Attachment Orientation and Pre
Attachment Orientation and Pre
sciences
Article
Attachment Orientation and Preferences for Partners’ Emotional
Responses in Stressful and Positive Situations
Brian N. Chin 1, * , Lauryn Kim 2 , Shelby M. Parsons 2 and Brooke C. Feeney 2
Abstract: Attachment theory proposes that close relationships help us to regulate our emotions
in stressful and positive situations. However, no previous studies have examined preferences for
a partner’s emotional response to one’s own stressful and positive situations or tested whether
these preferences differ based on attachment orientation. This study examines the association of
attachment orientation and preferences for partners’ emotional responses relative to one’s own
emotional responses in stressful and positive contexts among 425 United States adults who were
currently in a committed relationship of ≥6 months. Data were collected in 2020. Overall, participants
preferred their partners to feel and express less distress, less worry, more calm, and more hope than
themselves during stressful situations and for their partners to feel and express more excitement,
pride, and hope than themselves during positive situations. Higher attachment anxiety predicted
preferences for partners to feel and express more distress/worry in stressful situations, whereas higher
attachment avoidance predicted preferences for partners to feel and express less hope in stressful
situations. Statistical interactions of attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance indicated that
the combination of low attachment anxiety and high attachment avoidance (dismissing avoidance)
was associated with preferences for partners to feel and express less positive emotions in positive
situations, whereas the combination of high attachment anxiety and high attachment avoidance
(fearful avoidance) was associated with preferences for partners to feel and express more negative
emotions in stressful situations and less positive emotions in positive situations. This investigation
Citation: Chin, B.N.; Kim, L.; Parsons,
provides novel evidence for links between attachment orientation and preferences for partners’
S.M.; Feeney, B.C. Attachment
Orientation and Preferences for
emotional responses in two theoretically important contexts, which has implications for the nature
Partners’ Emotional Responses in and function of emotion regulation in close relationships. Future research is needed to determine the
Stressful and Positive Situations. generalizability of these findings to more collectivist cultural contexts.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77. https://
doi.org/10.3390/bs14010077 Keywords: close relationships; relational regulation; emotion regulation; social support; affect
have found that avoidantly attached individuals show less positive facial affect when view-
ing positive stimuli [18,19], whereas anxiously attached individuals show more negative
facial affect when viewing positive stimuli (i.e., happy faces) [20]. Similarly, a daily diary
study indicated that anxious attachment and avoidant attachment are both associated with
blunted positive emotional responses to positive daily events [21].
2. Current Research
We aimed to build on past research examining attachment and emotional responses
by investigating whether attachment orientation predicts individuals’ preferences for
partners’ emotional responses, relative to their own emotional responses, in stressful
situations (Aim 1) and positive situations (Aim 2). We conducted an online survey study
of individuals who were currently in a committed romantic relationship that had been
ongoing for at least six months. Participants reflected on times when they discussed
stressful situations and positive situations with their romantic partners and indicated their
preferences for their partners’ emotional responses (relative to their own responses) in those
situations. We assessed participants’ preferences about two aspects of partners’ emotional
responses to each type of situation—their partner’s emotional experience (felt emotion)
and their partner’s emotional expression (expressed emotion). We assessed preferences
for specific emotions that were theoretically relevant to stressful situations (distress, worry,
calm, hope) and positive situations (excitement, pride, hope, calm). We tested the following
specific hypotheses based on the postulates of attachment theory and past research on adult
attachment orientation and emotions.
First, we predicted that attachment anxiety would be associated with preferences for
partners to respond to one’s stressful situations by feeling and expressing more distress,
more worry, less calm, and less hope than themselves (Hypothesis 1a). We also predicted
that attachment avoidance would be associated with preferences for partners to respond
to one’s stressful situations by feeling and expressing less distress, less worry, less calm,
and less hope than themselves (Hypothesis 1b). We hypothesized that these preferences
would help anxious individuals, who tend to hyperactivate their negative emotions in
stressful situations, and avoidant individuals, who tend to deactivate their positive and
negative emotions in stressful contexts, to accomplish their emotion regulation goals in
stressful situations.
