Block-4
Block-4
Block
4
ANIMAL WELFARE-II
UNIT 16
Improving Animal Welfare through Social Conditions 111
UNIT 17
Improving Animal Welfare through Human Contact 125
UNIT 18
Improving Animal Welfare through Genetic Selection 144
UNIT 19
Animal Ethics and Sentience 159
UNIT 20
Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-I 177
UNIT 21
Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-II 191
COURSE NAME: ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR AND ANIMAL COURSE CODE: MZOE-002
WELFARE ETHICS
April, 2024
Indira Gandhi National Open University, 2024
ISBN: ..................................
All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced in any form, by mimeograph or any other
means, without permission in writing from Indira Gandhi National Open University.
Further information on Indira Gandhi National Open University courses may be obtained from the
University’s office at Maidan Garhi, New Delhi-110 068 or IGNOU website www.ignou.ac.in.
Printed and published on behalf of Indira Gandhi National Open University, New Delhi by the Registrar,
MPDD, IGNOU.
Printed at:
BLOCK 4: ANIMAL WELFARE-II
In the previous block we have discussed about concept of animal welfare and its scientific
understanding. The five freedoms and their advantages and disadvantages were also
discussed. You were also apprised about the application of animal welfare knowledge in the
form of animal welfare assessment and audit schemes. Then the question is how to improve
the welfare of animals based on the results of welfare assessment or audit? The purpose of
this block is to answer this question with discussion on improving animal welfare through
social conditions, human contact and genetic selection. In this block we will address why
animal welfare is considered to be important from ethical and moral perspectives and
different areas of ethical thinking concerning the use of animals. After completing this block
you should have a better theoretical grasp of animal welfare ethics and how these can be
applied to decision-making for animal treatment, understand the scientific evidence for the
presence of sentience in different types of animals and appreciate the role of veterinarians in
ethical treatment of animals and their welfare.
UNIT 16, Improving Animal Welfare Through Social Conditions delineates the complexity
of social relationships, costs and benefits of social living, dominance relationships, social
environment and its relation to welfare improvement and solving socially induced welfare
problems.
UNIT 17, Improving Animal Welfare Through Human Contact focuses on influence of
human contact on animal welfare, the effects of negative and positive contacts and neglect,
opportunities and methods to improve human-animal relationships, animal signals and
human-animal conflict.
UNIT 18, Improving Animal Welfare Through Genetic Selection describes the power of
animal breeding, genetic vs. non-genetic determination, undesirable effects for welfare of
past breeding strategies and using breeding to improve welfare.
UNIT 19, Animal Ethics and Sentience delineates different types of ethics, ethical views of
animals, ethical decision making, animal sentience and scientific evidence for animal
sentience.
UNIT 20, Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-I focuses on consequentialism, contractarian
utilitarian and respect for nature ethical approaches with case studies and ethical dilemmas
for animal welfare.
UNIT 21, Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-II describes the deontological, animal rights and
virtue ethics, ethical approaches with case studies and practical ethical decision-making
Objectives
After studying this block, you should be able to:
• describe the methods / principles for improving the animal welfare through human
contact, genetic selection and social conditions,
109
110
UNIT 16
IMPROVING ANIMAL WELFARE
THROUGH SOCIAL
CONDITIONS
Structure
16.1 Introduction 16.4 Solving Socially Induced
Objectives Welfare Problems
16.1 INTRODUCTION
In previous Units we have discussed how, in recent decades, the use of
animals has grown:
• More than 70 thousand million farm animals (birds and mammals) are
reared worldwide each year, and
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
b) Practical and Professional Skills: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
• Species
Social groups span a continuum of size and social complexity under wild
conditions. The tendency to adopt similar social strategies persists under
captive conditions. Social groups can be transient in nature and formed to
112 achieve a specific purpose.
Unit 16 Improving Animal Welfare through Social Conditions
Example: Migratory herds or flocks, and schools of fish.
The membership of these groups may be very fluid and the identity of the
individual members matters little to the others in the group. The effect of one
animal on another may be limited to one animal copying the direction of
movement of another, such as communal movement in a fish school in the
presence of a predator. Communication may therefore be limited to changes in
visual stimuli and the identity of the conspecifics is unknown and of little
relevance. At the opposite end of the spectrum lie complex societies, which
may persist indefinitely, whose membership is fairly stable over time and in
which the identity of individual members is of great importance.
Benefit Explanation
Efficient predator detection Surveillance for predators requires less individual effort
and is more effective when in a group.
Mutual predator defence Where an individual may fail to repel a predator, a group
may succeed.
Prey saturation This works in two ways. At its simplest, a single predator
can only attack one animal and the likelihood of being
the target is reduced when many other potential targets
are present. Prey saturation also occurs when females
synchronise oestrus and young are born during a short
window of time and the local predator population is
unable to attack all.
Efficient food acquisition For predators, the capture of prey is easier with help.
For all animals, locating a food resource is easier when
animals search together.
Group living brings with it challenges that are not faced by an independent
individual. Principal amongst these challenges is the need to divide limited
resources between group members. Societal rules are adopted which
minimise conflict as much as possible. These include the protection and
respect of an individual’s personal space (that space around it which cannot
be entered without causing withdrawal or defence) and the formation and
respect of dominance relationships.
SAQ 1
i) What are the advantages of living in a social group?
ii) What is the principal challenge that group living brings that is not faced
by an independent individual?
Fig. 16.1: Commercial flock of turkeys with several thousand group size
Fig. 16.2: Aggression between pigs of similar RHP (photo credit M. Farish,
SRUC)
Lack of substrate to
direct foraging
behaviour towards
Non-social causes
(e.g. dietary
imbalance, poor
climate, infection
Fig. 16.3: Diagram illustrating how chronic social stress from competition for
limited resources can lead to abnormal behaviour (tail-biting) and
social stress 117
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
Competition for food and space are known to increase the risk of these
behaviours occurring. Furthermore, a disproportionate number of smaller, less
competitive individuals tend to perform redirected behaviour since they are
more likely to experience social stress and to be unable to cope with the
combination of a challenging physical and social environment.
Table 16.2: Constraints that prevent the use of naturalistic social group
structures in commercial farming.
Constraint Outcome
Mechanical equipment is best The use of machinery for feeding and cleaning is
suited to large groups more compatible with unnaturally large groups.
Activity 1 (Visit): Visit a nearby Zoo and choose an animal species with which
you are quite familiar. Study the social environments (like composition of
group, demographics, similarity in competitive ability, sudden forced
introduction and inability to disperse etc). Write your observations on normal or
abnormal social environments.
SAQ 2
i) Differentiate the characteristics of wild social groups and captive groups.
iv) List the constraints that prevent the use of naturalistic social group
structures in commercial farming.
The tendency of captive animals to revert to the social strategies of their wild
counterparts is strong. Consequently, reference can be made to the wild
behavioural ecology of a species to predict the conditions most likely to lead to
social harmony. In progressive zoos, effort is made to create groups with an
appropriate demographic mix of ages and sexes. On farms this is problematic
as it counteracts the benefits of housing animals together of similar size and
age. This has not stopped research that seeks to find a practical way to
introduce wild grouping strategies onto commercial farms. In pigs, family pen
systems that allow adult females and their litters to freely integrate shortly after
birth mean that grouping litters together when older and when aggression is
more damaging is no longer essential. This also mimics the wild strategy of
integrating nonlittermates at a very young age when aggression is less
damaging. To date, this family pen system has not been widely adopted by
commercial pig producers due to the increased space needed and more
variable weaning weight. The evidence would indicate that social stress can
be minimised on farms by housing livestock in groups that approximate those
of their wild counterparts, but there is some way to go before practical and
economic constraints that limit their uptake can be overcome.
