put a gun to his head? He just feels too damn guilty over that
fucking toothpick!” Thus, veterinarians fear a client’s guilt can prevent them
from making the right choice for the animal’s welfare.
In the decision-making stage, the veterinarian’s goals are typically instru-
mental; that is, he or she strives to help clients assuage problematic emotions
and make rational choices. Veterinarians tightly manage feelings of guilt by
strictly requiring clients to conform to specific emotional directives. In fact,
the veterinarians in this study relied on the same repertoire of emotion man-
agement strategies that DeCoster (1997) observed in his study of physician–
patient interaction. These strategies include reinterpretation, redirection, and
rationalization. Veterinarians reinterpreted their clients’ guilt in favor of more
constructive emotions. For instance, one participant encouraged her clients to
reinterpret guilt as love for the animal: “Lots of owners who choose euthana-
sia feel guilt and they doubt their decision, but that’s just a sign of your love
for Scratchers. It is not a sign that you are making a bad decision, but that you
care deeply for Scratcher’s best interest.”
After the client makes the difficult decision to euthanize his or her com-
panion animal, veterinarians continue to suppress their client’s feelings of
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
348 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
guilt. In fact, participants used the same strategies to neutralize guilt after
euthanasia they previously relied on to facilitate timely decisions. For exam-
ple, clients often felt guilty considering euthanasia for terminally ill pets out
of the fear they might make the decision “too soon.” To facilitate timely deci-
sions, veterinarians might redirect their client’s guilt toward the negative
emotions they would unquestionably feel in the future if they allowed the
animal to suffer needlessly. However, even after the death of the animal, vet-
erinarians continued to redirect their client’s expressions of guilt into concern
for the animal’s feelings by focusing on how euthanasia ended the pet’s suf-
fering: “We did everything we possibly could, and at least she is not suffering
anymore. No more chemo. No more needles. No more throwing up.”
Rationalizing was an especially helpful tactic in facilitating timely deci-
sions and alleviating guilt after euthanasia. In the decision-making phase,
veterinarians rationalize euthanasia as one justifiable outcome among other
equally legitimate alternatives. However, after the client makes a choice, the
veterinarian may immediately rationalize that option as the best choice.
Consider the case of a Rottweiler named Spike. At first, Spike’s veterinarian
validates both the option of continuing life-sustaining treatment and the
choice to euthanize: “Though Spike could certainly respond to the new treat-
ments, 12-years-old is really old for a Rottweiler. It is obvious that you took
really good care of him and gave him a great life. So, I know whatever deci-
sion you make, it’s the right one.” Yet, after the client makes the decision to
euthanize Spike, the veterinarian exclusively rationalizes his client’s choice:
“You did the right thing. . . . This was the right call in my opinion. Many
people make the mistake of waiting too late. A big problem we run into is
when people won’t make timely decisions and the animal suffers. It is obvi-
ous that you love Spike and you didn’t want him to suffer.”
Even in cases when euthanasia seemed irrefutably in the best interest of
the animal, simply thinking about ending the life of his or her pet can threaten
a client’s identity as a good and loving pet owner. In fact, anxious pet owners
expressed considerable relief when hearing their veterinarian corroborate
their decision to end their animal’s life. Thus, as medical experts in animal
health and well-being, veterinarians wield a great deal of authority in
defining the best interest of animals. The veterinarians I observed relied on
this authority to define euthanasia as a positive, medically appropriate choice,
thereby engaging in tight control of owners’ emotions. In fact, as the follow-
ing participant demonstrates, veterinarians even rationalized euthanasia when
they had hoped for a different outcome for their patient: “As long as you feel
like [euthanasia] is somewhat medically justifiable. . . I will transition from
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 349
After the euthanasia of a couple’s cat, the wife tearfully remarks, “You
must hate this part of your job?” The veterinarian thoughtfully
responds, “You know it is really a blessing to be able to end suffering
of animals and to help people say goodbye to their pets. I am fortunate
to be able to do this for my patients. We went through heart failure with
my granddad and it was a horrible way to go. Really it [euthanasia] can
be a special time for me because I get to see how much people love
their animals.”
The tight emotion work of veterinarians helped clients resolve their guilt and
reconstruct their identity as good pet owners who, by choosing euthanasia for
their animal, made a kind and loving decision.
In addition to the instrumental goals of “getting the job done” and shaping
euthanasia as a positive practice, some veterinarians sought to manipulate or
exploit clients’ feelings of guilt for financial, personal, and professional gain.