Second, we predicted that attachment anxiety would be associated with preferences for
partners to respond to one’s positive situations by feeling and expressing more excitement,
more pride, more hope, and less calm than themselves (Hypothesis 2a). We also predicted
that attachment avoidance would be associated with preferences for partners to respond to
one’s positive situations by feeling and expressing less excitement, less pride, less hope,
and more calm than themselves (Hypothesis 2b). We hypothesized that these preferences
would help anxious individuals, who desire their partner’s approval and validation, and
avoidant individuals, who prefer to keep their attachment needs deactivated, to accomplish
their emotion regulation goals in positive situations.
3. Method
3.1. Participants and Procedures
Participants were 425 adults recruited through Amazon Mechanical Turk (n = 253)
and Volunteer Science (n = 172) for a study of romantic relationships. Participants’ ages
ranged from 18 to 88 years (M = 41.3, SD = 12.9). In total, 51.5% identified as female, 47.5%
as male, and 0.9% as genderqueer or nonbinary. In total, 56.7% of participants identified
as White, 32.0% as Asian, 3.5% as Black, 3.5% as multiracial, 2.8% as Hispanic, 0.5% as
Native/Indigenous, and 0.7% as another race; 1 participant preferred not to disclose their
race. In total, 79.8% of participants were cohabiting with their romantic partner. Sample
size was determined based on the desire to maximize power by collecting data from as
many participants as possible; data collection was halted when funding was no longer
available. Data were collected between March and December 2020. Inclusion criteria were
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 4 of 17
being ≥18 years old, fluent in English, and in a committed romantic relationship of at least
six months. All study procedures were approved by a university institutional review board
and complied with APA ethical standards for the treatment of human participants.
After providing informed consent, participants completed questionnaires via Qualtrics
assessing their demographic information, attachment orientation, and preferences for being
supported by romantic partners in stressful and positive situations. Participants were
compensated USD 3 for completing the study via Amazon Mechanical Turk; participants
did not receive compensation for completing the study via Volunteer Science.
We conducted a post hoc power analysis using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 [22] to compute
achieved power for a linear regression with two tested predictors, a significance criterion
of α = 0.05, and sample size = 425. Achieved power was 1.00 to detect a medium effect
(f2 = 0.15) and 0.74 to detect a small effect (f2 = 0.02).
3.2. Measures
3.2.1. Attachment Orientation
We assessed general attachment orientation using a 14-item version of the Experiences
in Close Relationships (ECR) scale [23]. Participants rated the extent to which they agreed
with statements assessing attachment anxiety (seven items, e.g., “I worry about being
abandoned”) and attachment avoidance (seven items, e.g., “I try avoiding getting too close
to people”) in their global attachment relationships (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly
agree). Attachment anxiety (α = 0.92) and attachment avoidance (α = 0.88) composites were
computed by averaging the scores of items for each subscale.
to what extent do you typically prefer that your partner respond in the following ways?
Some questions below ask about two different aspects of your emotional life. One is your
emotional experience, or what you feel inside. The other is your emotional expression, or
how you communicate your emotions to others in the way you talk, gesture, or behave.”
Participants rated four items assessing the extent to which they preferred their partner
to feel excitement, calm, pride, and hope about the positive situation relative to their
own feelings (−3 = much less than me, 0 = as much as me, 3 = much more than me).
Participants also rated four items assessing the extent to which they preferred their partner
to express excitement, calm, pride, and hope about the positive situation relative to their
own expressions of each emotion (−3 = much less than me, 0 = as much as me, 3 = much
more than me).
4. Data Analysis
Our descriptive analyses included bivariate correlations between predictor and out-
come variables and paired samples t-tests evaluating whether participants differed in their
preferences for partners to feel vs. express distress, worry, calm, and hope in stressful
situations or in their preferences for partners to feel vs. express excitement, pride, calm,
and hope in positive situations.
To address our first aim, we used linear regression models to test associations of
attachment orientation (attachment anxiety, attachment avoidance, and the interaction
of attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance) and preferences for partners’ emotional
responses to stressful situations (feeling and expressing distress, worry, calm, and hope)
relative to their own feelings and expressions.
To address our second aim, we used linear regression models to test associations
of attachment orientation and preferences for partners’ emotional responses to positive
situations (feeling and expressing excitement, calm, pride, and hope) relative to their own
feelings and expressions.
To interpret statistically significant interactions of attachment anxiety × attachment
avoidance, we conducted planned follow-up analyses of simple slopes of attachment
anxiety at one standard deviation above and below the mean value of attachment avoidance.