Providing young animals with the opportunity to gain social skills early in life is
worth exploring (Box 16.1). 119
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
Box 16.1: Opportunity to Gain Social Skills Early in Life to Young
Animals
Appropriate and rich early life experiences are instrumental in many species
achieving mature forms of adult behaviour. Establishment of social
relationships is a cognitively demanding task and likely to benefit in many
species from the opportunity to engage in social contact with new animals
when young and practice social skills in a non-damaging context. This may
be one of the key benefits of play fighting and why this behaviour is seen
commonly in the young of some species. In pigs where damaging aggression
in older animals is particularly routine, early life social contact may be
particularly valuable as a way to minimise later aggression. Here the
opportunity to freely meet unfamiliar piglets from an adjacent litter around two
weeks of age eases the establishment of dominance relationships when they
are next grouped with unfamiliar pigs after weaning. It is likely that the
opportunity to engage in play and especially play fighting may speed the
acquisition of the skills needed to assess RHP.
• Firstly they provide the loser with the chance to avoid the winner, and
• Genotype, and
• Past and current environment.
The genotype can therefore affect the social behaviour shown by an individual,
which has direct consequences for others in the group. Over past decades
evidence has grown that many captive environments cause social stress but
management change has not been adopted due to economic or practical
constraints. Where progress is lacking, it ought to be technically feasible to
reduce the expression of harmful social behavioural traits through selective
breeding, as those traits examined to date are under some degree of genetic
control. (Unit 18 will discuss in more detail the evidence for this genetic
determination, the likely consequences of breeding for behavioural change,
the practical manner in which it could be applied and the ethical considerations
of doing so).
16.5 SUMMARY
Let us summarise whatever you learnt so far:
3. What is meant by the term ‘resource holding potential’ (RHP) and list
some common attributes that affect RHP.
16.7 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) The advantages of living in a social group includes: social learning;
social support; efficient predator detection; mutual predator
defence; prey saturation; efficient food acquisition, and energy
conservation.
ii) Principal amongst these challenges is the need to divide limited
122 resources between group members.
Unit 16 Improving Animal Welfare through Social Conditions
iii) Dominance hierarchy can be defined as a social ranking system
within a group of the same species in which certain forms of status
and privilege are held by those ranking at the top, usually the
stronger or more aggressive members.
v) The attributes that determine RHP are varied, but can be physical
(e.g. body size and strength), determined by knowledge level (and
therefore often related to age) or symbolic through the display of
‘status symbols’.
ii) The family pen system has not been widely adopted by
commercial pig producers due to the increased space needed and
more variable weaning weight, besides practical and economic
constraints.
Terminal Questions
1. Refer to Subsections 16.2.1 and 16.2.2.
2. Refer to Subsections 16.3.1 and 16.3.2.
3. Refer to Subsections 16.2.3 and 16.3.1.
4. Refer to Subsection 16.2.3.
SUGGESTED READING
1. Broom, D.M. and Fraser, A.F. (2015). Domestic Animal Behaviour and
Welfare. CABI International (This book gives some excellent examples
of how captive environments lead to behavioural changes that can harm
welfare. It also gives a good summary of many of the basic concepts in
the field of social behaviour).
2. Keeling, J.L. Social Behaviour in Farm Animals (2001). CAB
International (This book summarises the costs and benefits of group
living and how social behaviour has evolved and responded to
domestication. It then gives a detailed description of the social behaviour
of several of the major farm species including fish and horses).
124
UNIT 17
IMPROVING ANIMAL WELFARE
THROUGH HUMAN CONTAC
CONTACT
Structure
17.1 Introduction Farm Management
Objectives Animal Research
17.2 Influence of Human Contact 17.6 Methods for Improving
on Animal Welfare Human Animal
Predictability Relationships
17.1 INTRODUCTION
Any animal under human care depends for their survival, health and welfare
on the quantity and quality of the care given by the owner or other involved
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
persons. Feed, water, shelter and veterinary care are essential elements that
should be provided by the owner for any animal in captivity. However, even if
these needs are met, the quality of the interaction between the human and
animal can hugely impact the welfare of the animal under care.
Humans are part of the animal kingdom as much as other animals. However,
note that the word ‘animal’ here refers to non-human animals.
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
b) Practical and Professional Skills: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
• Survival
• Health and
• Welfare
For captive animals the owner or other involved persons (care takers) have
the capacity to determine the animals’ trust, including their day-to-day welfare.
The animals’ welfare will thus strongly depend on the actions of the involved
persons, and the effect of their negative and positive actions is discussed in
detail in the following sections. Here, we first discuss the basis of what makes
an action positive or negative.
One reason for why humans may treat animals badly is because they do not
perceive their actions as negative or harmful for the animal or they are
unaware of what it is that causes a negative response in the animal. Animals’
(including human) behavioural and physiological responses are strongly
influenced by the extent to which they have predictability and controllability. A
lack of predictability and controllability, even in positive situations, can
increase behavioural and physiological stress up to a pathological state.
17.2.1 Predictability
Predictability, or ‘the ability to predict’ what is going to happen, allows the
individual to prepare or anticipate on what is going to come.
Example: If the owner of a dog is always nice when he comes home and
greets the dog, the animal will run towards the door and show a positive
behavioural response to the human. If the owner is always angry when arriving
home and he kicks the dog badly, then the animal will run away and hide as
soon as he hears the owner. However, if the owner sometimes behaves nicely
upon returning home but sometimes unexpectedly kicks the dog then the
animal will not know when to approach or when to hide. As a consequence,
the animal is likely to experience more stress than a dog which is always
kicked.
Research experiments in rats showed that rats that are given electric shocks
at irregular times developed severe gastric ulcers. However, when the rat
could predict the occurrence of the electric shock, by seeing a light flashing up
just before receiving the shock, they had fewer ulcers. When the action of the
human is predictable, the receiver of the behaviour (the animal) can prepare in
the best manner to sustain its health and welfare. This preparation may
include safeguarding itself (e.g. hiding) from receiving harmful behaviour or
relaxation when having the certainty of knowing that feed will be given every
day. This preparation not only affects behaviour and welfare but also closely
relates to physiology and thus health. If an individual knows when it will be
able to eat and drink, the metabolism adjusts to these timings to optimize
digestion, resulting in a better uptake of nutrients. 127
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
17.2.2 Controllability
In the above mentioned example of the rat the least amount of ulcers
developed when the rats where given the opportunity to control the duration of
the electric shocks. These rats would see, as in the above scenario, a light
flashing up when an electrical shock was about to come but they could then
stop the electric shock sooner by operating a lever. Rats that received the
same amount and duration of electric shocks, but that could not control the
shock themselves, showed severe gastric ulcers whereas the animals that
could control the shock even outperformed the control animals that received
no shocks at all. The receipt of electric shocks, as was done in this 1970s
research experiment, is quite severe. Let’s look at some relevant examples of
a lack of controllability in which no direct physical harm is done to the animals.
Caged pets and zoo animals have often no control over when humans, or
other animals, will interact with them. They cannot physically escape the
physical contact, gaze (eye contact) or presence of humans. This may result in
profound stress even though the animal may be physically well cared for.
Example: A well-cared for rabbit that is kept in a cage that is located in a room
with a cat right next to the cage may experience constant stress due to the
constant exposure to a natural predator.
Zoo animals often cannot hide from the constant presence of humans. When
given the opportunity to hide (through proper housing facilities), the animals
often make frequent use of these hiding places, to the disadvantage of the zoo
as visitors are unable to see the animal during parts or the majority of the day
Activity 2 (Visit): If possible, visit a zoo and observe the animals’ behaviour
to the presence of humans. Do they have the possibility to hide from contact
if they wanted to? Write your observations with implications to welfare.
SAQ 1
i) How can unpredictable negative handling by humans result in
fearfulness in the animal?
It is important to realize that not all animals that are showing difficult
behaviour, fear or aggression are being treated badly. Note that behaviours
may also arise from other factors, such as genetics or previous experiences.
Also, animals that are not showing any of these behaviours but that are
treated badly are not necessarily treated that way on purpose. As discussed in
Unit 4, humans may treat animals in a similar way as they have been treated,
whereby they may perceive some actions (such as beating) as normal.
Therefore do not make uninformed judgments based on human-animal
interactions that you may see and be cautious if you want to address a
situation in which you see an animal being maltreated.