For example, a few participants subtly encouraged guilt in their clients with
the hope of persuading them to pay for expensive life-saving procedures.
Novice interns occasionally “played on the client’s guilt” in order to coax
them into treatments or procedures they were eager to learn. However, when
most participants claimed to “use the client’s emotions to my advantage,”
they were typically referring to the psychological benefit they feel when
advocating the best interest of their patient. For example, veterinarians may
enhance guilt emotions if they believe it will help encourage clients to make
the decision to euthanize a suffering patient. Although participants some-
times viewed “gently pushing clients towards the best interest of the animal”
as justifiable, most regarded preying on client emotions simply for profes-
sional or financial gain as unethical, unsavory, and unprofessional behavior.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
350 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
It is no good [for the patient] to try and sugar-coat it and be all touchy-
feely stepping around the truth for the owners. . . . Based on your
medical knowledge, if you think the animal is suffering you sometimes
have to use the word suffering and not sugar-coat it because you think
it might hurt some feelings.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 351
In light of a common belief that emotions are at odds with good decision
making, medicalizing is a tight emotion work strategy designed to encourage
an owner to see the veterinarian as an impartial and rational expert whose
advice is based on science and rationality rather than sentiment and personal
attachments.
Once a client chooses euthanasia, veterinarians shift their demeanor from
that of a detached medical professional to a sympathetic moderator of the
euthanasia experience. No longer concerned with the potential influence of
emotions on the outcome of negotiations, veterinarians acknowledge grief and
the fact that coming to such a decision is often an exasperating and agonizing
experience. Dr. Miller describes how she moves into “euthanasia mode”:
As Dr. Miller implies above, participants often had to suppress their own anger,
sadness, and disappointment in order to transition into euthanasia mode.
Although most considered patient advocacy an important part of their job, they
also saw it as their job to help owners deal with the death of their animal or, at
least, to “help them feel their grief is appreciated and appropriate.” No longer
concerned with negotiating the best possible outcome for patients, the veteri-
narian’s goal moves toward “providing a safe space to grieve for animals” and
toward “creating an atmosphere conducive to the expression of emotion.”
To achieve their new goals, veterinarians use a variety of loose emotion
management techniques outlined in Table 1. They express empathy and sym-
pathy, listen to owners, reassure them, offer a comforting touch on the arm or
stroke the animal’s fur, and, in some cases, even hug the owners. Not
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
352 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 353
Table 1. Strategies for Loosely Managing Client Grief After the Decision to
Euthanizea
Strategy Definition Example
Catharsis Coaxing the client to “If you need to talk about anything, I am here for you.
express/talk about felt . . . Would you like me to stay with you?”
emotions.
Empathizing Understanding/ “I lost my cat of nine years to the same kind of
identification with the cancer just last year. It was a terrible process. I
client’s experience by know exactly what you are feeling right now.”
the verbal proclamation “I think just being there makes them feel better to
of similar emotion(s). some extent. Even though I am really busy I try to
This strategy may let them talk. It seems like the right thing to do.
include listening There is only so much you can say to comfort a
attentively to a client’s person.You don’t really know them but you can
stories about their listen to them. That is a gift too.”
animals or whatever
they choose to talk
about.
Sympathizing Stating or expressing “It looks like this was very painful for you.You
an emotion for the obviously care deeply for Scratchers. It was a
owner (feeling for the terrible accident.”
owner). This strategy “I just try to look sympathetic and convey through
may include using “body body language that I’m sorry. Taking the time to give
language” or gestures them a Kleenex is a form of communication.You
to provide emotional are bonding with them through interaction without
support or convey words.”
sympathy.
Reassurance Talking to instill confidence “I can see that you really loved him and it is natural
in the owner (that their to cry and grieve when we experience loss. Lots
grief is “normal” and of people really grieve for their pets. They are
legitimate). important parts of our families.”
Redirecting Encouraging owners “I ask the owner have you had them since they were
to focus on positive a kitten or a puppy? And then I try to get them to
memories of their pet think of those moments—the good times when
rather than thinking they weren't sick.”
about their death.
Comforting Putting a hand on the “If you are not the kind of person who feels
touch owner’s body (shoulder comfortable touching the person, then make sure
or hand) or offering you touch or pet the animal in the process. . . to let
a hug. Touch may also the owners know that you care.”
include touching the
animal.
a. Several strategies (catharsis, empathize, sympathize, and reassure) mirror those operational-
ized by DeCoster’s (1997) study titled “Physician Treatment of Patient Emotions.”