We conducted these analyses with and without controlling for participants’ gender.
Because all associations were unaffected by the inclusion of gender as a covariate, we only
report the results of the more parsimonious models that did not control for gender.
5. Results
Descriptive Analyses
Descriptive statistics for preferred partner emotions in stressful situations and positive
situations are shown in Table 1. Bivariate correlations of primary outcome variables are
shown in Table 2.
M (SD)
Attachment anxiety 3.3 (1.5)
Attachment avoidance 3.6 (1.4)
During stressful situations:
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 6 of 17
Table 1. Cont.
M (SD)
I want my partner to feel:
Distress −0.7 (1.6)
Worry −0.7 (1.5)
Calm 1.0 (1.4)
Hope 1.3 (1.2)
I want my partner to express:
Distress −0.6 (1.6)
Worry −0.7 (1.5)
Calm 1.0 (1.5)
Hope 1.2 (1.4)
During positive situations:
I want my partner to feel:
Excitement 0.8 (1.2)
Pride 1.1 (1.2)
Hope 1.0 (1.2)
Calm 0.2 (1.3)
I want my partner to express:
Excitement 0.8 (1.3)
Pride 0.9 (1.3)
Hope 0.9 (1.3)
Calm 0.1 (1.2)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18
1. Attachment anxiety --
2. Attachment avoidance 0.53 --
3. Partner feels distress 0.34 0.18 --
4. Partner feels worry 0.28 0.18 0.55 --
5. Partner feels calm −0.07 −0.08 −0.43 −0.28 --
6. Partner feels hope −0.11 −0.18 −0.11 −0.19 0.32 --
7. Partner expresses
0.23 0.08 0.74 0.52 −0.43 −0.12 --
distress
8. Partner expresses
0.24 0.18 0.54 0.82 −0.29 −0.17 0.56 --
worry
9. Partner expresses calm −0.16 −0.10 −0.47 −0.30 0.76 0.35 −0.44 −0.34 --
10. Partner expresses
−0.06 −0.15 −0.15 −0.16 0.30 0.62 −0.10 −0.13 0.39 --
hope
11. Partner feels
0.15 −0.02 0.43 0.30 −0.27 0.14 0.38 0.32 −0.27 0.00 --
excitement
12. Partner feels pride 0.18 0.01 0.29 0.21 −0.10 0.22 0.29 0.26 −0.10 0.14 0.65 --
13. Partner feels hope 0.10 −0.12 0.28 0.23 −0.13 0.19 0.32 0.23 −0.10 0.14 0.57 0.71 --
14. Partner feels calm 0.13 0.12 0.12 0.15 0.15 −0.01 0.05 0.11 0.05 −0.10 0.11 0.11 0.15 --
15. Partner expresses
0.06 −0.09 0.30 0.20 −0.18 0.12 0.34 0.23 −0.17 0.06 0.76 0.56 0.57 0.04 --
excitement
16. Partner expresses
0.00 −0.14 0.12 0.13 −0.04 0.19 0.23 0.14 0.03 0.22 0.45 0.74 0.60 −0.04 0.55 --
pride
17. Partner expresses
−0.05 −0.18 0.16 0.13 −0.12 0.17 0.28 0.17 −0.03 0.21 0.49 0.62 0.79 0.05 0.51 0.63 --
hope
18. Partner expresses
0.05 0.04 0.04 0.05 0.12 −0.04 0.01 0.07 0.10 −0.08 0.07 0.07 0.15 0.79 0.04 −0.06 0.13 --
calm
Note. Bolded coefficients denote statistical significance at p < 0.05.
As shown in Table 1, participants generally preferred their partners to feel and express
less distress, less worry, more calm, and more hope relative to themselves during stressful
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 7 of 17
situations and for their partners to feel and express more excitement, pride, and hope
relative to themselves during positive situations. Participants also generally preferred
their partners to feel more calm, but not express more calm, relative to themselves during
positive situations.
Participants reported a stronger preference for partners to feel (vs. express) more
hope than themselves in stressful situations (Mdiff = 0.11, 95CIdiff = 0.00, 0.22, paired
samples t(424) = 2.01, p = 0.045, d = 0.10). Participants did not differ in their preferences for
partners to feel vs. express distress (Mdiff = −0.08, 95CIdiff = −0.19, 0.03, paired samples
t(423) = −1.39, p = 0.17, d = −0.07), to feel vs. express worry (Mdiff = 0.05, 95CIdiff = −0.04,
0.14, paired samples t(424) = 1.16, p = 0.25, d = 0.06), or to feel vs. express calm (Mdiff = 0.00,
95CIdiff = −0.09, 0.10, paired samples t(423) = 0.05, p = 0.96, d = 0.00).