• Species
• Breed
• Genetics
• Life history.
There are three ways in which fear may develop as a consequence of the
human animal relationship (or the lack thereof):
a) Active Negative Handling: The most apparent way in which fear can
develop is through active negative handling. 129
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
Example: Physical abuse (e.g. beating, kicking).
c) Neglect: Neglect, or ‘the state of being uncared for’, does not provide
the animal with the opportunity to fight for better care. Instead, it can
only wait for help or try to find alternative solutions to obtain food or
attention.
Examples: Facial expressions that signal anger, such as raising the upper lip,
showing teeth and tightening the eyes, and sound such as growling.
Activity 3 (Review): List for yourself what you consider negative handling.
Are there in the above examples situations that you would not consider as
negative? Write your observations with implications to welfare.
SAQ 2
i) What are the negative effects that suboptimal human-animal interactions
can have despite good intentions?
v) Write the three ways in which fear may develop as a consequence of the
human-animal relationship?
Whereas solitary species would move away from contact, group living species
strongly respond to social interaction and have a need for social attention.
Although positive contact is beneficial for any species, social species are more
affected by it. As discussed previously about neglect, healthy animals may
suffer mentally and physically if their social needs are neglected. Positive
contact therefore directly contributes to the welfare of the majority of the
domesticated social species through the following ways:
a) Play behaviour
SAQ 3
i) Positive contact contributes to the welfare of domesticated social
species. Comment.
ii) How can social bonds reduce or eliminate the effects of negative
experiences?
a) Veterinary practice
b) Farm management
c) Animal research
Despite this limited contact, the few moments of contact can greatly impact on
the behaviour of the animals. If the stock workers approach the animals
unexpectedly, roughly and loudly, and if handling is rough and related to stress
and pain, then the animals can become fearful of humans and more fearful in
general. A ‘Human-Approach Test’ is a standardized test to scientifically
assess the fearfulness of animals to human presence, and can be carried out
in farm facilities. Fearful animals are more difficult to handle, which may lead
to injuries or bruises for both handler and animal. Stockmanship can be
improved by acting calmly and in line with the species perception of the
environment.
Having a main caretaker increases the opportunity for the animals to form a
trusting bond with the caretaker.
SAQ 4
i) How can the relationship between the vet and the animal be improved?
SAQ 5
i) Write three practical science-based methods for improving human-
animal relationship.
iii) How can gentle physical contacts improve human animal relationships?
c) Suddenly approaching the animal from the back, may scare the animal.
Example: Making yourself look larger is a cue for showing that you are
stronger or dominant.
SAQ 6
i) Give examples for prey species.
ii) Write the behavioural signals that can be perceived by prey animal as
threat and should therefore be avoided.
17.8.1 Attacks
Animals that feel threatened may defend themselves or their offspring by
attacking. As discussed earlier, animals usually signal their intent in various
ways such as facial expression, body posture and sound. If these signals are
ignored then the animal may attack. Humans, including children, unaware of
the threat signals of animals will have a greater chance of being involved in an
attack. In particularly children, keen to approach and touch animals, may be
vulnerable to being injured when the animal tries to defend itself. Teaching
children about the signals that animals make is a more effective way of dealing
with this than keeping them away from animals (e.g. see the ladder of
aggression for dog behaviour). Attacks of wild or captive wild animals can
happen for the same reason, when initial threat signals are ignored. However,
wild animals may also attack humans if the human violates the animal’s
territory or forms a threat to the animal or its offspring. Wild animals usually try
to avoid aggression towards humans, this is generally the last resort of animal
that feels threatened or under attack. Trying to avoid surprising a wild animal
(for example by making noises if walking in forests where wild animals live)
and withdrawing or allowing the animal an escape route if unexpectedly
encountering a wild animal can help to reduce human-wild animal conflicts.
Another less frequent form of attack is seen where wild animals kill humans as
prey, which might occur if the wild animal is injured and less able to hunt other
prey species. Captive wild animals, and to some exten domestic animals, may
also attack out of fear and panic when being handled.
Examples: Bull fights in Spain, Cock fights in South India, Taji dolphin drive
hunt in Japan.
Activity 8 (Identification): Identify and list any cultural events in the region
where you live (country or state) in which animals are being maltreated,
injured or killed as a ritual or tradition. Write your opinion about such events.
SAQ 7
i) What are the usual signals given by animals before attacking?
17.9 SUMMARY
Let us summarise what you have learnt so far:
• Animals will show various signals through their body language, facial
expression and sound. Paying attention to these signals will improve the
mutual communication.
• Prey species will show different signals and give a different response to
situations than predator species.
• Knowing the differences, and why animals respond the way they do,
help us in understanding their behaviour and enables to handle them in
a manner then will increase trust. Human-animal conflicts may arise if
animal signals are not understood or are ignored, in which case the
animal may attack.
• Animals may also injure or kill humans unintentionally when they are in
panic or when kept in a confined space.
• Human-animal conflict may also be injurious or fatal for the animal, for
example in traditional cultural events.
In the next Unit, we will discuss how to improve animal welfare through genetic
selection.
2. Explain the basic physiological pathway of how gentle positive touch can
result in better growth.
4. Write three important signals that dogs give as a threat before they
attack.
17.11 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) Unpredictable negative handling by humans can result in
fearfulness in the animal from physical contact, such as physical
abuse, as well as from nonphysical contact through negative
verbal and body language or neglect.
ii) When the action of the human is predictable, the receiver of the
behaviour (the animal) can prepare in the best manner to sustain
its health and welfare. This preparation may include safeguarding
itself (e.g. hiding) from receiving harmful behaviour or relaxation
when having the certainty of knowing that feed will be given every
day. This preparation not only affects behaviour and welfare but
also closely relates to physiology and thus health.
vi) The most apparent way in which fear can develop is through active
negative handling (Physical abuse like beating, kicking). Negative
behaviour may have a strong component of unpredictability and
uncontrollability for the receiver. A high unpredictability and
uncontrollability are likely to result in fear behaviour, even in the
absence of direct negative behaviour from the human.
4. i) The relationship between the vet and the animal can be improved
by making the intervention as fast and painless as possible and by
comforting the animal with – to the species appropriate – contact.
Letting the owner stay with the animal may reduce the animal’s
fear in case of a good relationship.
ii) Predictability and controllability are key factors for good welfare
and thus contribute to a good relationship. Predictability can be
increased by giving structure to the day, with set meal times and
care. Being predictable in your behaviour towards the animal can
Providing the animal with a certain degree of controllability can
make it easier for the animal to fulfill its needs. reduce stress and
can increase the bond.
iii) Examples for wild predator species are tigers, lions and
domesticated predator species dogs, cats, and mink.
Terminal Questions
1. Refer to Section 17.2.
SUGGESTED READING
1) Grandin, T., and Johnson, C. (2005). Animals in translation: Using the
mysteries of autism to decode animal behaviour. New York: Scribner.
3) Websites:
4) https://www.bsavalibrary.com/content/cil/canine-and-feline-behaviour/
ladder_of_aggression
143
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
UNIT 18
IMPROVING ANIMAL WELFARE
THROUGH GENETIC SELECTION
Structure
18.1 Introduction Examples of Undesirable
Effects of Breeding
Objectives
Reversing Undesirable Effects
18.2 The Power of Animal
of Breeding
Breeding
18.5 Using Breeding to Improve
Selective Breeding
Welfare
The Structure of the Animal
Barriers to Selection: Ethical
Breeding Industry
Considerations
18.3 Genetic Versus Non-
Barriers to Selection: Practical
Genetic Determination
Considerations
18.4 Undesirable Effects for
18.7 Summary
Welfare of Past Breeding
Strategies 18.8 Terminal Questions
Causes of Undesirable Effects 18.9 Answers
on Welfare
18.1 INTRODUCTION
Domestication has shaped the characteristics of species for thousands of
years and more recent selective breeding has dramatically increased the rate
of change from the ancestral state. Those features which are the target of
improvement through genetic selection, such as growth rate, are referred to as
‘traits’. Through the process of domestication, the focus of informal breeding
was on reducing the fearfulness and aggressiveness of species towards
humans. More recent structured breeding programmes focus on improving
traits of economic importance (in livestock), convey an aesthetic quality (e.g. in
dog breeding) or allow scientific discovery (e.g. in strains of mice developed as
models of human disease).