When I graduated from vet school . . . one of the first things a vet told
me was, “You know Ray, there is only one thing that you need to do
well as a veterinarian.” I said, “What is that?” He said, “How to eutha-
nize an animal. . . . If you can euthanize an animal well, gracefully, and
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
354 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
with respect and compassion, when they [clients] go home and have
that visceral response, they will think, ‘My God. I really love my vet-
erinarian. He really understands how I feel.’ Even though it was a
negative experience or an extremely emotional experience, they will
feel that way. They absolutely will remember that compassion when
their future pets need services.”
While this research does not explore the pet owner’s perspective, many of the
veterinarians I studied strongly argued that “dealing with client emotions” is
essential to client satisfaction and building long-term relationships with cli-
ents. Although most participants denied the instrumental goal of profit as
their primary motivation for emotion work, they contended that emotion
work is a critical component to client satisfaction and, by extension, a veteri-
narian’s financial success.
During my research, it became clear that small-animal veterinarians
believe that veterinary service includes both maintaining the health and well-
being of animals and attending to the emotional needs of their clients. In
addition to engaging in the emotion work outlined above, the participants in
this study also provided literature on pet loss and recommend books to their
clients. They also worked in veterinary clinics that often offered referrals to
local therapists specializing in pet loss, and some even employ full-time
counselors to assist grieving pet owners (Dunn, Mehler, and Greenberg 2005;
Mercer 2007). Small-animal veterinary clinics commonly sell urns, caskets,
jewelry, and other products designed for memorializing deceased pets.
Although veterinary hospitals tend to make little profit on the urns and other
memorial items they sell, offering such acts of veneration suggests that ani-
mals are worthy of such honor, ceremony, and grief (and, by extension, worth
the cost of expensive medical care). Finally, veterinarians increasingly invest
in designer spaces for euthanasia known as meditation, comfort, or grieving
rooms with soothing wall colors, comfortable seating, and low lighting. Thus,
in addition to emotion work that allows for grief, veterinarians create literal
spaces for grief.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 355
participants typically received four to five times more cards or gifts from
clients after a euthanasia service compared to any other type of veterinary
consultation. Thus, the majority of cards, letters, photographs, gift certifi-
cates, gourmet food, flowers, gift baskets, and monetary donations to special
hospital funds come from clients after euthanasia procedures. While I wit-
nessed the desks of novice interns quickly fill with displays of euthanasia-
related gratitude, many of the more experienced participants estimate that
they have received several hundred such gifts over the course of their
careers. Although appreciative of their clients’ gestures, many veterinarians
feel baffled by the level of gratitude they receive for providing an “unsuc-
cessful” service that culminates in the death of the pet.
To make sense of the seemingly paradoxical response of pet owners, we
must briefly explore the larger cultural context surrounding human–animal
relationships. Researchers have long documented ambiguous societal atti-
tudes and ambivalent emotions toward nonhuman animals (Arluke and
Sanders 1996; Herzog 2010). For example, people train dogs to fight to their
death, race them for gambling purposes, leave them chained to fences in the
backyard, breed them in “puppy mills,” train them for dog shows, take them
on family vacations, buy them designer accessories, pamper them with spa
treatments, or leave them substantial inheritances. As medical providers to
nonhuman animals, veterinarians encounter clients with widely disparate
views and attitudes regarding animals in their charge. However, an animal’s
position along a continuum from valued subject (patients deserving of quality
medical care) to functional object (the client’s property) has clear implica-
tions for the kind of treatment it receives.
As subjects, companion animals provide valued emotional support to pet
owners, who consider them cherished friends or even family members (Albert
and Bulcroft 1988; Gosse and Barnes 1994; Katcher 1989). These animals
may receive hundreds or thousands of dollars worth of veterinary care. In fact,
according to a 2006 national survey on pet ownership, U.S. households spent
approximately 24.5 billion on veterinary care specifically for dogs and cats
(AVMA 2008a). Moreover, pet owners increasingly travel great distances to
bring their beloved companions to state-of-the-art veterinary facilities offering
the most sophisticated veterinary care available. For example, animals receive
advanced medical, dental, and surgical care, including dialysis, root canals,
hip replacements, chemotherapy, cataract extractions, and even pacemakers.
