Journal of Business Research: Gerrit Hufnagel, Manfred Schwaiger, Louisa Weritz
Journal of Business Research: Gerrit Hufnagel, Manfred Schwaiger, Louisa Weritz
A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T
Keywords: This paper presents an empirical analysis of propositions concerning the consequences of price (un)fairness
Personalized pricing perceptions in personalized pricing based on individual consumer characteristics or customers’ previous pur
Dynamic pricing chase behavior. In three online experiments, we analyze consequences of personalized pricing for price-favored
Price fairness perceptions
and price-disadvantaged customers in an e-commerce setting. Results reveal negative attitudinal and behavioral
E-commerce
reactions to personalized pricing for both disadvantaged and even favored customers, mediated by price fairness
perceptions. Respondents’ aversion to personalized pricing is confirmed by a second study applying a within-
subject design. In addition, we evaluate consumers’ responses to various levels of information sensitivity in
personalized pricing mechanisms and find that consumers generally refuse personalized pricing approaches
regardless of the underlying personal data1.
* Corresponding author at: Institute for Market-Based Management, Munich School of Management, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität, Kaulbachstr. 45/I, 80539
Munich, Germany.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (G. Hufnagel), [email protected] (M. Schwaiger), [email protected] (L. Weritz).
1
We would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable input. The paper has benefitted from it a great deal.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2021.10.002
Received 19 December 2019; Received in revised form 29 September 2021; Accepted 3 October 2021
Available online 9 February 2022
0148-2963/© 2021 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
(Streitfeld, 2000). Amazon was accused of identifying existing cus empirical evidence of the consequences of price discrimination for both
tomers by means of cookies and of evaluating a customer’s previous favored and disadvantaged customers (Study 1). Section 4 (Study 2)
shopping behavior to discriminate prices for a DVD between loyal cus then serves as a robustness check for our central findings in Study 1. In
tomers and new customers (Huang, Chang, & Chen, 2005; Rosencrance, Section 5 we manipulate the degree of information sensitivity (Study 3),
2000). When consumers discovered Amazon’s pricing strategy, their before we conclude with theoretical and managerial implications as well
complaints were soon picked up by the media. The extremely negative as limitations and further research suggestions (Section 6).
media coverage led Amazon to discontinue their innovative price
discrimination approach immediately (Streitfeld, 2000). The Amazon 2. Literature review and hypotheses
example suggests that firms face a dilemma: While data-based price
discrimination can increase profitability, it could also alienate cus Price fairness, as perceived by consumers, is a central driver of the
tomers by arousing unfairness perceptions (Xia, Monroe, & Cox, 2004). acceptance of dynamic and personalized pricing offers, and leads to
Previous studies have indicated that consumers’ acceptance of a immediate changes in consumer behavior (Dickson & Kalapurakal,
price relies on their perception of its fairness (Cox, 2001; Kahneman, 1994). Perceived unfairness results in lower benevolence trust, lower
Knetsch, & Thaler, 1986; Maxwell, 2002). Price fairness is defined as the likelihood to purchase from this retailer, higher willingness to spread
judgment about whether a transaction or the process of concluding a negative WOM, both privately and publicly, and increased engagement
transaction is reasonable, acceptable, or just (Bolton, Warlop, & Alba, in additional search activities (Garbarino & Maxwell, 2010; Lii and Sy
2003). Research has shown that consumers respond to a price-setting 2009). In addition, if the magnitude of perceived price unfairness rea
practice that is perceived to be unfair with negative attitudinal and ches a certain level, customers may choose to complain, ask for a refund,
behavioral reactions (Haws and Bearden 2006; Huang et al., 2005; and/or leave the seller (Xia et al., 2004) to protect themselves from
Maxwell & Garbarino, 2010). Perceived unfairness leads to distrust and being taken advantage of in the future and/or to take revenge by
diminished shopping intentions, both offline and online (Campbell, harming the seller. Customers want to offset disparities resulting from
1999; Huang et al., 2005; Kahneman et al., 1986; Lii & Sy, 2009). unequal input–output ratios by exercising adverse behaviors to produce
Furthermore, price unfairness fosters switching behavior (Garbarino & internal consistency (Samuelson and Zeckhauser 1988).
Maxwell, 2010; Grewal, Hardesty, & Iyer, 2004) and negative word-of- Consumers generally prefer constant prices over price discrimination
mouth, both privately and publicly (Lii & Sy, 2009; Malc, Mumel, & (Huang et al., 2005). The latter is considered particularly unfair if new
Pisnik, 2016). customers receive better prices than existing/loyal customers (Bolton
Price fairness perceptions have been extensively discussed in mar et al., 2010; Darke and Dahl 2003; Maxwell and Garbarino, 2010; Tsai
keting and economics literature across various contexts (Grewal & and Lee, 2007).
Compeau, 1999; Wu et al., 2012). Given that its importance has further Consumers evaluate the fairness of a price both consciously and
increased with technological developments, an evolving research stream unconsciously based on at least one reference price (Xia et al., 2004),
investigates the effects of price discrimination tactics in an online which depends on historical or competitive prices, or prices paid by
context. However, these studies have targeted the impact of price other consumers. This relationship is moderated by transaction simi
discrimination only one-dimensionally by concentrating mainly on larity, a company’s motives for the price change, a customer’s prior trust
customers that are disadvantaged by a certain price discrimination in the company as well as the customer’s knowledge, beliefs, loyalty,
tactic. To understand the holistic effects and the associated implications and social norms. Customers perceive a price disadvantage compared to
for consumer behavior, changes in attitudes and behavior for both another customer group as less unfair if they have a high level of trust in
disadvantaged and favored consumers must be assessed. Thus, we the company, if the company sticks to social norms, or if the company
address this shortcoming by incorporating both customers that are has a justified motive to increase its prices (Victor et al., 2019; Xia et al.,
favored by price discrimination and those who are disadvantaged by 2004).
such a practice. The theory of procedural justice (Lind & Tyler, 1988) relates to the
In addition, prior research largely focused on purchase-timing-based process through which outcomes are obtained. Consumers evaluate
and purchase-frequency-based price discrimination and paid little whether the transaction partner adheres to “fair” rules or standards in
attention to other innovative discrimination approaches, such as the price setting process and the way in which their price has been
personalized pricing tactics using buyer identification (Lii & Sy, 2009; determined (Xia et al., 2004). Samuelson and Zeckhauser (1988) have
Malc et al., 2016; Wu, Liu, Chen, & Wang, 2012). Empirical findings on shown empirically that people are biased in favor of the status quo.