The discussion is relevant to all captive species where humans influence the
transmission of genetic information but many of the examples will be from
animal husbandry and agriculture due to the very large number of animals
involved and the scale of the breeding industry that supports their genetic
improvement.
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
b) Practical and Professional Skills: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
• describe how breeding can have benefits for welfare but also the
barriers that need to be overcome to achieve this.
Broiler Chicken: One of the best examples of the power of selective breeding
is the change in growth rate of broiler (i.e. meat) chickens. Selection over the
past 50 years has been extremely successful, increasing growth rate from 25
to 100 g per day, meaning that birds currently reach slaughter weight in
around 6 weeks.
Pig Breeding: The global pig breeding industry has also had major success in
improving growth rate. Here, the annual improvement in growth rate is around
5 g/pig/day which may seem unimpressive given that a pig may grow in
excess of 1kg per day. However, summed over many generations of selection,
the change means that pigs now grow several times faster than they did a few 145
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
decades ago. Even over shorter time spans, the improvement can be large.
For example, the UK has seen a 21% improvement in the weight of meat
produced from each sow per year over only a 5 year period.
Dairy Cows: Vast increases have also been achieved in milk production from
dairy cows where the highest genetic merit cows now produce in excess of
12,000 litres of milk per year. Over longer time periods, changes can be truly
profound and such changes need to be supported by improvements in
nutrition and management (e.g. a high genetic merit cow can only achieve
high yields of milk if fed appropriately).
It is then possible to screen animals for their genetic makeup (their ‘genotype’)
at each of these points to find those whose genome most positively influences
a trait. As costs fall of determining the base-pair sequence of DNA, animal
breeding is also likely to make use of actual sequence data for particularly
important genes.
The domestic species however differ substantially in which traits are selected
upon, to what degree and the methods by which this is achieved.
Consequently, the amount of genetic progress in ‘improving’ a species from its
ancestral state is much greater in some species than in others. In the pig and
poultry industries, large, multinational breeding organizations supply elite
genetic material to farmers and the performance of their offspring in each trait
of interest is measured and compared on dedicated ‘nucleus’ farms. These
industries are also at the cutting edge of developing and using new breeding
technologies to increase the accuracy and speed of response to selection. The
dairy industry is not far behind these others in embracing new technology.
These industries contrast with the sheep sector where breed societies manage
the registration of elite animals but the elite animals themselves and their
offspring are managed on normal farms. To date, the use of molecular genetic
information has been slow in this industry. Even here, however, the uptake of
modern genetic selection methods such as genomic selection is increasing.
Companion animal breeding shows most similarity with sheep breeding in that
breeding occurs mostly at a local level but elite animals are registered by a
146 breed society.
Unit 18 Improving Animal Welfare through Genetic Selection
Fast rate of
genetic
improvement
Fig. 18.1: Factors determining how quickly a trait responds to selective breeding
Some traits respond to breeding more rapidly than others. Within any species,
this speed of progress is determined principally by four factors (Fig. 18.1), plus
the generation interval of the species (how quickly a new-born animal itself
becomes a parent):
SAQ 1
i) What is meant by traits?
v) What is Phenotyping?
SAQ 2
i) What is meant by genotype?
Selection of dog breeds, and more recently cat breeds, for particular aesthetic
characteristics, such as short noses relative to the skull length, small size,
shorter limbs and long backs, have also resulted in undesirable welfare-related
traits. For example short-nosed or ‘flat-faced’ breeds of dog are prone to
respiratory and thermoregulatory distress, long backed dogs experience spinal
weakness and paralysis and dogs bred for short limbs can experience arthritis
and other joint problems.
Udder Lifespan
7% 14% Production
32%
Mastitis
9%
Fertility
20%
Feet and legs
6%
Fig.18.2: Relative weight placed on traits within the Profitable Lifetime Index
used to breed dairy cows
SAQ 3
i) Write the causes of undesirable genetic effects on welfare.
Piglet mortality is close to 20%, including still-births, and has remained at this
high level without improvement for a long time. Past selection strategies have
focussed on breeding sows to produce the maximum number of piglets born.
Piglet birth weight fell as a consequence of this which increased the mortality
rate of piglets during the first one or two days of life. Adjusting selection criteria
to include neonatal survival, in addition to number born, is a more sustainable
strategy and one that has allowed litter size to be increased without increasing
the proportion of piglets that die.
The Danish pig industry has pioneered this approach and now has extremely
large litters (Fig. 18.3). It has achieved an increase of 2.3 pigs weaned/ litter in
the last 10 years. To actually reduce the proportion of piglets that die would
require greater selection pressure to be placed on survival (i.e. making it a
higher priority relative to other traits under selection). Different strategies could
also be taken, such as reducing the variation in birth weight within a litter.
Currently large differences exist between the smallest and largest piglets
within a litter and the smallest often die as they fail to compete for access to
milk. Canalised selection programmes aim to reduce variation in a trait and
could improve survival by removing from the population particularly small and
large piglets. Selecting for improved placental efficiency might also improve
birth weight and help to reduce the proportion of piglets that die. A major
cause of piglet mortality is crushing by the mother as she lies down. A minority
of mothers are responsible for the majority of crushing deaths with some killing
31% of their piglets. Breeding for improved maternal behaviour therefore could
have a major role to play in improving piglet survival.
Fig. 18.3: Selective breeding for large litter size in pigs has been highly
successful but tends to create a large number of small and weak
piglets
• Welfare traits are often costly and difficult to measure (e.g. health traits
and redirected behaviour where it is easier to identify the victims than
the perpetrators).
Fig. 18.4: Practical barriers that make it difficult to improve a welfare trait
through selective breeding
From the discussion above it should come as no surprise that few welfare
traits are currently being improved by selective breeding even though many
technically could be. It is also evident that those which are under selection
have a clear welfare impact but also a clear economic impact. So far, this
means that survival and health traits that majorly constrain productivity have
been the focus of attention. It is likely that selection on other traits will become
more feasible in the future as the automation of phenotyping becomes
possible and the costs of technology for exploiting molecular genetic
information fall (such as genomic selection which does not require routine
phenotyping).
SAQ 4
i) How may breeding help to alleviate significant welfare problems that
have existed for many decades?
ii) Write any two ethical considerations in genetic selection.
18.7 SUMMARY
Let us summarise what you have learnt so far:
• Animal breeding has proven to be a highly effective way of maximising
the benefits that humans derive from animals, whether these are
economic, cultural or aesthetic.
• Genetic change has been achieved at different rates and by different
means in different species. The application of modern breeding
techniques is likely to accelerate the improvement of traits under
selection.
• In most cases, traits that affect welfare are heritable and often to a
higher degree than economically important traits that are currently the
focus of selective breeding.
• We have seen many examples of how breeding can have negative
effects on welfare in all forms of captive species. Inclusion of welfare
traits in breeding strategies has in some cases reversed (at least in part)
the negative effects of past selection. 155
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
• Many other traits cause serious welfare problems and have become a
tolerated and routine aspect of the way that we manage animals. These
traits could be improved substantially by placing selection pressure on
them.
3. Give examples of the way in which past selection strategies have had a
negative effect on animal welfare.
18.9 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) The features which are the target of improvement through genetic
selection, such as growth rate, are referred to as ‘traits’.
3. i) Genes often have pleiotropic effects, meaning that one gene can
influence the expression of several traits at the same time even if
the traits appear to be unrelated. Two genes with different effects
(e.g. one affecting a production trait and one affecting a welfare
trait) can also be inherited together if located in close proximity on
the chromosome. This means that breeding for an economic or
aesthetic trait has the potential to lead to desirable or undesirable
genetic change in other traits of welfare significance.