As objects, companion animals are legally their owners’ property. Some
animals are subjected to cosmetic and medically unnecessary services,
including the surgical amputation of their ears, tails, or claws for the benefit
of their owners. Some pet owners are unwilling to spend even minimal
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
356 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
financial resources on their animal’s health care because they see the animal
as easily replaceable. Frustrated pet owners request euthanasia when their
animal barks too loudly, scratches the furniture, digs holes in the yard, or
urinates outside the litterbox. Healthy, well-behaved companion animals can
be “put down” or euthanized at their owners’ request. For example, pet own-
ers request euthanasia because they have simply grown tired of the demands
of pet care, their new boy/girlfriend is allergic to the animal, or they are mov-
ing to a new residence that does not allow pets.
Recall the unexpected finding of this research that veterinarians are far
more likely to receive gratitude for euthanasia compared to times when they
cured animals or restored their health. This finding stands in sharp contrast to
the gratitude pattern encountered by Lois’s (2001) rescue workers. When res-
cue workers saved victims, family members lavished gratitude on the work-
ers. Yet, when death occurred, family members rarely offered gratitude for
the service rescue workers provided them. Lois (2001, 173) speculates that,
when the victim died, family members did not feel obligated to provide grati-
tude because “socioemotional norms dictated that the families’ emotional
grief and bad fortune far outweighed the emotional support the rescuers had
provided during the mission.” By comparison, why did clients send so many
more gestures of gratitude when their companion animals died compared to
when the veterinarian successfully “saved” the animal? Moreover, shouldn’t
the pet owner’s bad fortune—the loss of their pet—outweigh the emotional
support they were provided by the veterinarian?
Arluke and Sanders’s (1996) concept of a sociozoologic scale coupled with
Clark’s (1997) notion of the socioemotional economy help to answer these ques-
tions. Because of our different positions on the sociozoologic scale, grief over
the death of any companion animal is rendered less socially legitimate com-
pared to the grief over the loss of a human. In light of the ambiguous societal
attitudes regarding the value of animal life discussed above, pet owners certainly
encounter others who simply do not understand their intense feelings of grief
over the death of a companion animal. Some people consider grieving for ani-
mals silly or overly sentimental and respond to the loss with an insensitive
remark such as, “It’s only a cat. What’s the big deal?” Friends and family may
want to comfort loved ones after the death of a beloved animal, but their actions
demonstrate that they may not fully understand or appreciate the loss. For exam-
ple, although well-intended, telling a pet owner “You might feel better if you just
go get another dog” can evoke the same reaction as telling a grieving widower
“Don’t worry, you can easily get a new wife.”
Because dogs and cats sit much higher along the sociozoologic scale com-
pared to mice, those who cherish their canine or feline companions may not
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 357
understand the grief felt by mice enthusiasts over the loss of such an animal.
Consider the case of a young software engineer and his pet mouse named
Sam. Upon hearing that his companion would likely not survive, the young
man was inconsolable. Behind closed doors, some staff members jokingly
whispered to each other, “This guy is nuts. He is crying about a mouse!”
Although the veterinarian herself struggled to understand this man’s emo-
tional attachment to a mouse, she quickly chastised her staff: “That mouse
meant a great deal to this guy and, if we in the veterinary profession think that
his grief is strange, who else is going to understand what he is going through?
It is our job to support his feelings about this mouse and make him feel like it
is okay to cry over the death of a mouse.” While many of us try to keep mice
out of our homes, exterminating them as pests, others cherish their compan-
ionship and consult veterinarians to ensure their health and well-being.
The sociozoologic scale suggests that, despite some people’s attraction and
dedication to the mouse, most people in society will relate to the death of mice
in the same way the veterinary technicians did, thinking “It’s no big deal, it’s
just a mouse.” Although some pet owners experience greater bonds with ani-
mals than they do with humans (Barker and Barker 1988; Carmack 1985),
scholars note a broad societal tendency to trivialize grief over the loss of an
animal companion (Chur-Hansen 2010; Hart, Hart, and Mader 1990; Meyers
2002; Weisman 1991). In one survey of pet owners, for example, more than 50
percent of respondents believed that society did not view the death of a pet as
a loss worthy of grief (Adams, Bonnett, and Meek 2000). When a human fam-
ily member dies most people are surrounded by nurturing friends and family,
but they rarely receive the same attention when their animal dies.
The socioemotional economy suggests that the differing sympathy reac-
tions to deaths of humans and nonhuman animals reflect the fact that society
creates narrow sympathy margins for the loss of those deemed less worthy.