consumers’ reactions to innovative pricing practices based on personal When new rules (like personalized pricing) are applied, people need
information are scarce (Malc et al., 2016). We follow the call for time to become acquainted with these rules and perceive them to be fair
research by Grewal et al. (2004) or Lii and Sy (2009), as well as by the (Dickson & Kalapurakal, 1994). The social norm theory (Maxwell, 1999)
Marketing Science Institute (Stephen & Lamberton, 2016), and provide may explain perceived fairness in the price setting process (Lind & Tyler,
empirical evidence showing how both favored and disadvantaged cus 1988; Tyler, 1989). Fairness in that sense is defined by norms and
tomers react to personalized pricing. Our main contribution is to reveal behavior accepted by society (Maxwell, 1999), and a price change that
that, irrespective of whether consumers benefit from personalized does not comply with the socially accepted standard is perceived as
pricing or not, they react with less favorable behavioral outcomes (e.g. unfair (Dickson & Kalapurakal, 1994). Maxwell and Garbarino (2010)
purchase intention) compared to uniform pricing. We reveal the mech identified several corresponding norms: All buyers should be charged
anism underlying this (at least in parts) counter-intuitive result by the same price for a given product at the same time; a seller should not
showing that perceived price-unfairness causes the behavioral pattern make use of loyal or frequent customers by charging them higher rates
and that the sensitivity of information used to personalize prices plays or charge a higher price to infrequent or new customers; and a seller
only a minor role. should also reject favoring infrequent customers with lower prices
While our empirical evidence adds to the body of knowledge in the (Maxwell & Garbarino, 2010). Haws and Bearden (2006) state that
academic field, practitioners may learn to include changes in loyalty and consumers accept different prices at different sellers, but they perceive
customer patronage in their simulations of the effects of a switch to different prices at the same seller as unfair. Additionally, price differ
personalized price discrimination, rather than just focusing on margin ences are perceived as more unfair if they occur in close temporal
calculations coming from different prices. proximity. These effects hold for both positive and negative price de
Our study is organized as follows: In Section 2 we illustrate the state viations (Haws and Bearden 2006; Lii and Sy, 2009).
of the art in research and discuss the interplay of personalized price A first attempt to analyze the effect of dynamic prices in e-commerce
discrimination and information sensitivity. In section 3 we provide on price fairness perceptions was conducted by Grewal et al. (2004).
347
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Consumers view an Internet retailer using identification tactics as less that utilizes individual customer data to set prices (see Fig. 2.).
fair than a company using purchase timing as a tactic (Grewal et al., In order to not get an incomplete picture of the effects due to a
2004). The authors suppose that the familiarity consumers have with narrow choice of behavioral or attitudinal outcome variables that would
purchase timing strategies, which are regularly employed by the avia create difficulties when deriving implications (Martin et al., 2009), we
tion industry, favors the perceived price fairness of such an approach analyze customers’ reactions to personalized price discrimination from a
(Grewal et al., 2004). In other words: Industry behavior may turn into multidimensional perspective.
norms.
H3: Customers disadvantaged by personalized price discrimination
So far, the impact of dynamic or personalized pricing has mainly
show a) lower price fairness perceptions, b) lower purchase in
been targeted one-dimensionally, concentrating on customers that are
tentions, c) lower benevolence trust, d) higher intentions to search
disadvantaged by a certain price discrimination tactic (Priester et al.,
for alternative offerings, e) higher private complaint intentions, and
2020; Bolton et al., 2003; Xia et al., 2004). To understand the holistic
f) higher public complaint intentions than customers favored by
effects and the associated implications for consumer behavior, changes
personalized price discrimination.
in attitudes and behavior for both disadvantaged and favored consumers
should be assessed. Early studies indicate that people seem to dislike Research in contexts other than e-commerce showed that the sensi
online price discrimination even if they are granted discounts (Wang & tivity of collected information, “the level of privacy concern an indi
Krishna, 2012). vidual feels for a type of data in a specific situation” (Weible, 1993,
We aim to provide a holistic portrayal of the relationship between p. 30), can intensify consumers’ privacy concerns (Niemann &
personalized price discrimination and consumer behavior. Thus, both Schwaiger, 2016; Kehr, Kowatsch, Wentzel, & Fleisch, 2015). The more
favored customers receiving a lower price compared to a uniform price, sensitive the collected information is perceived to be, the higher the
as well as disadvantaged customers receiving a higher price, are studied. perceived risks and the lower the willingness to share information
We expect that price discrimination in e-commerce leads to negative (Malhotra, Kim, & Agrarwal, 2004; Phelps, Nowak, and Ferrell, 2000).
outcomes for both favored and disadvantaged customers. Following several calls for research (Grewal et al., 2004, 2011; Lii and
Sy, 2009), we investigate several types of consumer information to
H1: Relative to uniform prices, price discrimination leads to a) lower
manipulate the sensitivity level of personalized pricing mechanisms: the
price fairness perceptions, b) lower purchase intentions, and c)
devices used by consumers (Hannak, Soeller, Lazer, Mislove, and Wilson,
higher intentions to search for alternative offerings for both favored
2014), their geographical location (Mikians, Gyarmati, Erramilli, and
and disadvantaged customers.
Laoutaris, 2013; Borgesius and Poort (2017)), their past browsing and
Malc et al. (2016) found the largest negative effect of price purchasing behavior (Albarracin & Wyer, 2000; Kang, Hahn, Fortin,
discrimination on consumers’ price fairness perceptions if consumers Hyun, & Eom., 2006; Ouellette & Wood, 1998; Shimp & Kavas, 1984;
compare their prices with the price paid by a close friend. Furthermore, Shimp & Moody, 2000), and information obtained from social media pro
based on a correlation analysis, they found positive connections be files (Youyou et al., 2015).
tween price fairness perceptions and buying intentions, future shopping We predict that the information sensitivity of the personalized
intentions, and negative correlations with respect to switching to other pricing tactic increases consumers’ negative attitudinal and behavioral
sellers, and negative referrals of the seller to others. We aim to enhance reactions:
these findings by analyzing if consumers’ price fairness perceptions are
H4: The information sensitivity of the personalized pricing tactic
the central construct that mediates the relationship between price
increases customers’ negative reactions (i.e. a) lower fairness per
discrimination and consumer response.
ceptions, b) lower purchase intentions, c) lower trust, d) higher in
H2: Customers’ negative reactions to price discrimination (i.e., a) tentions to search for alternative offerings, e) higher private
lower purchase intentions, b) higher intention to search for alter complaint intentions, and f) higher public complaint intentions) to
native offerings) are mediated by price fairness perceptions. personalized price discrimination.