Participants reported a stronger preference for partners to feel (vs. express) more pride
(Mdiff = 0.11, 95CIdiff = 0.03, 0.20, paired samples t(424) = 2.58, p = 0.010, d = 0.13), more
hope (Mdiff = 0.13, 95CIdiff = 0.05, 0.20, paired samples t(424) = 3.28, p = 0.001, d = 0.16), and
more calm (Mdiff = 0.16, 95CIdiff = 0.08, 0.24, paired samples t(424) = 4.05, p < 0.001, d = 0.16)
than themselves in positive situations. Participants did not differ in their preference for
partners to feel vs. express excitement (Mdiff = 0.06, 95CIdiff = −0.03, 0.14, paired samples
t(424) = 1.36, p = 0.18, d = 0.07).
Aim 1: Preferences for partners’ emotional responses to one’s own stressful situations.
We examined whether participants’ attachment orientation was associated with their
preferences for partners’ emotional responses, relative to their own emotional responses,
during the participants’ stressful situations (Table 3).
Table 3. Main effect and interaction of attachment anxiety and avoidance on preferences for partners’
felt and expressed emotions when providing support in stressful situations.
B SE t p 95CI
Partner Feels Distress
Step 1
Intercept −0.72 0.08 −9.62 <0.001 −0.86, −0.57
Attachment anxiety 0.55 0.09 6.30 <0.001 0.38, 0.73
Attachment avoidance −0.01 0.09 −0.07 0.95 −0.18, 0.17
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.24 0.08 3.07 0.002 0.09, 0.39
Partner Feels Worry
Step 1
Intercept −0.66 0.07 −9.28 <0.001 −0.80, −0.52
Attachment anxiety 0.39 0.08 4.59 <0.001 0.22, 0.55
Attachment avoidance 0.07 0.08 0.83 0.41 −0.10, 0.24
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.14 0.07 1.91 0.06 −0.00, 0.29
Partner Feels Calm
Step 1
Intercept 1.02 0.07 14.92 <0.001 0.88, 1.15
Attachment anxiety −0.06 0.08 −0.80 0.43 −0.22, 0.09
Attachment avoidance −0.08 0.08 −0.93 0.35 −0.23, 0.08
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.07 0.07 1.02 0.31 −0.07, 0.21
Partner Feels Hope
Step 1
Intercept 1.30 0.06 22.56 <0.001 1.19, 1.42
Attachment anxiety −0.02 0.07 −0.36 0.72 −0.16, 0.11
Attachment avoidance −0.20 0.07 −2.90 0.004 −0.33, −0.06
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.11 0.06 1.88 0.06 −0.01, 0.23
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 8 of 17
Table 3. Cont.
B SE t p 95CI
Partner Expresses Distress
Step 1
Intercept −0.64 0.08 −8.53 <0.001 −0.79, −0.49
Attachment anxiety 0.41 0.09 4.69 <0.001 0.24, 0.59
Attachment avoidance −0.09 0.09 −1.06 0.29 −0.27, 0.08
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.02 0.08 0.24 0.81 −0.13, 0.17
Partner Expresses Worry
Step 1
Intercept −0.71 0.07 −9.81 <0.001 −0.85, −0.57
Attachment anxiety 0.32 0.09 3.69 <0.001 0.15, 0.48
Attachment avoidance 0.11 0.09 1.22 0.22 −0.06, 0.27
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.17 0.08 2.27 0.02 0.02, 0.32
Partner Expresses Calm
Step 1
Intercept 1.01 0.07 14.16 <0.001 0.87, 1.15
Attachment anxiety −0.21 0.08 −2.52 0.012 −0.38, −0.05
Attachment avoidance −0.03 0.08 −0.41 0.68 −0.20, 0.13
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.08 0.07 1.04 0.30 −0.07, 0.22
Partner Expresses Hope
Step 1
Intercept 1.19 0.07 17.51 <0.001 1.06, 1.32
Attachment anxiety 0.03 0.08 0.33 0.74 −0.13, 0.19
Attachment avoidance −0.22 0.08 −2.76 0.006 −0.38, −0.06
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.00 0.07 0.06 0.96 −0.13, 0.14
Consistent with Hypothesis 1a, participants with higher attachment anxiety preferred
their partners to feel and express more distress, to feel and express more worry, and
to express less calm relative to themselves during the participants’ stressful situations.