Nevertheless, as shown in the death of “Sam the mouse,” veterinarians
assume an affective role in comforting all bereaved pet owners. Although the
vet was bewildered by her client’s grief, she took him to the grieving room
and tried her best to offer sympathy and inspire confidence that grief over the
death of any animal is “normal” and legitimate. Therefore, according to the
combined logic of the socioemotional economy and the sociozoologic scale,
pet owners felt obligated to provide veterinarians gratitude because reciproc-
ity norms dictated that they were not owed the sympathy and emotional sup-
port they received from veterinarians for their loss.
Hochschild’s (1989) concept of the “economy of gratitude” offers further
explanation for why veterinarians received many more gestures of gratitude
related to euthanasia compared to other services. Hochschild argues that
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
358 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
people offer each other gratitude only when their behavior is thought to go
“above and beyond” what is expected. Thus, perhaps clients feel obligated to
respond to euthanasia with gratitude because they believe they are getting
something extra that is not paid for in the typical fee-for-service exchange. In
the typical veterinary consultation, no extra gratitude is deemed necessary
because veterinarians receive monetary fees in what the client perceives to be
an equal exchange for services. However, when it comes to euthanasia, cli-
ents are never billed for the “extras” such as the time veterinarians spend
providing emotional support. In this light, the seemingly paradoxical response
of pet owners becomes a logical, appropriate reaction to a valuable service—
veterinary emotion work.
Moreover, the content of euthanasia-related cards and letters sent by cli-
ents demonstrate that clients recognize and appreciate the emotion work of
their veterinarians. For example, clients wrote, “Thank you for helping us
through such a tough time,” “Thank you for your kindness, compassion, and
understanding,” and “Thank you for helping us come to such a difficult deci-
sion.” In fact, clients often mention specific emotion-management strategies
such as listening and “being there for me.” For instance, pet owners often
note their appreciation for the veterinarian’s validation of their grief, stating:
“You really get my loss in a way that other people who don’t have pets just
can’t understand—that bond we have with our animals.” In addition to grief,
clients also frequently mention the veterinarian’s efforts to help them resolve
feelings of guilt. As Dr. Turner’s client wrote:
I just had so much guilt over not catching his cancer earlier, but you
helped me know how difficult it is to see when some animals are sick
because they are stubborn or brave like Dawson. You helped me to
finally say goodbye to my beloved friend.
Discussion
Although the work experience of veterinarians has drawn relatively little
attention from social scientists, emotion-laden encounters between veterinarians
and bereaved pet owners provide a rich context to examine emotion manage-
ment in the workplace. This study provides further empirical evidence for the
significance of the role of interpersonal emotion management. In a novel
health care setting, the veterinarian’s repertoire of emotion management
techniques overlaps considerably with many of the strategies utilized by
physicians (DeCoster 1997). The analysis of veterinary emotion work not
only substantiates the findings of other scholars, it expands our understand-
ing of interpersonal emotion management as contextually and emotionally
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 359
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
360 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
the ability to help clients during the stressful time of pet loss is part of what
attracted them to the profession in the first place. Likewise, having empathy
and, more importantly, the ability to convey it effectively to clients is a key
part of their moral identities as a professional—that is, these qualities estab-
lish them as “good” and competent veterinarians.
Significant for scholars interested in human–animal interaction, veteri-
nary emotion work underscores the importance of human–animal bonds for
many of today’s pet owners. Pet owners frequently experienced intense guilt
and grief when confronted with end-of-life decisions for beloved companion
animals. Subsequent research is needed to explore the pet owner’s perspec-
tive of veterinary emotion work. For example, while my participants argued
that dealing with client emotions is essential to building long-term relation-
ships with clients, more research is needed to establish the client’s point of
view. Interviews with pet owners might investigate client satisfaction with
emotion management techniques outlined in this research. This type of
research could aid grief counselors who wish to better understand grief asso-
ciated with pet loss. In addition, veterinary practitioners who seek to improve
client satisfaction would also benefit from this research.
Finally, this research has important implications for veterinary practitio-
ners as it illuminates an often veiled or ignored aspect of the profession.
Although managing client’s emotions is not generally considered an official
aspect of a veterinarian’s job description, my research suggests that small-
animal veterinarians are doing emotion work. For example, not only do vet-
erinarians believe they provide important emotional support to clients
(Pilgram 2010), but pet owners in this study thanked their veterinarians spe-
cifically for providing them comfort and counsel. Although some veterinari-
ans consider such emotion work outside of the domain of knowledge,
experience, and responsibility of veterinarians, my research reveals that
many veterinarians are rethinking old notions of professional responsibility
to include managing the emotions of clients whose animals they euthanize.