The first two hypotheses are summarized in our first research In addition, we expect that customers favored and disadvantaged by
framework (Fig. 1). personalized price discrimination are impacted differently by the in
Previous studies (see Appendix A for an overview) have examined formation sensitivity of the data that is used to discriminate prices.
either random price discrimination (between customers or sellers), time- Because favored customers benefit from a lower price, they do not
based price discrimination (i.e., dynamic pricing), or buyer identifica perceive the use of their personal data as severely as those customers
tion in e-commerce (Grewal et al., 2004; Lii & Sy, 2009) – a rather weak who are disadvantaged by price discrimination.
form of personalized pricing. Empirical evidence regarding other forms
H5: The information sensitivity of the data that is used to discrimi
of personalized pricing based on more sensitive consumer information is
nate prices positively moderates the difference between favored and
missing so far. In the subsequent hypotheses, we shift our focus from
disadvantaged customers and their reactions.
rather generic price discrimination approaches to personalized pricing
348
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
349
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
able to recall the price they paid or the price their friend paid (n = 13) Table 2
were excluded from analysis. Moreover, the data set was freed from Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations of Study 1
subjects who were identified as fast-clickers,4 and we excluded subjects Variables 1 2 3 4
with incomplete data regarding their demographics, resulting in a final
1 Price Fairness Perceptions 1.000
sample of 307 subjects. However, all significant key results remained 2 Purchase Intention 0.841*** 1.000
significant even when including these. 51% of our subjects are female 3 Future Search Intention -0.259*** -0.293*** 1.000
and age ranges from 22 to 72 years (Mage = 40.96, Mdnage = 39.00). 83% 4 Product Involvement 0.090 0.083 -0.009 1.000
of all subjects are employed full-time, while 8% are unemployed. In
Mean 5.26 4.88 6.13 5.43
addition, 52% of all subjects indicated a gross household income of more Standard Deviation 1.77 1.95 1.08 1.23
than $50,000 per year. Detailed descriptive statistics relating to the
respondents’ characteristics are shown in Appendix D. Note: *** p < .01, ** p < .05, * p < .10; N = 307.
Table 1 shows that the random assignment led to about equal dis
tribution of participants across the three experimental groups in our
included as covariates. The covariates are discussed only when they
final sample. Median completion time does not differ significantly across
were significant (see Table 3).
groups. In addition, group homogeneity could be assumed based on a set
Price fairness perceptions (see Model 1). Compared to uniform prices,
of Chi-square tests for categorical covariates5 and Kruskal-Wallis tests
we found a strong negative effect of price discrimination on price fair
for at least ordinally scaled covariates6. We provide correlations and
ness perceptions for disadvantaged customers (B = -3.019, t = -19.696,
descriptives in Table 2.
p < .001). If customers were favored by price discrimination, we
Results of the realism check demonstrate that the scenarios and the
observed a slightly negative effect on their price fairness perceptions (B
prices stated appear equally realistic to the participants between all
= -0.258, t = -1.633, p = .104). While the effect of price discrimination
groups. Results of one-way ANOVAs can be found in Appendix E.
on price fairness perceptions of price-favored customers points in the
hypothesized direction, the significance level is above 10%. Despite this,
Method
we may state support for H1a with reference to Study 2, where we
A set of ordinary least squares (OLS) regressions including mediation
addressed this issue in a within-subject design. Frequency of online
analyses were run to test our hypotheses. The regression analyses were
purchases has a significant positive impact on price fairness perceptions
conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics and the PROCESS macro developed
(B = 0.259, t = 1.789, p = .075). From our results, we infer that both
by Hayes (2018). PROCESS is increasingly used in the marketing liter
customers who benefit from price discrimination and those who are
ature including moderation and mediation analyses (e.g., Hüttel,
disadvantaged by price discrimination have lower price fairness per
Schumann, Mende, Scott, & Wagner, 2018; Inman & Nikolova, 2017;
ceptions than customers who pay uniform prices.
Mende, Scott, & Bolton, 2018). The bootstrapping method was applied
Purchase intention (see Model 2). Only customers who are disadvan
for estimating the relative indirect effects (Hayes, 2018). Compared to
taged by price discrimination have lower future purchase intentions (B
other methods, the bootstrapping method is considered to be more
= -1.421, t = -6.959, p < .001). No significant effect was found for
powerful and to perform best, and it takes into account the irregularities
customers who are favored by a lower price (B = -0.110, t = -0.786, p =
of the sample distribution of the relative indirect effect (Hayes, 2009;
.432). These results only partially support H1b. However, the results of
Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010). We followed Mallinckrodt, Abraham, Wei,
the relative indirect effects (Model 3) show that, compared to uniform
and Russell’s (2006) recommendation of generating at least 10,000
prices, both lower (i.e., favored customers, relative indirect effect =
bootstrap confidence intervals.
-0.166, LLCI = -0.351, ULCI = -0.006) and higher prices (i.e., disad
vantaged customers, relative indirect effect = -1.942, LLCI = -2.432,
3.2. Results
ULCI = -1.500) lead to significantly lower future purchase intentions,
supporting H2a. In addition, the difference between the relative indirect
In our mediation models (PROCESS model = 4; 10,000 resamples)
effect of favored and disadvantaged customers also proves to be highly
consumers’ purchase intention as well as their future search intention
significant (relative indirect effect = -1.7761, LLCI = -2.221, ULCI =
were used as dependent variables, the reference price as the indepen
-1.376).
dent variable, and consumers’ price fairness perceptions as a mediator.
Future search intentions (see Model 4). Results reveal that price
Age, gender7, income8, and frequency of online purchases9 were
discrimination leads to higher future search intentions for favored cus
tomers (B = 0.326, t = 2.257, p = .025) as well as for disadvantaged
Table 1 customers (B = 0.755, t = 3.573, p < .001). Thus, H1c is supported.
Sample Distribution and Median Completion Times across the Experimental However, this effect is not mediated by price fairness perceptions (see
Groups of Study 1.