Contrary to Hypothesis 1a, attachment anxiety was not associated with preferences for
partners to feel calm, feel hope, or express hope during stressful situations.
Consistent with Hypothesis 1b, participants with higher attachment avoidance pre-
ferred their partners to feel and express less hope relative to themselves during the par-
ticipants’ stressful situations. Contrary to Hypothesis 1b, attachment avoidance was not
associated with preferences for partners to feel or express distress, worry, or calm during
stressful situations.
Next, we tested the interaction of attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance on pref-
erences for partners’ emotional responses during the participants’ stressful situations. We
observed interactions of attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance predicting preferences
for partners to feel distress and express worry during the participants’ stressful situations.
Examination of marginal slopes for attachment anxiety at low and high levels of attachment
avoidance in Figure 1 indicated that: (A) attachment anxiety was more strongly associated
with the preference for partners to feel more distress (relative to themselves) during stress-
ful situations when participants also had higher levels of attachment avoidance (fearful
avoidance) (b = 0.79, SE = 0.12, t = 6.82, p < 0.001, 95CI = 0.56, 1.01) than when participants
had lower levels of attachment avoidance (preoccupied attachment) (b = 0.32, SE = 0.12,
t = 2.71, p = 0.007, 95CI = 0.09, 0.55); and (B) attachment anxiety was associated with the
preference for partners to express more worry (relative to themselves) during stressful situ-
ations when participants also had higher levels of attachment avoidance (fearful avoidance)
(b = 0.48, SE = 0.11, t = 4.29, p < 0.001, 95CI = 0.26, 0.71) but not when participants had
avoidance (fearful avoidance) (b = 0.79, SE = 0.12, t = 6.82, p < 0.001, 95CI = 0.56, 1.01) than
when participants had lower levels of attachment avoidance (preoccupied attachment) (b
= 0.32, SE = 0.12, t = 2.71, p = 0.007, 95CI = 0.09, 0.55); and (B) attachment anxiety was
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 associated with the preference for partners to express more worry (relative to themselves) 9 of 17
during stressful situations when participants also had higher levels of attachment avoid-
ance (fearful avoidance) (b = 0.48, SE = 0.11, t = 4.29, p < 0.001, 95CI = 0.26, 0.71) but not
when
lowerparticipants had lower
levels of attachment levels of(preoccupied
avoidance attachment avoidance (preoccupied
attachment) (b = 0.14, SEattachment) (b
= 0.11, t = 1.26,
=p0.14, SE = 0.11, t = 1.26, p
= 0.21, 95CI = −0.08, 0.37).= 0.21, 95CI = −0.08, 0.37).
Interaction plots
Figure1.1.Interaction
Figure plots showing
showing preferences
preferences for
forpartners
partnerstotofeel
feeldistress
distress(A)
(A)and
andexpress worry
express (B)
worry
during stressful situations by attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance.
(B) during stressful situations by attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance.
Therewas
There wasnonointeraction
interactionof
ofparticipants’
participants’ attachment anxiety ××attachment
attachment anxiety attachmentavoidance
avoidance
predicting preferences for partners’ other felt or expressed emotions.
predicting preferences for partners’ other felt or expressed emotions.
Aim2:2:Preferences
Aim Preferencesfor
forpartners’
partners’emotional
emotionalresponses
responsestotoone’s
one’sown
ownpositive
positivesituations.
situations.
Weexamined
We examinedwhether
whetherparticipants’
participants’attachment
attachmentorientation
orientationwas
wasassociated
associatedwith
withtheir
their
preferences for partners’ emotional responses, relative to their own emotional
preferences for partners’ emotional responses, relative to their own emotional responses, responses,
duringthe
during theparticipants’
participants’positive
positivesituations
situations(Table
(Table4).
4).
Consistent with Hypothesis 2a, participants with higher attachment anxiety preferred
their partners to feel and express more excitement, to feel and express more pride, and
to feel more hope than themselves during positive situations. Contrary to Hypothesis 2a,
attachment anxiety was not associated with preferences for partners to feel calm, express
calm, or express hope during positive situations.