This research reveals veterinarians’ growing commitment to recognize the
intense feelings of grief, pain, and sorrow resulting from the death of a pet. For
some veterinarians, caring for the emotional needs of clients feels natural because
they are extremely empathetic to animals and the bonds people share with them.
However, others report considerable anxiety and stress related to this important
aspect of their work. Despite the fact that veterinary students repeatedly identify
dealing with owner emotions as one of their greatest concerns, veterinary schools
have been slow to incorporate training around this issue into the formal curricu-
lum (Cohen-Salter et al. 2004; Herzog, Vore, and New 1989; Pilgram 2010).
Academic coursework related to recognizing and managing the emotional states
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 361
Acknowledgment
I wish to gratefully acknowledge the institutional support I received from Northeastern
and Drury Universities as well as the Charlotte Newcombe and Woodrow Wilson
Fellowship Foundations for their generous support of my research. I would also like
to thank Arnold Arluke, Clinton Sanders, Tom Koenig, Silvia Dominguez, Justin
Betz, Elizabeth Lowe, Andrea Hill, Jeremy Eggerman, Editor Kent Sandstrom, and
four anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments and helpful suggestions on
earlier versions of this article.
Funding
The author disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article: The author received financial support
for this research in the form of a Doctoral Dissertation Fellowship from the Charlotte
W. Newcombe Foundation distributed by the Woodrow Wilson National Fellowship
Foundation.
References
Adams, Cindy, Brenda Bonnett, and Alan Meek. 2000. Client expectations of veterinarians.
Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 217 (9): 1303-9.
Albert, Alexa, and Kris Bulcroft. 1988. Pets, families, and the life course. Journal of
Marriage and the Family 50:543-52.
American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). 2008a. Results of the 2006
AVMA survey of companion animal ownership in US pet-owning households.
Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 232 (5): 695-96.
American Veterinary Medical Association (AVMA). 2008b. Report on veterinary
compensation. Schaumburg, IL: Center for Information Management of the
American Veterinary Medical Association.
American Veterinary Medical Association. 2010. Market research statistics: U.S. vet-
erinarians. http://www.avma.org/reference/marketstats/usvets.asp (accessed Sep-
tember 25, 2011).
Arluke, Arnold. 1997. Veterinary education: A plea and plan for sociological study.
Anthrozoos 10 (1): 3-7.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
362 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Arluke, Arnold. 2004. The use of dogs in medical and veterinary training: Under-
standing and approaching student uneasiness. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare
Science 7 (3): 197-204.
Arluke, Arnold, and Clinton Sanders. 1996. Regarding animals. Philadelphia: Temple
University Press.
Atwood-Harvey, Dana. 2005. Death or declaw: Dealing with moral ambiguity in a
veterinary hospital. Society and Animals 13:315-42.
Barker, Sandra, and Randolph Barker. 1988. The human-canine bond: Closer than
family ties? Journal of Mental Health Counseling 10:46-56.
Blumer, Herbert. 1969. Symbolic interactionism: Perspective and method. Engle-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Brown, Brenda, Herbert Richards, and Carol Wilson. 1996. Pet bonding and pet
bereavement among adolescents. Journal of Counseling & Development 74:505-9.
Bryant, Clifton, and William Snizek. 1976. Animal doctors: Careers, clientele, and prac-
tice modes of veterinarians. In The rural workforce: Non-agricultural occupations
in America, edited by Clifton D. Bryant, Donald J. Shoemaker, James K. Skipper,
and William E. Snizek. South Hadley, MA: Bergin & Garvey.
Bustad, Leo, Linda Hines, and Charles Leathers. 1981. The human-companion animal
bond and the veterinarian. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal
Practice 11 (4): 787-810.
Carmack, Betty J. 1985. The effects on family members and functioning after the death of
a pet. In Pets and the family, edited by Marvin B. Sussman, 3-10. New York: Haworth.
Chieffo, Carla, Alan Kelly, and James Ferguson. 2008. Trends in gender, employment,
salary, and debt of graduates of US veterinary medical schools and colleges. Journal
of the American Veterinary Medical Association 233 (6): 910-17.