Terminal Questions
SUGGESTED READING
1. Rauw, W.M., Kanis, E., Noordhuizen-Stassen, E.N. and Grommers, F.J.
(1998). Undesirable side effects of selection for high production
efficiency in farm animals: a review. Livestock Production Science, 56:
15-33 (This paper published in a peer reviewed journal gives the most
comprehensive description of the welfare traits that have been harmed
by selective breeding for productivity).
2. Rodenburg, T.B. and Turner, S.P. (2012). The role of breeding and
genetics in the welfare of farm animals. Animal Frontiers, 2: 16-21 (This
paper is also published in a peer review journal and is open access. It
describes how far we have come in improving welfare in the pig and
poultry industries by using breeding).
4. https://dairy.ahdb.org.uk/technical-information/breeding-
genetics/%C2%A3pli/
158
Unit 19 Animal Ethics and Sentience
UNIT 19
ANIMAL ETHICS AND
SENTIENCE
Structure
19.1 Introduction 19.5 Overview of Animal
Sentience
Objectives
What is Sentience?
19.2 What is Ethics?
Why is Sentience Important
Personal Ethics
in Animal Welfare?
Professional Ethics
19.6 Scientific Evidence for
Organisational Ethics Animal Sentience
19.3 Ethical Views or Theories Evidence from Neuroscience
Contractarian View Evidence from Behavioural
Utilitarianism Science
19.1 INTRODUCTION
In previous Units you have learnt different conceptions of animal welfare and
the scientific methods we can use to measure and describe animal welfare.
We have also covered how animal welfare can be assessed, the different
steps that are required, and the methods that can be used to improve animal
welfare.
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
b) Practical and Professional Skills: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
1) Personal ethics
2) Professional ethics
In both cases, these set out the ethical view of the profession and should
supersede any personal ethical viewpoints if they conflict with these
statements when at working as a veterinarian.
SAQ 1
i) What is meant by ethics?
a) Personal Ethics:
b) Professional Ethics:
c) Organisational Ethics:
162
Unit 19 Animal Ethics and Sentience
1) Contractarian view
2) Utilitarian view
This view suggests that being moral is in your own self-interest. By showing
consideration for the feelings or positions of others, we are really behaving in a
way that is for our own sake. Part of these arguments suggest that only human
are truly able to make contracts, and as animals cannot, then people only
need to treat them as well as is needed for them to be used effectively. This
moral position suggests that any animal use for human benefit, such as food,
new medical treatment or financial gain (such as entertainment) is acceptable.
This view only protects animals where they matter to people, and might confer
greater protection on an animal that is important to humans, rather than for its
own sake.
In reality this ethical view might explain how the same species (for example a
rabbit) might be treated differently, both legally and in terms of accepted
approaches, if it is a pet rabbit, a food rabbit, a laboratory rabbit or a wild
rabbit often considered a pest in some environments. The rabbit is still a rabbit
in all situations, but the view of the rabbit’s value changes depending on which
situation the rabbit is from the perspective of human benefit.
19.3.2 Utilitarianism
The key principle of this view is: ‘Morality is about maximising human and
animal well-being’.
This ethical view can be considered as legal rights (which are created and
exist within legal systems) and moral rights (which are not created by law).
Although rights were originally seen as being something akin to ‘moral status’
and only held by humans, some philosophers have extended this view to
animals as well. They argue that rights should not be just based on biological
arguments (such as belonging to the species Homo sapiens), but should be
extended to all animals possessing particular capacities or capabilities (such
as sentience). Supporters of animal rights can span a range of differing
positions concerning which capacities that animals must have to have rights,
and which rights should be extended to animals. For some supporters of this
ethical view, such as Tom Regan (a well-known philosopher of animal rights),
all creatures that are sentient should have equal rights and inherent value,
regardless of their use to others. This view then explicitly contradicts the
utilitarian view that suggests that some animals can suffer if this is for the
greater good, and the total sum of benefit is greater than the amount of
suffering. For animal rights supporters ‘the disrespectful treatment of the
individual in the name of social good [is] something the rights view will
not…ever allow’ (Regan, 1989, ‘The Case for Animal Rights’).
This ethical view addresses how to be a morally virtuous person in the way we
behave towards animals, where virtue can be considered to be in possession
of a consistent and persistent set of dispositions to think, behave, judge and
feel in the right way. This may lead to feelings of mercy towards animals, or
lead people to behave with compassion, temperance and honesty instead of
callousness, cruelty or greed. Unlike some other ethical views, such as
utilitarianism or rights views, a significant role for emotions is allowed, as well
as reason. This view can also be related to the ethics of care – where it can be
considered wrong to harm animals not because it will cause more suffering (as
with the utilitarian view) or because the animal has rights which will be violated
(the rights view) but because harming the animal reflects a lack of care or an
inappropriate emotional response in the person doing the harming. These
ethical views are considered contextual views of our ethical responsibilities
towards animals, and a specific area of ethical thinking in this area is the
164 relational view, which will be discussed next.
Unit 19 Animal Ethics and Sentience
19.3.5 The Relational View
The key principle of this view is: ‘Morality grows out of our relationship with
animals and one another’
This is a relatively modern ethical view that considers that the right thing to do
is about the relationships that we have with others, including animals. A key
part of this is about respect, and this branch of ethics is often used in health
care and clinical practice, although it can also apply to our treatment of
animals. This places an importance on the relationships between animals and
humans, and thus our duties towards animals depend on whether they are
close to us or not, or whether they are under our care or not (such as
comparing domestic animals to wildlife). This is similar to virtue ethics
(discussed in 19.3.4) but goes further in encouraging close relationships
between humans and animals, and that responsibilities lie in the closeness of
those relationships. Thus, following this view, a relational theorist might argue
that our pet dog should be treated better than an animal on a farm or in a
laboratory (at least by the owner) as the relationship is stronger. In addition,
we may have a greater moral obligation towards domestic animals, as we
have to some extent ‘created’ them through selective breeding, compared to
wild animals. Whilst this might well be what happens in practice, this does
mean that there might be categories of animals, such as wildlife pests, to
which no protection at all might be extended.
• The capacities of animals, and in particular the capacity for suffering and
sentience, have been key components of many of these ethical views.
It is now to the discussion of animal sentience that we will turn in the next
section.
Contractarian
Utilitarian
Animal rights
Virtue ethics
Relational
Respect for
nature
SAQ 2
i) Write the key ethical principle in the following views / theories:
ii) How the Contractarian ethical view differs from the Utilitarian view?
‘The question is not, can they reason, nor can they talk, but can they suffer?’
In some Eastern religions, such as Jainism, all matter, not just animals, is
endowed with sentience, and is compatible with Hinduism, Buddhism and
Sikhism that recognise non-human animals as sentient beings.
• In the European Union, the Treaty of Lisbon, which came into force in
2009, recognises that animals are sentient and mandates member
states to pay full regard to animal welfare when formulating policy.
Although animal sentience is not always well defined in the legislation, this has
allowed countries to explain what this should mean in practice. In New
Zealand, the recognition of animal sentience was considered to allow them to
‘set a new standard for society’s expectation of the way animal are treated by
focusing on positive welfare states and…enhancements.’ In Quebec, animals
were distinguished from property by stating that ‘animals are not things. They
are sentient beings and have biological needs’. In the European Parliament,
animals are now considered ‘living beings endowed with sensitivity, interests
of their own and dignity, that benefit from special protection’.
Thus, animal sentience is important for animal welfare because the very
definition of animal welfare rests on the ability of animals to experience
subjectively positive and negative emotional states, and because the
protections given to animals in law can be defined in terms of their sentience.