Consistent with Hypothesis 2b, participants with higher attachment avoidance pre-
ferred their partners to feel and express less excitement, less pride, and less hope than
themselves during the participants’ positive situations. Contrary to Hypothesis 2b, attach-
ment avoidance was not associated with preferences for partners to feel calm or express
calm during positive situations.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 10 of 17
Table 4. Main effect and interaction of attachment anxiety and avoidance on preferences for partners’
felt and expressed emotions (relative to one’s own emotions) when providing support in positive
situations.
B SE t p 95CI
Partner Feels Excitement
Step 1
Intercept 0.81 0.06 13.99 <0.001 0.70, 0.93
Attachment anxiety 0.27 0.07 3.97 <0.001 0.14, 0.41
Attachment avoidance −0.17 0.07 −2.43 0.016 −0.30, −0.03
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.15 0.06 2.55 0.011 0.04, 0.27
Partner Feels Proud
Step 1
Intercept 1.05 0.06 18.40 <0.001 0.94, 1.16
Attachment anxiety 0.30 0.07 4.41 <0.001 0.17, 0.43
Attachment avoidance −0.15 0.07 −2.25 0.025 −0.28, −0.02
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.17 0.06 2.86 0.004 0.05, 0.28
Partner Feels Hope
Step 1
Intercept 0.98 0.06 17.32 <0.001 0.87, 1.09
Attachment anxiety 0.28 0.07 4.13 <0.001 0.14, 0.41
Attachment avoidance −0.29 0.07 −4.35 <0.001 −0.42, −0.16
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.09 0.06 1.53 0.13 −0.03, 0.20
Partner Feels Calm
Step 1
Intercept 0.21 0.06 3.39 <0.001 0.09, 0.33
Attachment anxiety 0.12 0.07 1.67 0.10 −0.02, 0.27
Attachment avoidance 0.09 0.07 1.26 0.21 −0.05, 0.24
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.21 0.06 3.35 <0.001 0.09, 0.34
Partner Expresses Excitement
Step 1
Intercept 0.76 0.06 12.34 <0.001 0.64, 0.88
Attachment anxiety 0.19 0.07 2.63 0.009 0.05, 0.33
Attachment avoidance −0.21 0.07 −2.94 0.004 −0.36, −0.07
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.14 0.06 2.25 0.025 0.02, 0.27
Partner Expresses Pride
Step 1
Intercept 0.94 0.06 15.28 <0.001 0.82, 1.06
Attachment anxiety 0.15 0.07 2.01 0.046 0.00, 0.29
Attachment avoidance −0.26 0.07 −3.62 <0.001 −0.41, −0.12
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.03 0.06 .41 0.68 −0.10, 0.15
Partner Expresses Hope
Step 1
Intercept 0.85 0.06 14.14 <0.001 0.73, 0.97
Attachment anxiety 0.08 0.07 1.13 0.26 −0.06, 0.22
Attachment avoidance −0.27 0.07 −3.78 <0.001 −0.41, −0.13
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.09 0.06 1.50 0.13 −0.03, 0.22
Partner Expresses Calm
Step 1
Intercept 0.05 0.06 0.85 0.40 −0.07, 0.16
Attachment anxiety 0.05 0.07 0.73 0.47 −0.09, 0.19
Attachment avoidance 0.02 0.07 0.23 0.82 −0.12, 0.15
Step 2
Attachment anxiety × attachment avoidance 0.17 0.06 2.91 0.004 0.06, 0.29
Figure
Figure 2.2.Interaction plots
Interaction showing
plots preferences
showing for partners
preferences to feeltoexcitement
for partners (A), express
feel excitement excite-
(A), express
ment (B), feel calm (C), express calm (D), and feel pride (E) during positive situations by attachment
excitement (B), feel calm (C), express calm (D), and feel pride (E) during positive situations by
anxiety and attachment avoidance.
attachment anxiety and attachment avoidance.