Model 5 favored customers, relative indirect effect = 0.008, LLCI =
Experimental Scenario N Completion time (Mdn, in -0.019, ULCI = 0.043; disadvantaged customers, relative indirect effect
Group min)
= 0.097, LLCI = -0.202, ULCI = 0.390). Hence, H2b cannot be sup
1 Same price 110 3.64 ported. Age is the only covariate with a significant impact on future
2 Favored customers 93 3.78 search intention (B = 0.013, t = 2.202, p = .028; see Model 4 in Table 3).
3 Disadvantaged 104 3.66
Perceived Information Sensitivity. A repeated measures ANOVA was
customers
Total 307 3.68 used to test differences in perceived information sensitivity of multiple
data types and consumers’ willingness to reveal those data types to
marketers. The Huynh-Feldt estimate of the departure from sphericity
4
Participants who took less than half of the median completion time were was ε = 0.861 (see Table 4). Perceived data sensitivity was significantly
screened out. affected by data types, F(3.445,1054.086) = 118.243, p < .001. Results of
5
Gender, education, employment status, and social media usage. the pairwise comparisons of group means (Bonferroni tests) are visual
6
Product involvement, age, income, Internet usage, frequency, and amount ized in Table 5, and showed significant differences (p < .001) in means
of online purchases. of perceived data sensitivity for all data types, except for two compari
7
Dummy coded: 0 = male, 1 = female. sons, namely geographical location versus past shopping behavior (Mdiff =
8
Dummy coded: 0 = less than $50,000, 1 = equal or above $50,000. -0.225, p = .253) and past browsing behavior versus social media data
9
Dummy coded: 0 = less than weekly online purchases, 1 = at least weekly (Mdiff = 0.088, p = 1.000) (see Table 5).
online purchases.
350
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Table 3
Mediation Analysis Study 1
Model 1: Mediator Variable – Price Fairness Perceptions Model 2: Mediation Model – Purchase Intention
Constant 5.769 0.280 20.636 0.000 Constant 1.723 0.383 4.504 0.000
Favored customer − 0.258 0.158 − 1.633 0.104 Favored customer − 0.110 0.140 − 0.786 0.432
Disadvantaged customer − 3.019 0.153 − 19.696 0.000 Disadvantaged customer − 1.421 0.204 − 6.959 0.000
Price Fairness Perceptions 0.643 0.051 12.663 0.000
Covariates Covariates
Age 0.010 0.006 1.530 0.127 Age 0.009 0.006 1.612 0.108
Gender 0.106 0.129 0.820 0.413 Gender − 0.121 0.114 − 1.068 0.287
Income 0.145 0.131 1.106 0.270 Income − 0.104 0.115 − 0.905 0.366
Frequency of Online Purchases 0.259 0.145 1.789 0.075 Frequency of Online Purchases 0.165 0.128 1.287 0.199
Model 4: Mediation Model – Future Search Intention Model 5: Relative Indirect Effect Through Price Fairness
Perceptions on Future Search Intention
Constant 5.406 0.396 13.649 0.000 Favored customer 0.008 0.015 − 0.019 0.043
Favored customer 0.326 0.144 2.257 0.025 Disadvantaged customer 0.097 0.150 − 0.202 0.390
Disadvantaged customer 0.755 0.211 3.573 0.000
Price Fairness Perceptions − 0.032 0.053 − 0.612 0.541
Covariates
Age 0.013 0.006 2.202 0.028
Gender − 0.034 0.118 − 0.289 0.773
Income − 0.015 0.119 − 0.128 0.898
Frequency of Online Purchases 0.174 0.133 1.312 0.191
R-square 0.134
F 6.632
d.f.1/d.f.2 7/299
p-value 0.000
Note: N = 307.
a
Standard errors from the mean result of bootstrapping procedure.
b
LLCI/ULCI = lower-/upper-level of bias corrected bootstrap 95%-confidence interva l.
Willingness to reveal information. The Huynh-Feldt estimate of sphe comparisons of group means revealed significant differences (p < .001)
ricity showed a substantial deviation (ε = 0.778) between data types. for all comparisons of data types, except for past browsing behavior versus
Participants’ willingness to reveal information was significantly affected social media data (Mdiff = 0.130, p = .858). The mean values for both
by data types, F(3.110,951.713) = 183.407, p < .001. Again, pairwise variables are displayed in Table 6.
Based on the results of this study, three data types for implementing
personalized price discrimination were selected for Study 3: consumers’
Table 4 device used (low information sensitivity), their geographical location
Mauchly’s Sphericity Test of Study 1 (medium information sensitivity), and their past browsing behavior (high
Huynh-Feldt F Sig. information sensitivity).
Perceived Information Sensitivity 0.861 118.243 0.000
Willingness to Reveal Information 0.778 183.407 0.000 Table 6
Note: *** p < .01, ** p < .05, * p < .10; N = 307. Mean Values of Perceived Information Sensitivity and Willingness to Reveal
Information by Data Types of Study 1
Note: *** p < .01, ** p < .05, * p < .10; N = 307. Note: N = 307.
351
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Mean 4.81 4.64 5.85 5.59 5. Study 3: Consumer reactions to Data-Based personalized price
Standard Deviation 1.01 1.09 0.97 1.21
discrimination
Note: *** p < .01, ** p < .05, * p < .10; N = 440.
5.1. Experimental design
352
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Study 1) factorial between-subject design. Again, subjects were (30) = 25.917, p = .679), and annual household income (H = 3.717, p =
recruited via MTurk. As in Study 1, only workers from the United States .156). Results of a one-way ANOVA demonstrate that the treatments are
who have completed more than 1,000 tasks with an approval rate of perceived as equally realistic (MRealistic = 5.48) with no significant mean
greater than 97% were allowed to participate. In turn, they were differences between the six conditions (F(5,499) = 0.726, p = .604),
rewarded with $0.90 for taking part. which were easy to understand for the participants (MEasy = 6.72,
First, participants were presented with a base scenario identical to Brown-Forsythe(5,410.850) = 2.095, p = .065). Correlations and de
the one successfully applied in Study 1. In addition, depending on their scriptives of our focal variables are provided in Table 9.
experimental group, participants randomly received the information
why their price varied (i.e., lower or higher reference price) from the
5.2. Results
price their friend paid (i.e., based on consumer data with low, medium,
or high information sensitivity). Appendix G displays the treatments for
A series of 2 × 3 ANCOVA analyses controlling for perceived infor
all six experimental groups.
mation sensitivity, general Internet privacy concerns, product involve
ment, age, gender12, income13, frequency of online purchases14, and
Measures
employment status15 were estimated in order to analyze the effect of the
Appendix C provides an overview of the variables used in this study.
price-change-direction (favored vs. disadvantaged customers) and the
Previously validated scales were utilized and adapted to this research
personalized pricing tactics (based on customers’ devices, their
context if necessary. Established multi-item scales that had also been
geographical location, or their past browsing behavior) on attitudinal
partly applied in Study 1 were used again. All items were captured either
and behavioral reactions. Levene’s test and normality checks were car
on a 7-point Likert scale or on a seven-point semantic differential scale.
ried out and the assumptions met for each ANCOVA analysis. The group
Internal consistency reliability estimates for all reflective multi-item
means and standard deviations for all dependent variables of interest are
scales were above α = 0.80, indicating reliable measures.
summarized in Table 10.