Cognitive or judgement bias – as with attention bias tests, this has also
developed from human studies, and stems from a tendency to be more
pessimistic when in a low mood state (classically this might be a propensity to
report that a glass is half empty rather than half full). Typically for these
studies in animals, animals are first trained to associate a tone, location, odour
172 or coloured symbol with a positive reward, and an opposing tone, location,
Unit 19 Animal Ethics and Sentience
odour or colour with a negative reward. Once animals have learned these
associations they may experience a negative treatment or be managed in a
barren environment and are then presented with the two stimuli again, as well
as intermediate ‘probe’ tones, locations, odours or colours. Animals that are
more pessimistic will judge the intermediate to be more likely to be associated
a ‘punishment’ and less likely to be associated with a reward. An example of
this is shown in Fig. 19.1. This is called a ‘go-no go’ trial as the dog first learns
that a dark colour is rewarded with a treat, whereas the pale colour is not. The
dog is considered to have learned the task when it always approaches the
dark colour, and does not approach the pale colour. In the test phase we want
to know if the dog will approach an intermediate colour or not. A dog that
approaches the middle tone might be more optimistic or more likely to be in a
positive mood state compared to a dog which does not approach.
(Left: Training phase –dog learns associations; Right: Testing phase –dogs that
approach the middle bowls are more optimistic than dogs that do not).
These types of behavioural testing procedures allow us to ask the animal very
complex questions, and the changes in behaviour in different contexts may
provide us with further evidence that supports an argument that animals are
sentient. Interestingly, birds, mammal and fish have been shown to be able to
do these complex behaviours, but some reptiles, amphibians, octopoda and
decapods can also show some of these responses, and bees have been
shown to respond in a similar way to mammals in cognitive bias testing. We do
not yet have definitive data that demonstrates sentience in a way that
everyone accepts. However, we do start to have very convincing evidence
accumulating that, on the balance of probabilities, most if not all vertebrates
are sentient, and this may also extend to other orders of animals as well.
SAQ 3
i) What is meant by sentience?
19.7 SUMMARY
Let us summarise what you have learnt so far:
• Ethical views can be our personal ethics, but may also be influenced by
professional or organisational ethical views.
• The first five of these ethical views consider the suffering and sentience
of animal to be important but the respect for nature view tends to focus
on animals at the level of a species.
3. Briefly describe the six main ethical views relating to our treatment of
animals.
19.9 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) Ethics are a set of moral reactions or principles that serve as a
guide for a person’s actions.
iii) (a) Personal Ethics: Our own personal views on what is right or
wrong (e.g some people might find hunting, or eating meat, morally
unacceptable whereas others do not). (b) Professional Ethics:
Accepted codes of practice held by a particular group of people or
professions (e.g. Veterinarians swear an oath on joining their
profession with ethical or moral principles by which the practice of
their profession is governed). (c) Organisational Ethics: Some
organisations or companies may also have ethical or moral
principles referred to as organisational ‘values’ or social
responsibilities (e.g. a food retailer that might trade on having
higher levels of animal welfare than other retailers).
ii) The Contractarian view suggests that being moral is in your own
self-interest and suggests that any animal use for human benefit,
such as food, new medical treatment or financial gain (such as
entertainment) is acceptable. This view only protects animals
where they matter to people, and might confer greater protection 175
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
on an animal that is important to humans, rather than for its own
sake. The Utilitarianism suggests that morality is about maximising
human and animal well-being. This theory is a well-known and
well-used approach to animal ethics, but is based on kindness
towards animals as its starting point, not self interest (as in the
Contractarian view).
3. i) Sentience is the capacity to feel, perceive or experience
subjectively.
ii) Animal sentience is important for animal welfare because the very
definition of animal welfare rests on the ability of animals to
experience subjectively positive and negative emotional states,
and because the protections given to animals in law can be
defined in terms of their sentience.
iii) Evidence for the existence of sentience is problematic because
sentience is defined as the subjective emotional experiences of an
animal. They are considered to be the private and unobservable
feelings. Some argue that this can never be accessible, whereas
others contend that it is possible to assess feelings with
developments in neuroscience. Further, it may be very unlikely we
can ever understand exactly what the animal may feel and whether
that is the same as we would ourselves feel.
iv) The most frequently studied feelings of animals are pain, fear, and
the basis of positive emotional states, such as play and
exploration.
Terminal Questions
1. Refer to Section 19.2.
2. Refer to Subsection 19.2.1 and 19.2.2.
3. Refer to Section 19.3.
4. Refer to Section 19.5.
5. Refer to Subsection 19.6.1.
6. Refer to Subsection 19.6.2.
7. Refer to Subsection 19.6.3.
SUGGESTED READING
1. Nielsen (2020). Asking Animals: An introduction to animal behaviour
testing. CABI, UK
2. Palmer & Sandoe (2018). Animal Ethics (Chapter 1) In: Animal Welfare,
3rd Edition, Ed. M.A. Appleby, A.S. Olsson & F. Galindo, CABI.
3. Wemelsefelder (2001). Assessing the whole animal: a free choice
profiling approach. Animal Behaviour 62, 209-220.
Useful websites:
https://www.ciwf.org.uk/farm-animals/animal-sentience/#start)
https://www.animal-ethics.org/different-ethical-theories/
176
Unit 20 Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-I
UNIT 20
ANIMAL WELFARE THEORIES
THEORIESI
Structure
20.1 Introduction Farming of Pigs for Meat
20.1 INTRODUCTION
In the previous Unit you were introduced to different ethical frameworks that
can be applied to animals, and were made aware that for many of these a
belief in animal sentience is a key part of this ethical view. We also discussed
some of the supporting evidence for animal sentience for different classes of
animal. You should now be aware that, although animal sentience is accepted
quite widely for some classes of animal (mammals and birds), and is accepted
in some circles for others (fish, reptiles), this is still a contested concept that is
not universal. Animal sentience cannot be definitively demonstrated by
scientific evidence, and thus acceptance of the weight of evidence is often the
dominant view of whether animals are sentient or not. However, the place of
animals in human society, and the moral status of animals are central to
animal welfare thinking. In this Unit, and the next, we will consider in more
depth the different ethical views / theories of animals. This initially involves 177
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
understanding the two over-arching philosophical approaches to animal ethics:
consequentialism and deontology, and then how this has influenced schools of
thought and applications in modern times.
In this unit we will consider consequential ethical approach and its application
to animal welfare with case studies.
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
b) Practical and Professional Skills: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
20.2 CONSEQUENTIALISM
Consequentialism, as the name suggests, is the belief that the morality of an
action can be understood by its consequence or impact. It is based on two
principles:
1) Whether an act is right or wrong depends only on the results of that act
2) The more good consequences an act produces the better or more right
that act is.
This view argues that there is no intrinsically moral way to behave and that is
might be acceptable to lie or otherwise behave in a way that may appear to be
immoral if the outcome is good. For example, it might be acceptable under this
approach to lie to a murderer about the location of the intended victim as the
consequence of lying (that the victim will live) is better than the consequence
of not lying (the victim is killed). Examples of consequentialism that you have
already come across are Contractarian, Utilitarianism and Hedonism. We will
discuss the first two of these in the light of our use of animals shortly.
SAQ 1
i) What is meant by consequentialism?
iii) How ‘Act consequentialism’ and ‘Rule consequentialism’ differ from each
other?
179
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
How does this sort of ethical view apply to animals? It is generally considered
by philosophers that non-human animals cannot make agreements, cannot
enter into contracts with us and do not have moral duties. Note that not all
people think like this. For example, there are some that consider that when we
work with or live with animals that are larger than us or potentially able to do
us harm we have entered into a contract with the animal that they will not harm
us in exchange for some other benefit (for example that we will feed, protect
and care for them). More widely though, we can bring non-human animals into
the Contractarian ethical view indirectly, by the moral obligations we might
have to the humans that own them. For example, if we have agreed to look
after a friend’s dog whilst they are away on holiday then the dog becomes
protected as part of our moral obligation based on the contract we have made
with the owners of the dog. We can also think of other ways in which animals
might be protected by our contracts with other humans or by the benefits that
accrue to us by taking good care of animals. We shall look at some cases
where this ethical viewpoint might be applied in the next section.