6. Discussion
To our knowledge, this study was the first to investigate preferences for partners’
emotional responses to one’s own stressful and positive situations relative to one’s own
emotional responses in these situations. This design facilitated interpretation of these
preferences in terms of whether they reflected a desire for partners’ emotional responses
to amplify, dampen, or maintain individuals’ own emotions in each context. Overall, we
found that participants preferred for their partners to dampen their negative emotions
in stressful contexts and to amplify their positive emotions in both stressful and positive
contexts. We found evidence for most hypothesized associations of attachment orientation
and preferences for partners’ emotional responses in each context. Compared to securely
attached individuals, anxiously attached individuals preferred their partners to feel and
express more negative emotions in stressful situations and more positive emotions in
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 12 of 17
positive situations, while avoidantly attached individuals preferred their partners to feel
and express less positive emotions in both stressful situations and positive situations. We
also found that individuals with high anxiety and high avoidance (i.e., fearful avoidance)
preferred their partners to feel and express more negative emotions in stressful situations
and fewer positive emotions in positive situations, whereas individuals with high avoidance
and low anxiety (i.e., avoidant dismissing) preferred their partners to feel and express less
positive emotions in positive situations. These findings provide a greater understanding of
the emotion regulation strategies that individuals prefer to use in their close relationships
and how these preferences are shaped by our general attachment working models.
recalled by participants was impacted by their attachment orientation. For example, future
studies are needed to test the alternative possibility that anxiously attached individuals
preferred their partners to feel more distress because they recalled more intense stressful
events than securely attached individuals [10,21]. Future studies could also address this
limitation by including measures to assess the type and intensity of events recalled by each
participant and statistically controlling for these characteristics.
Overall, these findings contribute to research on attachment theory and interpersonal
emotion regulation by demonstrating that attachment insecurity shapes how individu-
als prefer to regulate their emotions in close relationships in two theoretically important
contexts. In general, we observed that anxiously attached individuals’ preferences were
consistent with a desire for their partners’ validation and approval, whereas avoidantly
attached individuals’ preferences were consistent with a desire to deactivate their emo-
tions and attachment needs. Future research is needed to establish the types of partner
emotional responses that will most benefit individuals during their stressful and positive
life contexts and over time. Moreover, there is a general need for continued research on
preferences for how partners listen and respond to different types of self-disclosures in
close relationships [33].
Author Contributions: Conceptualization, B.N.C., L.K., S.M.P. and B.C.F.; formal analysis, B.N.C.;
methodology, B.N.C., L.K., S.M.P. and B.C.F.; writing—original draft, B.N.C.; writing—review
and editing, L.K., S.M.P. and B.C.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version
of the manuscript.
Funding: This research was partially supported by a small undergraduate research grant for a senior
honors thesis project conducted at Carnegie Mellon University, and by a faculty completion grant
from the dean of faculty office at Trinity College awarded to the first author.
Institutional Review Board Statement: This study was conducted according to the guidelines of
the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Carnegie Mellon
University in 2019 with protocol code STUDY2019_00000486.
Informed Consent Statement: Informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in
this study.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 16 of 17
Data Availability Statement: The data and analysis code for this study are available at: https:
//osf.io/nkmbw/?view_only=bb9aa20a40fc4e26a89866ed7b264efc.
Conflicts of Interest: All authors declare no conflicts of interest.
References
1. Feeney, B.C.; Collins, N.L. A New Look at Social Support. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 19, 113–147. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
2. Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. Attachment orientations and emotion regulation. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2019, 25, 6–10. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
3. Bowlby, J. Attachment and Loss: Vol. 1 Attachment, 2nd ed.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1982.
4. Ainsworth, M.S. Infant–mother attachment. Am. Psychol. 1979, 34, 932–937. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
5. Cassidy, J. Emotion Regulation: Influences of Attachment Relationships. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994, 59, 228–249. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
6. Cassidy, J.; Berlin, L.J. The Insecure/Ambivalent Pattern of Attachment: Theory and Research. Child Dev. 1994, 65, 971. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
7. Collins, N.L.; Read, S.J. Adult attachment, working models, and relationship quality in dating couples. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990,
58, 644–663. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
8. Cassidy, J.; Kobak, R.R. Avoidance and its relation to other defensive processes. In Clinical Implications of Attachment; Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988; pp. 300–323.
9. Mikulincer, M.; Orbach, I. Attachment styles and repressive defensiveness: The accessibility and architecture of affective memories.