Chur-Hansen, Anna. 2010. Grief and bereavement issues and the loss of a companion
animal: People living with a companion animal, owners of livestock, and animal
support workers. Clinical Psychologist 14 (1): 14-21.
Clark, Candace. 1997. Misery and company: Sympathy in everyday life. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Coates, Thomas J., and Bernard Lo. 1990. Counseling patients seropositive for human
immunodeficiency virus: An approach for medical practice. Western Journal of
Medicine 153: 629-34.
Cohen-Salter, Cynthia, Susan Folmer-Brown, Kimberly M. Hogrefe, and Margaret
Brosnahan. 2004. A model euthanasia workshop: One class’s experience at Tufts
University. Journal of Veterinary Medical Education 31 (1): 72-75.
DeCoster, Vaughn A. 1997. Physician treatment of patient emotions: An application
of the sociology of emotion. In Social perspectives on emotion, edited by Rebecca
Erickson and Beverley Cuthbertson-Johnson, 151-77. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 363
Dunn, Kathleen, Stephen Mehler, and Helaine Greenberg. 2005. Social work with a
pet loss support group in a university veterinary hospital. Social Work in Health
Care 41 (2): 59-70.
Field, Nigel, Lisa Orsini, Roni Gavish, and Wendy Packman. 2009. Role of attachment
in response to pet loss. Death Studies 33:334-55.
Francis, Linda. 1994. Laughter, the best mediation: Humor as emotion management
in interaction. Symbolic Interaction 17 (2): 147-63.
Fuller, Ruth L., and Sally B. Geis. 1985. Communicating with the grieving family.
Journal of Family Practice 21 (2): 139-44.
Gage, Geraldine, and Ralph Holcomb. 1991. Couples’ perception of stressfulness of
death of the family pet. Family Relations 40:103-5.
Gauthier, DeAnn. 2001. Professional lapses: Occupational deviance and neutralization
techniques in veterinary medical practice. Deviant Behavior 22 (6): 467-90.
Gerwolls, Marilyn K., and Susan M. Labott. 1994. Adjustment to the death of a companion
animal. Anthrozoos 7 (3): 72-186.
Glaser, Barney, and Anselm Strauss. 1967. The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies
for qualitative research. Piscataway, NJ: Aldine Transaction.
Goffman, Erving. 1959. The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Anchor
Books.
Goffman, Erving. 1963. Behavior in public places: Notes on the social organization
of gatherings. New York: Free Press.
Gosse, Gerald H., and Michael J. Barnes. 1994. Human grief resulting from the death
of a pet. Anthrozoos 7 (2): 103-12.
Grier, Katherine. 2006. Pets in America: A history. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Books.
Groves, James E. 1978. Taking care of the hateful patient. New England Journal of
Medicine 298 (6): 883-87.
Hart, Lynette, Benjamin Hart, and Bonnie Mader. 1990. Humane euthanasia and com-
panion animal death: Caring for the animal, the client, and the veterinarian. Journal
of the American Veterinary Medical Association 197 (10): 1292-99.
Herzog, Herold. 2010. Some we love, some we hate, some we eat: Why it’s so hard to
think straight about animals. New York: HarperCollins.
Herzog, Harold, Tamara Vore, and John New. 1989. Conversations with veterinary
students. Anthrozoos 2 (3): 181-88.
Hetts, Suzanne, and Laurel Lagoni. 1990. The owner of the pet with cancer. Veterinary
Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 20 (4): 879-96.
Hochschild, Arlie. 1983. The managed heart: The commercialization of human feeling.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Hochschild, Arlie. 1989. The second shift. New York: Avon.
Irvine, Leslie, and Jenny Vermilya. 2010. Gender work in a feminized profession: The
case of veterinary medicine. Gender & Society 24 (1): 56-82.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
364 Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 41(3)
Jones, Lynn Cerys. 1997. Both friend and stranger: How crisis volunteers build and
manage unpersonal relationships with clients. In Social perspectives on emotion,
edited by Rebecca Erickson and Beverley Cuthbertson-Johnson, 125-48. Green-
wich, CT: JAI Press.
Katcher, Aaron H. 1989. How companion animals make us feel. In Perceptions of
animals in American culture, edited by R. J. Hoage, 113-27. Washington, DC:
Smithsonian Institution Press.
Lofland, John, David Snow, Leon Anderson, and Lyn Lofland. 2005. Analyzing social
settings: A guide to qualitative observation and analysis. 4th ed. Belmont, CA:
Wadsworth.