Let us now look at the use of a different group of animals, those used for
research purposes. In many countries, there are higher levels of legislation
governing the use of cats, dogs, monkeys and some other species in research
than there are for rats, mice and rabbits. This means that many more rodents
and rabbits are used in research facilities around the world, and relatively few
dogs, cats or monkeys. These species are all mammals, and might be
considered equally likely to be sentient compared to other species, but we
choose to restrict the number of species used because the general public
often objects to the use of specific groups of animals in research. At least
some species then benefit as they are rarely used in research experiments,
but the reason for this is not directly because of a special care for that animals
by researchers, but because of a contract between people that certain types of
animals might be deserving of better care.
Another example of this sort of ethical view might be where animal welfare of
research animals is improved because it leads to better quality research data.
As described in Unit 1, poor animal welfare can make scientific data more
variable, may lead to false outcomes that cannot be safely extrapolated to
human conditions and can hinder scientific advances. Again, the animals
might benefit by improved welfare, but this is because of the self-interests of
the scientist, who is motivated by creating better or more robust datasets, and
not because of an intrinsic interest in animal welfare or the lives of the
research animals.
Activity 1 (Case Study & Interaction): Read the Case Study 1 on Higher
Welfare Meat (Section 20.4.1). Interact with 10-15 meat consumers on their
willingness to pay more if they have access to higher welfare meat. Compare
their responses to the discussion on Contractarian approach to animal welfare
and write your observations:
181
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
SAQ 2
i) What is the key feature in contractarian ethical approaches?
Historically, this seems to have stemmed from Greek moral philosophy from
forms of Hedonism, proposed by Aristippus and Epicurus, and from Aristotle
who argued that eudaimonia (happiness, flourishing, prosperity or
blessedness are all proposed as translations) is the highest human good. A
number of Western philosophers have considered the application of this way
of thinking to human behaviour afterwards, with the creation of modern
Utilitarianism usually accredited to Jeremy Bentham in the late 1700s, and
modified by John Stuart Mill in the 1800s.
The foundation of this line of reasoning is in equality – that is that all animals
are equal, meaning that the interests of every being affected by an action
should be taken into account and given the same weight as the interests of
any other being. Often, in the application of Utilitarian ethical reasoning, the
interests of humans are considered to take priority over those of animals,
whereas the Animal Liberation line of thinking re-adjusts to consider all to be
equal. To Animal Liberationists, prioritizing human interests is considered
‘speciesism’, that is to provide special arguments for the rights of one species
(in this case humans) over another. This does not mean that animals ought to
have all the rights that we think humans should have, but argues for the
equality of consideration of interests. For example, animals ought to have an
equal right not to experience pain, at a level that matters to it, as a human
should. The action of inflicting pain may be different – for example slapping a
horse may not cause the amount of pain that slapping a child would, but hitting
a horse with a stick might inflict a similar amount of pain. From the Animal
Liberation view, these are both equivalent and if one is considered morally
wrong, then so must the other.
The goal of the animal liberation movement is to end the ‘speciesist’ bias
against animals by taking their interests more seriously than they are currently.
The focus, as utilitarianism, is on the amount of suffering and the numbers of
animals involved, hence the movement is most concerned about the use of
animals in experiments (which can involve millions of animals per year) and
those farmed for food, particularly in intensive systems (which can involve
billions of animals each year). Thus, using this reasoning, we might argue for a
very small amount of experimentation on animals where the outcomes would
improve the lives of many humans or animals, but against the vast majority of
studies, which have negligible impact on human well-being, or investigate
trivial issues (testing of cosmetics for example). Here the balance of suffering
versus benefit, or of pain versus pleasure, is weighed with both the human and
animal interests valued as having an equal consideration. Similar arguments
are made for the production of animals for food. Since many people like to eat
meat because they enjoy the taste or some other sensory aspect of eating
meat, but could be equally wellnourished by eating a non-animal or plant-
based meal then the modern animal production, particularly very intensive
agriculture, is problematic as it causes negative impacts on animal welfare,
with only a small benefit to humans. Following an Animal Liberation view, most 183
Block 4 Animal Welfare-II
animal experimentation and nearly all animal production would cease,
because the cost to the animals involved is not considered sufficiently
counterbalanced by the human benefits.
Modern pig farming in many countries is very intensive, with sows confined to
very small crates throughout gestation in many countries (this practice is
banned in some countries, but not in others), which prevents them from
turning around or interacting with other animals. Sows give birth to several
large litters, whilst in confinement and can suffer from leg weakness, lameness
and lesions from the low levels of activity. Their piglets are often kept in barren
housing, and may experience tail-docking, castration and tooth clipping all
without the use of anaesthesia. It would be reasonable to suppose that the
amount of suffering by the animals in these systems is quite significant. The
human benefit that derives from this is the ability to eat a meat product that
many consumers enjoy, for a low price so it is accessible to a large number of
people. From a utilitarian view this is problematic, as the negative effects on
the animals are not outweighed by significant human benefits. It does not
argue that the eating of meat is wrong, only that the balance of suffering to
benefits is not in favour of the benefits. By moving to a higher welfare farming
184 system, where sows are unconfined or able to forage outside, enriched or
Unit 20 Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-I
outdoor environments are used and surgical procedures are not carried out,
we might conclude that now the balance of suffering and benefits has shifted
to a more positive position, and eating meat under these circumstances is
acceptable.
Activity 2 (Case Study & Interaction): Average Life of a Male Chick in the
Egg Industry is One Day
At the layer-hen hatcheries, the sex of the newborn chicks is checked and
unwanted male chicks are killed when they are one day old. Around 7 billion
male chicks are killed / year (using gas chambers, mechanical machines,
manual killing, electrocution, stuffing in big plastic bags etc). The human
benefit that derives from killing male chicks is the ability of egg industry to
place 100 percent female chicks in layer farms, save costs and to produce
more eggs for a low price to the consumers.
Discuss the above case study with your friends and colleagues from a
utilitarian view on, (a) Balance of suffering to benefits, (b) Moving to a higher
welfare system and consuming eggs is more acceptable by shifting balance of
suffering and benefits to a more positive position (e.g. Technology to
determine chick’s sex long before hatching or technology to produce eggs with
female embryos only etc). Compare the outcome of the interaction with Case
Study on ‘Intensive Farming of Pigs for Meat (Section 20.5.3)’ and write your
observations:
SAQ 3
i) What is the essence of utilitarian ethical approach?
This view is quite different to the animal welfare views that we have so far
discussed, where the feelings of that animal and its suffering are important,
and perhaps most similar to a view of welfare that promotes naturalness. Here
it might be accepted that an individual animal suffers, but if this occurs within a
natural setting and is part of nature, such as the prolonged death of a hunted
animal by a predator, then this is acceptable, as ‘nature must take its course’.
Conservation of genetic resources and species, and maintaining biodiversity,
are inherent to this view of animals. When a species becomes extinct it is not
simply that there is a loss of resources, but the problem is the loss of the
species, which had an intrinsic value and this no longer exists.
Let us now consider how this might apply by considering some case studies:
Discuss the above with your friends and colleagues from Consequentialism
ethical dilemma perspective. Compare their opinions to the discussion given in
the above sections and write your observations:
SAQ 4
i) How is the ‘Respect for Nature View’ approach different from other
ethical approaches?
ii) What are the interventions to preserve species from extinction from
‘Nature Ethical View’ perspective?
20.7 SUMMARY
Let us summarise what you have learnt so far:
• Utilitarian ethics is a widely used ethical framework for animal use, that
looks to maximise happiness or benefits, at a minimal cost or
unhappiness.
• The respect of nature ethical view does not consider the individual as
relevant, but makes decisions based on preserving the species and its
188 integrity.
Unit 20 Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-I
• By considering different case studies, we saw that applying different
ethical views influences our decision-making, and that will affect whether
we view some uses of animals as right or wrong.
20.9 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) As the name suggests, consequentialism is the belief that the
morality of an action can be understood by its consequence or
impact.