Dependent variables. Due to the focus on the process of price
Price fairness perceptions. A simple effects analysis shows that disad
discrimination in the second study, we decided to measure price fairness
vantaged customers have significantly lower price fairness perceptions
perceptions (α = 0.927) using six items to capture both the distributive
(F(1,491) = 238.732, p < .001, B = -1.567). According to Cohen (1973),
(used in Study 1) and the procedural dimension of price fairness per
the effect size (η2 = 0.327) can be classified as large, supporting H3a. In
ceptions (Martin et al., 2009). The scales used to measure purchase
addition, we found a significant effect of the price personalization tactic
intention (α = 0.976) and future search intention (α = 0.920) were (F(2,491) = 3.658, p = .026, η2 = 0.015). However, pairwise comparisons
identical to those from Study 1. The impact of a personalized pricing
of group means (Bonferroni tests) revealed significant differences only
tactic on consumers’ benevolence trust (α = 956) was assessed using five for the comparison of price discrimination based on the device used vs.
items (Garbarino and Lee, 2003; Garbarino & Maxwell, 2010). Cus
the geographical location (Mdevice = 3.23 vs. Mlocation = 3.61, p = .028).
tomers’ intention to complain about their experience privately (α = Based on these results, we find no empirical support for H4a. Yet,
0.804) was captured with four items, while their intention to complain
perceived data sensitivity has a significant negative effect on price
publicly (α = 0.857) was assessed by five items (Singh, 1988). fairness perceptions (F(1,491) = 6.380, p = .012, B = -0.100). A possible
The same manipulation checks as in Study 1 were included to vali
explanation will be discussed in section six.
date that all three scenarios presented were equally realistic and that The two-way interaction between the price-change-direction and the
respondents recognized the price discrimination. In addition, re
price personalization tactic is non-significant (F(2,491) = 1.290, p =
spondents were prompted to recall the reason for the price difference to
.276). Thus, H5 is not supported.
check the manipulation of the data source used to discriminate prices
Purchase intention. Customers disadvantaged by personalized price
(multiple-choice question).
discrimination have a significantly lower purchase intention than those
We controlled for consumers’ product involvement (α = 0.934), their
who are favored by it (F(1,491) = 87.470, p < .001,B = -1.080), sup
Internet privacy concerns (α = 0.951), as well as their perceived sensi
porting H3b. Based on the partial eta square, the effect size is medium
tivity of the personal data that was utilized to discriminate prices (α =
(η2 = 0.151). Moreover, we detected a significant effect of the price
0.904). In addition, as in Study 1, scales for Internet usage, frequency
personalization tactic (F(2,491) = 2.520, p = .081, η2 = 0.010). However,
and number of online purchases, as well as social media usage, were
in contrast to our expectations, pairwise comparisons of group means
included to ensure that the groups do not differ significantly with regard
demonstrated significant differences only for the comparison of price
to these aspects. The last part of the survey addressed demographics.
discrimination based on the device used vs. the geographical location
(Mdevice = 2.47 vs.
Sample description
Mlocation = 2.87, p = .094). Thus, H4b has to be rejected. The two-
We collected a total of 606 survey answers. Respondents who were
way interaction between the price-change-direction and the price
not able to recall the price they paid or the price their friend paid, as well
personalization tactic is significant (F(2,491) = 2.412, p = .091) but small
as those who were not able to recall the reason for the price difference,
in magnitude η2 = 0.010. Customers who are price-disadvantaged
were excluded from analysis. In addition, the data set was freed from
respond more negatively to personalized price discrimination based on
subjects identified as fast-clickers11 and subjects with incomplete data
their geographical location than price-favored customers (B = -0.765, p
regarding their demographics, resulting in a final sample of 505
= .035), but the other comparisons were non-significant, lending partial
subjects.
support for H5. Perceived data sensitivity (B = -0.098, p = .054), general
47% of our subjects are female, and age ranges from 21 to 75 years
Internet privacy concerns (B = -0.192, p = .001), product involvement
(Mage = 40.47, Mdnage = 38.00). 46% of all subjects indicated a gross
(B = 0.127, p = .037), and gender (B = -0.272, p = .084) have a sig
household income of more than $50,000 per year. Participants’ random
nificant effect on purchase intention.
assignment to the different scenarios can be considered successful.
Future search intention. Results of our ANCOVA show a higher
Across the six experimental conditions, we found no significant differ
ences regarding age (H = 0.061, p = .970), gender (χ2(5) = 9.181, p =
.102), education (χ2(35) = 29.479, p = .731), employment status (χ2 12
Dummy coded: 0 = male, 1 = female.
13
Dummy coded: 0 = less than $50,000, 1 = equal or above $50,000.
14
Dummy coded: 0 = less than weekly online purchases, 1 = at least weekly
11
Participants who took less than half of the median completion time were online purchases.
15
screened out. Dummy coded: 0 = others, 1 = full-time.
353
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Table 9
Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations of Study 3
Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
8 Internet Privacy -0.121*** -0.150*** -0.139*** 0.203*** 0.255*** 0.211*** 0.141*** 1.000
Concerns
9 Product -0.007 0.069 0.058 0.102** 0.075* 0.077* 0.081* 0.063 1.000
Involvement
Mean 3.40 2.45 2.65 6.57 4.96 2.68 4.48 5.55 5.40
Standard Deviation 1.62 1.42 1.85 0.86 1.46 1.47 1.54 1.40 1.27
354
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
355
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Table 12
Overview of Hypotheses Test Results of Study 1 and 3
Study H1 Relative to uniform prices, price discrimination leads to…
1 a) lower price fairness perceptions, Partly supported
b) lower purchase intentions, and Partially supported
c) higher intentions to search for alternative offerings Supported
for both favored and disadvantaged customers.
H2 Customers’ negative reactions to price discrimination (i.e., a) lower purchase intentions, b) higher intention to search for a) Supported
alternative offerings) are mediated by price fairness perceptions. b) Not supported
Study H3 Customers disadvantaged by personalized price discrimination show…
3 a) lower price fairness perceptions, Supported
b) lower purchase intentions, Supported
c) lower benevolence trust, Supported
d) higher intentions to search for alternative offerings, Supported
e) higher private complaint intentions, and Supported
f) higher public complaint intentions Supported
than customers favored by personalized price discrimination.
H4 The information sensitivity of the personalized pricing tactic increases customers’ negative reactions (i.e., a) lower fairness Only partial support for H4e
perceptions, b) lower purchase intentions, c) lower trust, d) higher intentions to search for alternative offerings, e) higher
private complaint intentions, and f) higher public complaint intentions) to personalized price discrimination.
