(eBook PDF) Born to Talk: An Introduction to Speech and Language Development 6th Editionpdf download
(eBook PDF) Born to Talk: An Introduction to Speech and Language Development 6th Editionpdf download
https://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-born-to-talk-an-
introduction-to-speech-and-language-development-6th-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-born-to-talk-an-
introduction-to-speech-and-language-development-7th-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-an-introduction-to-
language-10th-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-an-introduction-to-
language-11th-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-language-development-an-
introduction-9th-edition/
(eBook PDF) Language Development: An Introduction 10th
Edition
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-language-development-an-
introduction-10th-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/original-pdf-an-introduction-to-
language-9th-australia-edition/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-an-introduction-to-
language-australia-new-zealand-edition/
https://ebooksecure.com/download/an-introduction-to-language-
ebook-pdf/
http://ebooksecure.com/product/ebook-pdf-introduction-to-eeg-and-
speech-based-emotion-recognition/
~ vi ~ Contents
Glossary 405
References and Suggested Readings 418
Name Index 465
Subject Index 473
Preface
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for the invaluable contributions of several people. Pamela
Hulit provided computer assistance in creating a number of tables included
in the book. Dr. Jill Gilkerson from the LENA Research Foundation provided
guidance in the selection of figures from their publications and language
samples from their database. Lori Shin from the LENA Research Foundation
tailored the graphic art to meet publication specifications. Taylor Weber,
a graduate student in speech-language pathology from the University of
Northern Colorado (UNC), assisted with the video project and created the
glossary. Andy Nagel, the videographer from Mirage Video Pro, and his assis-
tant Ian worked with us in a university and public school setting to produce
the videos. We appreciate the special talents of these people, and we are grate-
ful for their efforts on our behalf.
A special thank you is extended to the parents and their children who
graciously gave their time and language interactions to the video project,
Dr. Madeline Milian for her interview about learning English as a second
language and bilingual education, and Carol Haworth for her coordination
extraordinaire and expert speech-language pathology services in the school.
Finally, we thank the following people who reviewed the fifth edition of
this book: Courtney Karasinski, Grand Valley State University; Lori J. Marra,
Duquesne University; Monique T. Mills, The Ohio State University; Jean
Silbar, Grand Valley State University; Gabriella Simon-Cerijido, California
State University Los Angeles; and Laura J. Strong, Indiana University of
Pennsylvania.
Learning Objectives
This first chapter is designed to pique your interest in speech and language
as processes within the broader process we call communication. As a future
educator, you will be in a unique position to observe and facilitate the growth
of children in their journeys to be effective communicators. We define and
describe these processes, and we consider how speech and language interact
to produce a form of communication unique to humans. We also consider
how a speaker’s thoughts are conveyed to a listener’s brain through a series of
communication transformations known collectively as the speech chain.
T
he idea for Born to Talk was cultivated long before the first word of the
original manuscript was written, and it was probably a good thing that
there was a period of latency between the concept and the product.
During that latency, I* observed language development firsthand in my two
daughters, Yvonne and Carmen. I learned more about the power and won-
der of language in observing them than I have in all the books and all the
journal articles I have read over the course of my career because I witnessed
*
Lloyd Hulit.
~ 1 ~
~ 2 ~ Born to Talk
Ideas can be conveyed rather simply through three to five simple word
combinations and even nonverbally, but humans develop rules for putting
together words in complex arrangements in order to boost the power and effi-
ciency of their language. We can get a lot done by speaking in an organized
and clear manner. We use grammar to connect different types of words and
the rule structure for these combinations is called syntax. In the sentences
on the previous page, it is necessary to connect the nouns with other parts of
speech, so that the meaning occurs. Imagine if we did not have rules for com-
bining words. How would we make sense of messages if we only had nouns, for
example? Read the sentences aloud using only the three nouns and you will
get the point about the importance of syntax.
The arrangement of words is important for stating our meanings, and each
word is considered a morpheme—a unit of meaning. But we also have small
units of meaning that we add to words to enhance our messages. For example,
the word flowers in the example is not one, but two morphemes. Flower is the
name of the object and we use the s at the end to convey that there is more
than one. Thus the s is a plural morpheme. We will discuss morphemes in
greater detail in Chapters 4 and 5.
The fact that we do not all speak the same language suggests that some
aspects of language are learned. Languages are different in many ways. They
use different words and different rules for organizing words into grammatical
sentences. English, for example, stresses word order in its grammar system,
but other languages, such as Latin, place greater emphasis on word endings
than on order to indicate grammatical relationships. That is, all languages
have rules for making sentences grammatically correct, but the means by
which correctness is achieved vary. We can conclude, therefore, that although
the capacity for language is innate, and although all languages share very
basic rules, the specific conventions of any given language are learned. The
child who will speak English, for example, must learn the sounds of English as
well as its vocabulary and grammar.
Now let us put some of these pieces together into a definition. Language is
a system of abstract symbols and rule-governed structures, the specific conven-
tions of which are learned. The symbols of language may be sounds that are
combined into spoken words, or letters that are combined into written words,
or even the elements of sign language that are combined into larger units. It
is important to note that whatever the symbols, they are arbitrarily established
by the conventional usage of a given people. Furthermore, the symbols or
their combinations will change over time because language is a constantly
evolving phenomenon. Much more needs to be added to this definition (and
will be in the chapters that follow), but this will serve as a starting point.