Terminal Questions
1. Refer to Section 20.2.
SUGGESTED READING
190
Unit 21 Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-2
UNIT 21
ANIMAL WELFARE THEORIES
THEORIESII
Structure
21.1 Introduction 21.4 Practical Ethical Decision-
Making
Objectives
21.5 Summary
21.2 Deontological approaches
21.6 Terminal Questions
Animal Rights View
21.7 Answers
Case study 1: Farming Chickens
for Meat
Relational Ethics
21.1 INTRODUCTION
In the last Unit we discussed consequential ethical approaches and
understood how these were related to the outcomes of our actions, rather than
what we actually do. In these approaches the ends are seen to justify the
means and the main aim is to ensure that a good outcome is achieved from
our decision-making. However, even within this framework, there are several
schools of thought about how this can be applied and, as we investigated with
a series of case studies, these can lead to different thinking and outcomes for
animals depending on which ethical view we apply.
Objectives
a) Knowledge and Understanding: After studying this Unit you will be
able to:
This philosophical position believes that you cannot justify an immoral action
by showing that it produced good consequences (whereas consequentialism
would argue there is no such thing as a moral or immoral action). Thus, people
have a duty to act according to the rights and wrongs of the act, regardless of
the consequences. This view is most famously associated with the 18th
Century philosopher Immanuel Kant, who believed that it was not morally
acceptable to tell a lie, even if this would save someone from murder (we used
as an example in Unit 20). So, in comparison to Consequentialists who would
consider what things are good and then identify the right actions to achieve
that, Deontologists would first consider the right actions and then proceed from
there (Box 21.1).
It emphasizes respect and can provide the basis for human rights or
other rights (as we will discuss in Section 21.3.1), and thus the rights of
a single individual are given due regard even if those are at odds with
the interests of a larger group.
It also states that some acts are always wrong, and for these acts the
ends can never justify the means that might achieve them. From an
animal use point of view we might feel that it is never acceptable to carry
out a procedure on some types of living animal (such as a great ape)
that will cause persistent and untreatable pain, regardless of the good to
human medicine or health that may ensue (as is present in the laws of
some countries, but note that not all people might agree).
It can provide certainty, as actions are always the same because they
are not reliant on predicting future outcomes.
It also deals with intentions and motives of the actor. For example,
accidentally causing an injury to an animal would be acceptable in
deontological ethics as the act was not intended, whereas it may not be
192 from a consequentialism view since the outcome harmed the animal.
Unit 21 Animal Welfare Ethical Theories-2
However, deontological ethical views may also have a number of bad points:
• It sets absolute rules, whereas not all cases may fit under these rules
and that can make it difficult to implement.
How we determine what is the right thing to do, and what our duties are, and
to whom, are still open to discussion, as with other ethical views and these
might be applied in real-life situations in regard to our use of animals will be
discussed next. 193
Block d Animal Welfare-II
21.2.1 Animal Rights View
Defenders of animal rights believe that there are fixed ethical rules that always
apply to and limit our treatment of animals, as we have discussed above.
These suggest that there are things that are just not ethical to do to animals,
which must be prohibited. Although this applies to all views of animal rights,
what rights animals should have can vary from person to person, and thus the
animal rights view can cover a range of differing views of animal treatment and
use. At the milder end of this view, we might consider it to be the right of
animals to be ‘treated with respect’ or ‘treated humanely’. This would consider
an animal’s rights to have been respected if we do not carry out anything to an
animal that was an avoidable harm, but might allow livestock farming and use
of animals in experiments as long as every step was taken to avoid suffering
or harm. At the other end of the spectrum would be more radical views that
might consider that an animal should have rights that are just like human
rights. Holders of this view would contend that animals have the right not to be
killed for human benefit (except in self-defence) and thus this view would be
incompatible with farming of animals for meat and the use of animals in
research and experimentation.
We will now consider how holding this ethical view might influence our views
on the acceptable treatment of animals by looking at two case studies or
scenarios.
SAQ 1
i) What is Deontology ethical theory?
With the rise of utilitarianism and deontology, virtue ethics, as the third main
framework, has receded as a key area of ethics, although there are still
contemporary virtue ethicists who would argue for its relevance. Detractors
have suggested that virtue ethics can, in fact, be subsumed as part of the
other two areas. Deontology contains notions of virtue in understanding what
are duties and rights, and utilitarianism also contains key aspects of virtue
ethics. Eudaimonia, as we discussed briefly in Unit 20, is the concept of
human flourishing or happiness, and can be seen as the state of a virtuous
person, achieving happiness or flourishing by pursuing a virtuous life, or as an
end in weighing up the costs and benefits as part of utilitarian approaches.
Virtue ethics can be considered to differ from both deontology and
utilitarianism as its focus is mainly on being a particular type of person rather
than acting in a particular way. It can be linked to both other ethical
approaches, however, through happiness and how this can be achieved,
through being virtuous, acting in a specific way to increase happiness, or
aiming to increase the overall amount of happiness.
Thus, in our response to, and decision-making about, animals, this ethical
view argues we should behave in a caring, generous and honest way not
because of the outcomes for the animals, or because of the rights of the
animal, but because this is what makes a caring and virtuous person.
Consideration of our attitudes, and those that underlie use and abuse of
animals, can lead us to make judgements about what is ethical or not in our
treatment of animals. This is different to either of the other two considerations
of animal treatment (deontology or utilitarianism) as it does not permit anything
to be done to animals as long as interests are maximized, and nor does it
prohibit any use of animals if there are times when the harming of some
animals may prevent a moral catastrophe. Instead, virtue ethics can be used
to take into account the context-dependent situations, in which human-animal
relationships are usually found.
Relational ethical views might go further than this though. Because the key
idea is that there are value in close relationships, they encourage respect and
can provide a vehicle for improved virtue or eudaimonia, this suggests that
close relationships should be encouraged. This is because they have value in
themselves, where both the person and the animal may benefit and flourish,
because the closeness of the relationship leads to improved quality of care for
the animal, and improved well-being for the person.
A final point here is that, in the application of ethical reasoning, most ethical
views are influenced by the capacities that animals have. In considering
whether the interests of the animal are relevant (in utilitarian thinking) or the
types of animals that should be given rights (in Animal Rights moral reasoning)
then the capacity of the animal to suffer, to be sentient, is relevant. In some
cases the degree of rights or the assessment of harm/benefit might be greater
for some animals because they are perceived to have a greater capacity for
emotional experience than others (for example, this is likely an influence in the
greater rights given to the Great Apes compared to other primates). The
relational ethical view, and the respect for nature position, differ from other
views in that this is less important compared to the intrinsic value that the
animal may have, either as a species, or as an individual.
SAQ 2
i) What is the key feature in the Virtue ethics approach?
ii) How does the Virtue ethics approach differ from utilitarianism and
deontology approaches?
21.5 SUMMARY
Let us summarise what you have learnt so far:
• The third ethical framework that can apply to animals is virtue ethics,
which suggests it is not actions that are important, but the type of person
we are.
• Relational ethics argues that the way we should behave towards animals
depends on the closeness of the relationship we have with them, and 199
Block d Animal Welfare-II
thus we should provide better care to animals, such as pets, because we
have a closer relationship to them than to farm animals or laboratory
animals.
21.7 ANSWERS
Self-Assessment Questions
1. i) Deontology is an ethical theory that states that the morality of an
action should be based on whether the action itself is right or
wrong under a series of rules, rather than based on the
consequences of the action.
iii) The Categorical Imperative states that one should always act in
such a way that it could be applied to everyone in the same
situation.
Terminal Questions
1. Refer to Section 21.2.
SUGGESTED READING
1. Palmer and Sandoe (2018). Animal Ethics (Chapter 1) In: Animal
Welfare, 3rd Edition, Ed. M.A. Appleby, A.S. Olsson & F. Galindo, CABI.
201
Volume 2 Animal Welfare-II
GLOSSARY
3Rs : A set of three key principles in the use of animals
in experiments. These are (in decreasing order of
importance) replacement for the use of animals,
reduction in the numbers of animals used and
refinement to minimize the harms to the animal.
Cerebral Cortex : The outer layer of neural tissue that covers the
cerebrum in humans and other mammals, often 203
Volume 2 Animal Welfare-II
referred to as the ‘grey matter’. It is the mostly
highly developed part of the brain and is
responsible for most information processing in
mammals.
212