H5 The information sensitivity of the data that is used to discriminate prices positively moderates the difference between Partially supported with Purchase
favored and disadvantaged customers and their reactions. Intention as DV
H6 There is moderated mediation such that the negative effect of price discrimination on customer reactions is moderated by Partially supported (only
the information sensitivity of the personalized pricing tactic and mediated by price fairness perceptions. mediation)
study conducted by Malc et al. (2016) found significant correlations Previous studies have been criticized for largely concentrating on
between price fairness perceptions and consumer reactions to price selected behavioral or attitudinal outcome variables (Martin et al.,
discrimination in e-commerce, our study is the first that provides sta 2009). Taking a more comprehensive perspective in Study 3, we found
tistical evidence of the causality by analyzing price fairness perceptions that customers disadvantaged by personalized price discrimination react
as mediator in the relationship between price discrimination and con with lower purchase intentions, higher future search intentions, lower
sumer response. benevolence trust, and higher private as well as public complaint in
Results of Study 1 revealed negative attitudinal and behavioral re tentions. We could confirm the results of our first study and found evi
actions for both customer groups. Even favored customers who receive a dence that customers’ responses to personalized pricing are mediated by
lower price compared to uniform prices show lower purchase intentions their price fairness perceptions.
and higher intentions to search for alternative offers or sellers in the Lastly, we investigated several forms of buyer identification with
future. These facts were confirmed by Study 2 in a within-subject design. varying information sensitivity: Considering the device used by a
Yet, as expected, the negative effect on purchase intention and the customer (low information sensitivity), their geographical location
positive effect on future search intention is stronger for disadvantaged (medium information sensitivity), and their past browsing behavior
customers who are charged a price premium. For both customer groups (high information sensitivity) to personalize prices, we may respond to
we found a mediating effect of price fairness perceptions on purchase questions raised by Grewal et al. (2004) and Lii and Sy (2009). Our
intention. Comparing the direct and relative indirect effects of price statistical analyses could not detect significant differences in customers’
discrimination on purchase intention, we noticed that customers’ price reactions based on the information sensitivity of those price discrimi
fairness perceptions are the central construct that shapes customers’ nation practices. Results indicate that customers dislike price discrimi
reactions to price discrimination. Thereby, this paper provides well- nation regardless of how a price difference arises. Nonetheless, we found
grounded statistical evidence of the link between price fairness per effects of perceived data sensitivity on price fairness perceptions, pur
ceptions and consumer behavior. However, we could not detect a chase intentions, and private complaint intentions. Another important
mediating effect of price fairness perceptions on the relationship be factor that influences customers’ attitudinal and behavioral reactions to
tween price discrimination and customers’ future search intention. personalized price discrimination is their general Internet privacy con
Future search intentions seem to be directly affected by price discrimi cerns, showing significant effects on all outcome variables of interest. In
nation, but not indirectly through fairness perceptions. When searching summary, we conclude that customers have a general aversion to price
for additional products or sellers, consumers must trade off the discrimination, and that the negative reactions are dependent on indi
perceived benefits (e.g., monetary savings) relative to the costs of search vidual characteristics, such as subjective perceptions of data sensitivity
(e.g., time, effort) (Marmorstein, Grewal, & Fishe, 1992; Grewal et al., and privacy concerns.
1998). Facing a discriminated price is a clear signal that other, cheaper Furthermore, although only partly significant, our results (see Fig. 3)
offers may be available, and thus consumers’ future search intention is indicate the highest acceptance for personalized price discrimination
directly (positively) affected by price discrimination. Moreover, it must based on customers’ geographic location, despite the fact that customers
be noted that the development of the construct future search intention perceive their geographic location as being more sensitive than the de
ordinates back to the early 80 s (Della Bitta, Monroe, & Mcginnis, 1981), vice they use to make purchases. This finding is in line with the social
when price comparisons were not as easy as today when consumers may norm theory (Maxwell, 1999). Social norms are said to have a strong
simply check Google Shopping or price comparison websites like Pri influence on the success of discriminatory pricing systems in general,
ceGrabber.com. and therewith shape the applicability of price discrimination (Maxwell
356
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
and Garbarino, 2010). Consequently, the violation of social norms can 6.3. Limitations and further research
induce the perception of an unfair price and thereby provoke adverse
reactions (Rondan-Cataluña & Martin-Ruiz, 2011). Customers are used Consideration of the implications should be made in the light of
to local price differences, but they rarely or never experienced several limitations, which provide fruitful paths for future research.
discriminated prices based on the devices used or based on their past While scenario-based experiments, such as the ones conducted in this
browsing behavior. In this study, mean values for those two types of study, provide good internal validity, they may lack external validity.
price discrimination are almost identical, indicating that customers For pragmatic reasons, we had to rely on stated behavior, although we
generally refuse innovative price discrimination approaches not aligned share the concern that observational studies may be superior in this
with prevailing standards and norms. context. We suggest that future research should extend our research
questions to more realistic circumstances or test them in the field.
6.2. Managerial implications Furthermore, the experiments described in this paper focused on a single
product offered by a fictitious retailer. Yet, the effects of personalized
Practitioners will have to face negative customer reactions to price prices on consumer behavior may depend on product type or associa
discrimination that might offset potential monetary benefits. The pre tions with the retailer. Future studies might not only investigate these
requisite for customers’ negative attitudinal and behavioral reactions circumstances, but they should also take into account approaches to
are price comparisons that enable them to notice interpersonal differ mitigate the negative effects of personalized price discrimination. Pre
ences. Online, price discrimination can be easily discovered due to social vious studies suggest that consumers’ knowledge of information being
media, price comparison portals, or price search bots (Richards et al., collected has a negative impact on their concerns (Niemann &
2016). Online merchants thus need to be particularly mindful in the Schwaiger, 2016). Hence, managers may have possibilities to increase
application of discriminated prices, as such practices interfere with the price fairness perceptions by notifying the customer which information a
general consensus regarding fair pricing mechanisms and the corre firm has collected and how this information will be used (increasing
sponding social norms (Maxwell and Garbarino, 2010). The emergence information transparency) (Dinev & Hart, 2006). Another approach is to
of such norms is proven to vary substantially across different industries, present the price in different formats (e.g. discounts, Weisstein et al.,
which in turn must be recognized when fairness of a pricing discrimi 2013) to increase transaction dissimilarity, which in turn mitigates the
nation approach is evaluated. negative effects of dynamic price discrimination on consumers’
As Kuo, Rice, and Fennell (2016) note, increasing familiarity with a perceived fairness, trust, and repurchase intention. Further research
pricing practice favors perceived price fairness. Personalized price could provide additional empirical evidence whether individual dis
discrimination might be (more) successful in the future, once its appli counts should be applied to differentiate or even personalize prices.