We can now define speech, a relatively simple task if we understand com-
munication and language. Very simply, speech is the oral expression of lan-
guage. Sometimes people use the terms language and speech as though they
are interchangeable, but they clearly are not. If they were interchangeable,
one could not exist without the other because they would be one and the
same thing. In fact, speech can and does exist in the absence of language, and
~ 6 ~ Born to Talk
language exists in the absence of speech. Consider Toby the parrot or mynah
birds that can mimic human speech, often with remarkable clarity. These
birds produce speech, but they do not have language. That is, they can pro-
duce strings of sounds with the acoustic characteristics of human speech, and
human listeners recognize the sequences of sounds as words, but the speech
of these birds is devoid of meaning and, therefore, is not the oral expression
of language. They have speech but no language. Some human beings, most
notably those with severe cognitive challenges, may have the ability to imitate
speech perfectly even if they do not fully understand the language underlying
the speech. They have speech that reflects language abilities they do not have.
Even normal children, between the ages of 18 and 24 months, often produce
a form of speech known as echolalia, which is an imitation of words, phrases,
or even whole sentences in the absence of an understanding of what they are
saying. I* vividly recall a 3-year-old boy named Jerry, who arrived for his lan-
guage therapy session. Jerry had very little verbal language, but he frequently
used echolalia. When I said, “Hello, Jerry,” he responded “Hello, Jerry.” Then
he pointed at the sign at the top of the door saying, “E-X-I-T. For more infor-
mation call 1-800-234-6824.”
Language can also exist independently of speech. Children who are born
deaf, for example, may never learn to speak, but their deafness does not pre-
clude their use of language. If these children have no other problems and
receive proper stimulation and appropriate educational opportunities, they
can develop language abilities just as sophisticated and complete as those of
the hearing child who speaks. The child who is deaf and who does not have
speech must learn a different way to express language, most likely through
signs and gestures. In addition, the child who is deaf can receive and express
language through the written word.
We can best understand speech as a highly complex physiological process
requiring the coordination of respiration, phonation, resonation, and articu-
lation. Some of the movements involved in producing even the simplest utter-
ances are simultaneous and others are successive, but the synchronization of
these movements is critical.
Consider what happens in the production of the single word statistics. The
tip of the tongue is lifted from a resting position to an area on the roof of
the mouth just behind the upper teeth called the alveolar ridge to produce
the s sound. The tongue is pressed against the alveolar ridge hard enough
to produce constriction but not so hard as to stop the airflow altogether. As
the speaker slowly contracts the muscles of exhalation under precise control,
air is forced between the tip of the tongue and the alveolar ridge. Leaving
the tongue in the same area, the speaker now presses a little harder to stop
the airflow and then quickly releases the contact for the production of the
t sound. The tongue drops to a neutral position and the vocal folds in the
larynx vibrate to produce the vowel a. The speaker turns off the voice and lifts
the tongue to the alveolar ridge for the next t, then vibrates the vocal folds
*
Kathleen Fahey
A Connection of Brains ~ 7 ~
for the vowel i while the tongue stays in a forward but slightly lowered posi-
tion. The speaker turns voicing off again and moves the tongue to the alveolar
ridge yet again to produce the controlled constriction for the next s, followed
by increased pressure to stop the airflow and release it for the t. The voice is
turned on one more time and the tongue lowered to a neutral position for
the i, and then turned off as the tongue arches to the back of the mouth,
where it contacts the velum, or fleshy part of the roof of the mouth, for the
k. Finally, the tongue tip darts to the alveolar ridge for the production of the
final s sound.
All of this occurs in the production of one word! Imagine what occurs in
the production of a long sentence produced at an average rate of speed. In
addition to the production of each sound, we use suprasegmental aspects of
speech. The syllables in each word are produced with varied degrees of stress.
We also use intonation to emphasize certain words over others in order to
convey meaning, and we modulate our pitch and phrasing to make speech
interesting. Think of someone you know who speaks too fast, speaks in a
monotone, or doesn’t have a smooth rhythm. When you consider how many
intricate adjustments are made so quickly in the speech mechanism and the
suprasegmental aspects that we use to make our speech flow, it is difficult to
imagine that anyone learns to speak at all. But we do learn to speak, and we
do it easily and naturally over a very brief period of time.
breaking the vibrating airstream into sounds of speech, which are organized
into words and sentences. The listener’s ear receives these sounds. This fea-
ture is so obvious that we may need to note that other channels can be used
in communication and are used by other animals. Bees, for example, com-
municate by dancing, and that can be described as the visual channel. In fact,
Discovering Diverse Content Through
Random Scribd Documents
THE CASE OF THE FLYING EYE. Trans
Artists Productions. Released by
Tele Features. 6 min., sd., color,
16 mm. (Courageous Cat & Minute
Mouse cartoon series) Appl. author;
Gerald J. Rappoport. © Trans Artists
Productions, Inc. & Bob Kane; 26Jan61;
LP19382.
GOMER PYLE-USMC.
MICKEY.
DERMATOSIS CENICIENTA.
EPIZOONOSES.
TOXIC ANHIDROSIS.
TURBAN TUMOR.
NAKED CITY.
MANHUNT.
CASEY. See
LASSIE.
TIGHTROPE.
COMBAT!
CASKET 7.3. See
WAGON TRAIN.
COMBAT!
MISTER MAGOO.
GILLIGAN'S ISLAND.
DICK TRACY.
MASQUERADE.
CASUALTY. See
77 SUNSET STRIP.
BOPIN' HOOD.
CANE AND ABLE.
COMBAT!
FOXHOLE IN CAIRO.
CAT BALLOU. Harold Hecht Corp. Released
by Columbia Pictures Corp.
96 min., sd., color, 35 mm. Eastman
color. Based on a novel by Roy
Chanslor. © Harold Hecht Corp.;
1Jun65; LP30839.
TOP CAT.
GOMER PYLE-USMC.
CATACOMB AND DOGMA AFFAIR. See
CATACOMBS. See
LAWMAN.
CATALYSIS. See
CATAWOMPER. See
GUNSMOKE.
BONANZA.
ebooksecure.com