Lois, Jennifer. 2001. Managing emotions, intimacy, and relationships in a volunteer
search and rescue group. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 30 (2): 131-79.
Martin, Francois, Kathleen Ruby, Tiffany Deking, and Anne Taunton. 2004. Fac-
tors associated with client, staff, and student satisfaction regarding small animal
euthanasia procedures at a veterinary teaching hospital. Journal of the American
Veterinary Medical Association 224 (11): 1774-79.
Mercer, Laura. 2007. Vet social work program counsels pet owners, staff. The New
Social Worker14 (2): 8.
Meyers, Barbara. 2002. Disenfranchised grief and the loss of an animal companion.
In Disenfranchised grief: New directions, challenges, and strategies for practice,
edited by K. J. Doka, 251-64. Champaign, IL: Research Press.
Pilgram, Mary. 2010. Communicating social support to grieving clients: The veteri-
narian’s view. Death Studies 34:699-714.
Planchon, Lynn A., and Donald I. Templer. 1996. The correlates of grief after death of
a pet. Anthrozoos 10:107-13.
Quackenbush, James E. 1985. The death of a pet: How it can affect owners. Veterinary
Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice 15:395-402.
Rafaeli, Anat, and Robert Sutton. 1991. Emotional contrast strategies as means of
social influence: Lessons from criminal interrogators and bill collectors. Academy
of Management Journal 34:749-75.
Ross, Cheri, and Jane Baron-Sorenson. 1998. Pet loss and human emotion: Guiding
clients through grief. Philadelphia: Accelerated Development.
Sanders, Clinton. 1994. Annoying owners: Routine interactions with problematic clients
in a general veterinary practice. Qualitative Sociology 17 (2): 159-70.
Sanders, Clinton. 1995. Killing with kindness: Veterinary euthanasia and the social
construction of personhood. Sociological Forum 10 (2): 195-214.
Sanders, Clinton. 2010. Working out back: The veterinary technician and “dirty
work.” Journal of Contemporary Ethnography 39 (3): 243-72.
Sanders, Teela. 2004. Controllable laughter: Managing sex work through humour.
Sociology 38 (2): 273-91.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015
Morris 365
Sandstrom, Kent, Dan Martin, and Gary Alan Fine. 2010. Symbols, selves, and social
reality. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press.
Smith, Allen C., and Sherryl Kleinman. 1989. Managing emotions in medical school:
Student’s contacts with the living and the dead. Social Psychology Quarterly 52
(1): 56-69.
Stanford, Gregory, and Stephen Keto. 1991. Creation of the “virtual patient” in medical
interaction: A comparison of doctor/patient and veterinarian/client relationships.
American Sociological Association Paper.
Stern, Michael. 1996. Psychological elements of attachment to pets and responses to pet
loss. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 209 (10): 1707-11.
Stewart, Cyrus, John Thrush, George Paulus, and Patrick Hafner. 1985. The elderly’s
adjustment to the loss of a companion animal: People-pet dependency. Death
Studies 9:383-93.
Sutton, Robert. 1991. Maintaining norms about expressed emotions: The case of bill
collectors. Administrative Science Quarterly 36:245-68.
Swabe, Joanna. 1994. Emotion and interaction: Preserving the emotional order in the
veterinary consultation room. International Sociological Association Paper.
Thoits, Peggy. 1996. Managing the emotions of others. Symbolic Interaction 19:85-110.
Warren, Carol, and Tracy Karner. 2005. Discovering qualitative methods: Field
research, interviews, and analysis. Los Angeles: Roxbury.
Weisman, Avery. 1991. Bereavement and companion animals. Omega: Journal of
Death and Dying 22 (4): 241-48.
Wolkomir, Michelle, and Jennifer Powers. 2007. Helping women and protecting the
self: The challenge of emotional labor in an abortion clinic. Qualitative Sociology
30:153-69.
Wrobel, Thomas, and Amanda Dye. 2003. Grieving pet death: Normative, gender, and
attachment issues. Omega: Journal of Death and Dying 47 (4): 385-93.
Bio
Patricia Morris is an assistant professor of sociology at Drury University and author
of the latest book Blue Juice: Euthanasia in Veterinary Medicine in the Animals,
Culture, and Society series by Temple University Press. She teaches and researches
in the areas of medical sociology, work and occupations, human-animal relationships,
death and dying, and the sociology of emotions.
Downloaded from jce.sagepub.com at UCSF LIBRARY & CKM on April 13, 2015