J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 68, 917–925. [CrossRef]
10. Hudson, N.W.; Chopik, W.J. Seeing you reminds me of things that never happened: Attachment anxiety predicts false memories
when people can see the communicator. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2023, 124, 396–412. [CrossRef]
11. Hudson, N.W.; Fraley, R.C. Does attachment anxiety promote the encoding of false memories? An investigation of the processes
linking adult attachment to memory errors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 115, 688–715. [CrossRef]
12. Reynolds, S.; Searight, R.; Ratwik, S. Adult attachment styles and rumination in the context of intimate relationships. N. Am. J.
Psychol. 2014, 16, 495–506.
13. Silva, C.; Soares, I.; Esteves, F. Attachment insecurity and strategies for regulation: When emotion triggers attention. Scand. J.
Psychol. 2012, 53, 9–16. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14. Fraley, R.C.; Shaver, P.R. Adult attachment and the suppression of unwanted thoughts. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1997, 73, 1080–1091.
[CrossRef]
15. Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. Attachment in Adulthood: Structure, Dynamics, and Change, 2nd ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY,
USA, 2016.
16. Simpson, J.A.; Winterheld, H.A.; Rholes, W.S.; Oriña, M.M. Working models of attachment and reactions to different forms of
caregiving from romantic partners. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 93, 466–477. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
17. Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. Adult Attachment and Happiness: Individual Differences in the Experience and Consequences of Positive
Emotions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013. [CrossRef]
18. Magai, C.; Hunziker, J.; Mesias, W.; Culver, L.C. Adult attachment styles and emotional biases. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2000, 24, 301–309.
[CrossRef]
19. Spangler, G.; Zimmermann, P. Attachment representation and emotion regulation in adolescents: A psychobiological perspective
on internal working models. Attach. Hum. Dev. 1999, 1, 270–290. [CrossRef]
20. Sonnby-Borgstrom, M.; Jonsson, P. Models-of-self and models-of-others as related to facial muscle reactions at different levels of
cognitive control. Scand. J. Psychol. 2003, 44, 141–151. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
21. Gentzler, A.; Kerns, K. Adult attachment and memory of emotional reactions to negative and positive events. Cogn. Emot. 2006,
20, 20–42. [CrossRef]
22. Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral,
and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [CrossRef]
23. Brennan, K.A.; Clark, C.L.; Shaver, P.R. Self-report measurement of adult attachment: An integrative overview. In Attachment
Theory and Close Relationships; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1998; pp. 46–76.
24. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 2nd ed.; Guilford
Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018.
25. Collins, N.L.; Allard, L.M. Cognitive representations of attachment: The content and function of working models. In Blackwell
Handbook of Social Psychology: Interpersonal Processes; Blackwell Publishers Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2001; Volume 2, pp. 60–85.
26. Simpson, J.A.; Rholes, W.S. Fearful-avoidance, disorganization, and multiple working models: Some directions for future theory
and research. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2002, 4, 223–229. [CrossRef]
27. Collins, N.L. Working models of attachment: Implications for explanation, emotion, and behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1996, 71,
810–832. [CrossRef]
28. Feeney, B.C.; Van Vleet, M.; Jakubiak, B.K.; Tomlinson, J.M. Predicting the Pursuit and Support of Challenging Life Opportunities.
Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 43, 1171–1187. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 77 17 of 17
29. Peters, B.J.; Reis, H.T.; Gable, S.L. Making the good even better: A review and theoretical model of interpersonal capitalization.
Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2018, 12, e12407. [CrossRef]
30. Mikulincer, M.; Shaver, P.R. Attachment theory and emotions in close relationships: Exploring the attachment-related dynamics
of emotional reactions to relational events. Pers. Relatsh. 2005, 12, 149–168. [CrossRef]
31. Simpson, J.A.; Collins, W.A.; Tran, S.; Haydon, K.C. Attachment and the experience and expression of emotions in romantic
relationships: A developmental perspective. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2007, 92, 355–367. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
32. Butler, E.A.; Randall, A.K. Emotional Coregulation in Close Relationships. Emot. Rev. 2013, 5, 202–210. [CrossRef]
33. Itzchakov, G.; Reis, H.T. Listening and perceived responsiveness: Unveiling the significance and exploring crucial research
endeavors. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2023, 53, 101662. [CrossRef]
34. Wu, D.C.; Kim, H.S.; Collins, N.L. Perceived responsiveness across cultures: The role of cultural fit in social support use. Soc. Pers.
Psychol. Compass 2021, 15, e12634. [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual
author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to
people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.
Reproduced with permission of copyright owner. Further reproduction
prohibited without permission.