cation has gained more acceptance due to its widespread diffusion in Finally, future research could also transfer findings from this paper,
other contexts, similar to the development yield management has un which focuses on e-commerce, to brick-and-mortar stores. At present,
dergone (Kimes, 2002). Concerning that practice, it seems favorable that some supermarket chains are already testing personalized coupons
the services of hotels or airlines are less comparable, because they (Venkatesan & Farris, 2012). By offering coupons through loyalty pro
feature varying characteristics and additional benefits which are asso grams or apps, personalized price discrimination that is based on cus
ciated with the offer. Hence, personalized price discrimination might be tomers’ shopping behavior could be implemented offline (Iyer et al.,
best accompanied by a customization of the product or service, as that 2002). In this context, challenges concerning the multichannel or even
substantially decreases the similarity and thereby might divert attention omnichannel strategy of retailers that act online and offline should be
from price differences (Weisstein, Monroe, & Kukar-Kinney, 2013; Xia further elaborated to avoid cannibalization between sales channels.
et al., 2004).
Knowing that loyalty has a positive effect on fairness perceptions CRediT authorship contribution statement
when price differences are low (Martin et al., 2009), a committed
customer base may serve as a buffer to the negative ramifications of Gerrit Hufnagel: Formal analysis, Data curation, Methodology,
personalized prices (Victor et al., 2019). However, pricing strategists are Project administration, Writing – original draft, Visualization, Concep
well advised to integrate effects of loyalty, patronage, and search in tualization, Investigation. Manfred Schwaiger: Methodology, Investi
tentions in their simulation models when figuring out the impact of price gation, Formal analysis, Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing,
changes on their revenues. In the event that approaches to improve price Validation, Supervision, Resources. Louisa Weritz: Formal analysis,
fairness perception to a sufficient degree fail, responsible pricing man Methodology, Data curation, Project administration, Validation, Writing
agers should contrast different profitability scenarios capturing the – review & editing.
trade-off between the positive effect of (even small) increases in price
and the resulting drop in customer patronage and loyalty. From a
managerial perspective, the gain in revenues might outweigh the Declaration of Competing Interest
negative consequences of the customer behavior induced by personal
ized pricing. In any case, the results likely depend on company and The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
customer-specific characteristics, so that general statements seem to interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
lack validity at this time. the work reported in this paper.
357
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
358
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
(continued )
Scenario 1
You have decided to buy a new smartphone. In the course of an extensive online research, you When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out
have informed yourself and decided to buy the model shown below in online shop you know from that he bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same
previous purchases. online shop.
Now imagine that you have bought the offer as shown below. Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $100, as you
Please take a moment and have a look at the product. paid.
Scenario 2
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out
that he bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same
online shop.
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $120, whereas
the same smartphone cost you $100.
Scenario 3
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out
that he bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same
online shop.
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $80, whereas the
same smartphone cost you $100.
359
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Dependent Variables
Control Variables
17
The Xs shown in Appendix C are intended to indicate in which of the three studies the respective construct was queried with the associated scale. The values in
parentheses below indicate the respective Cronbach’s Alpha values.
360
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
(continued )
Control Variables
How many times did you purchase products online in the last 12
months?
• Never
Frequency of Online • Once or twice
x x x Doolin et al. (2005), adapted
Purchases • 3–6 times
• Monthly
• Bi-Weekly (only in Study 2)
• Weekly
Which social networks do you use on a regular basis (at least once a
week)?
• Facebook
• Instagram
Social Media Usage x x x Malhotra et al. (2004), adapted
• Twitter
• Snapchat
• LinkedIn
• None of these
Manipulation Checks
Manipulation Check Data Source What was the reason why you and your friend had to pay different prices?
• Our past browsing behavior before we made the purchase.
• The devices we used to make the purchase. x Self-developed
• Our geographical location when we made the purchase.
Demographics
Demographics Gender, age, education, employment status, and annual gross household income x x x
361
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Appendix D. Overview of descriptive statistics for demographics and Control variables in Study 1
Gender
Male 147 47.9
Female 160 52.1
Education
Some school but no degree 3 1.0
High school graduate 41 13.4
Some college but no degree 96 31.3
Bachelor’s degree 127 41.4
Master’s degree 31 10.1
Professional degree 5 1.6
Doctorate degree 2 0.7
Other 2 0.7
Employment status
Employed full-time 256 83.4
Employed part-time 5 1.6
Self-employed 14 4.6
Homemaker 1 0.3
Student 2 0.7
Retired 2 0.7
Unemployed 23 7.5
Other 4 1.3
362
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Note: N = 307.
Appendix F. Stimuli and manipulations of Study 2
Scenario 1:
The selected online shop offers you the smartphone for $100. The website, among others, lets you know that all products
are offered with a “best price guarantee”. Now imagine that you have bought the smartphone. Please take a moment and
have a look at the product.
In order to review the pricing strategy, you ask your best friend to request the same product from the same website
immediately. It turns out that your friend would have to pay the same price as you, namely $100 for the smartphone.
Scenario 2:
The selected online shop offers you the smartphone for $120. The website, among others, lets you know
that personalized prices are offered based on individual (non-transparent) criteria. Now imagine that you have bought
the smartphone. Please take a moment and have a look at the product.
In order to review the pricing strategy, you ask your best friend to request the same product from the same website
immediately. It turns out that your friend would have to pay a lower price than you, namely $100 for the smartphone.
Scenario 3:
The selected online shop offers you the smartphone for $80. The website, among others, lets you know that personalized
prices are offered based on individual (non-transparent) criteria. Now imagine that you have bought the smartphone.
Please take a moment and have a look at the product.
In order to review the pricing strategy, you ask your best friend to request the same product from the same website
immediately. It turns out that your friend would have to pay a higher price than you, namely $100 for the smartphone.
You have decided to buy a new smartphone. In the course of an extensive online research, Scenario 1: Favored customer, based on device used
you have informed yourself and decided to buy the model shown below in an online shop
you know from previous purchases. When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he
bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop.
Now imagine that you have bought the offer as shown below.
Please take a moment and have a look at the product. Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $120, whereas the same
smartphone cost you $100.
Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will
be informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the device
363
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
(continued )
customers use to make a purchase.
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he
bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop.
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $80, whereas the same
smartphone cost you $100.
Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will
be informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the device
customers use to make a purchase.
Scenario 3: Favored customer, based on geographical location Scenario 5: Favored customer, based on past browsing behavior
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he bought the When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he
same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop. bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop.
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $120, whereas the same Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $120, whereas the same
smartphone cost you $100. smartphone cost you $100.
Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will be Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will
be informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the
informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the customer’s
customer’s past browsing behavior.
geographical location when making a purchase.
Scenario 6: Disadvantaged customer, based on past browsing behavior
Scenario 4: Disadvantaged customer, based on geographical location
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he
When you tell your best friend about your new smartphone, it turns out that he bought the bought the same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop.
same smartphone as you on the same day in the same online shop.
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $80, whereas the same
Your friend shows you the invoice indicating that he paid $80, whereas the same smartphone cost you $100.
smartphone cost you $100.
Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will
Upon consultation with the online shop’s customer service department, you will be be informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the
informed that the online shop differentiates its prices according to the customer’s customer’s past browsing behavior.
geographical location when making a purchase.
References Doolin, B., Dillon, S., Thompson, F., & Corner, J. L. (2005). Perceived Risk, the Internet
Shopping Experience and Online Purchasing Behavior. Journal of Global Information
Management, 13(2), 66–88.
Albarracin, D., & Wyer, R. S. (2000). The Cognitive Impact of Past Behavior: Influences
Elmaghraby, W., & Keskinocak, P. (2003). Dynamic Pricing in the Presence of Inventory
on Beliefs, Attitudes, and Future Behavioral Decisions. Journal of Personality and
Considerations: Research Overview, Current Practices, and Future Directions.
Social Psychology, 79(1), 5–22.
Management Science, 49(10), 1287–1309.
Biswas, A., Pullig, C., Yagci, M. I., & Dean, D. H. (2002). Consumer Evaluation of Low
Garbarino, E., & Lee, O. F. (2003). Dynamic Pricing in Internet Retail: Effects on
Price Guarantees: The Moderating Role of Reference Price and Store Image. Journal
Consumer Trust. Psychology & Marketing, 20(6), 495–513.
of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 107–118.
Garbarino, E., & Maxwell, S. (2010). Consumer Response to Norm-Breaking Pricing
Bolton, L. E., Keh, H. T., & Alba, J. W. (2010). How Do Price Fairness Perceptions Differ
Events in E-Commerce. Journal of Business Research, 63(9–10), 1066–1072.
across Culture? Journal of Marketing Research, 47(3), 564–576.
Grewal, D., Ailawadi, K. L., Gauri, D., Hall, K., Kopalle, P., & Robertson, J. R. (2011).
Bolton, L. E., Warlop, L., & Alba, J. W. (2003). Consumer Perceptions of Price (Un)
Innovations in Retail Pricing and Promotions. Journal of Retailing, 87(Supplement 1),
Fairness. Journal of Consumer Research, 29(4), 474–491.
43–52.
Borgesius, F. Z., & Poort, J. (2017). Online Price Discrimination and EU Data Privacy
Grewal, D., & Compeau, L. D. (1999). Pricing and Public Policy: A Research Agenda and
Law. Journal of Consumer Policy, 40(3), 346–366.
an Overview of the Special Issue. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 18(1), 3-10.
Campbell, M. C. (1999). Perceptions of Price Unfairness: Antecedents and Consequences.
Grewal, D., Hardesty, D. M., & Iyer, G. R. (2004). The Effects of Buyer Identification and
Journal of Marketing Research, 36(2), 187–199.
Purchase Timing on Consumers’ Perceptions of Trust, Price Fairness, and Repurchase
Charness, G., Gneezy, U., & Kuhn, M. A. (2012). Experimental methods: Between-subject
Intentions. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 18(4), 87–100.
and within-subject design. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(1), 1–8.
Grewal, D., Monroe, K. B., & Krishnan, R. (1998). The Effects of Price-Comparison
Cohen, J. (1973). Eta-Squared and Partial Eta-Squared in Fixed Factor Anova Designs.
Advertising on Buyers’ Perceptions of Acquisition Value, Transaction Value, and
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 33(1), 107–112.
Behavioral Intentions. Journal of Marketing, 62(2), 46–59.
Cox, J. L. (2001). Can Differential Prices Be Fair? Journal of Product & Brand Management,
Hannak, A., Soeller, G., Lazer, D., Mislove, A., & Wilson, C. (2014). In Measuring Price
10(5), 264–275.
Discrimination and Steering on E-Commerce Web Sites (pp. 305–318). New York, NY:
Darke, P. R., & Dahl, D. W. (2003). Fairness and Discounts: The Subjective Value of a
ACM.
Bargain. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13(3), 328–338.
Haws, K. L., & Bearden, W. O. (2006). Dynamic Pricing and Consumer Fairness
Della Bitta, A. J., Monroe, K. B., & Mcginnis, J. M. (1981). Consumer Perceptions of
Perceptions. Journal of Consumer Research, 33(3), 304–311.
Comparative Price Advertisements. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(4), 416–427.
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical Mediation Analysis in the New
Dickson, P. R., & Kalapurakal, R. (1994). The Use and Perceived Fairness of Price-Setting
Millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408–420.
Rules in the Bulk Electricity Market. Journal of Economic Psychology, 15(3), 427–448.
Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process
Dinev, T., & Hart, P. (2006). An extended privacy calculus model for e-commerce
Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (Second Edition). Methodology in the Social
transactions. Information Systems Research, 17(1), 61–80.
Sciences. New York, NY, London: The Guilford Press.
Dixit, A., Braunsberger, K., Zinkhan, G. M., & Pan, Y. (2005). Information Technology-
Huang, J.-H., Chang, C.-T., & Chen, C.-Y.-H. (2005). Perceived Fairness of Pricing on the
Enhanced Pricing Strategies: Managerial and Public Policy Implications. Journal of
Internet. Journal of Economic Psychology, 26(3), 343–361.
Business Research, 58(9), 1169–1177.
Hüttel, B. A., Schumann, J. H., Mende, M., Scott, M. L., & Wagner, C. J. (2018). How
Consumers Assess Free E-Services. Journal of Service Research, 21(3), 267–283.
364
G. Hufnagel et al. Journal of Business Research 143 (2022) 346–365
Inman, J. J., & Nikolova, H. (2017). Shopper-Facing Retail Technology: A Retailer Sahay, A. (2007). How to Reap Higher Profits with Dynamic Pricing. MIT Sloan
Adoption Decision Framework Incorporating Shopper Attitudes and Privacy Management Review, 48(4), 53–60.
Concerns. Journal of Retailing, 93(1), 7–28. Samuelson, W., & Zeckhauser, R. (1988). Status Quo Bias in Decision Making. Journal of
Iyer, G. R., Miyazaki, A. D., Grewal, D., & Giordano, M. (2002). Linking Web-based Risk and Uncertainty, 1(1), 7–59.
Segmentation to Pricing Tactics. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 11(5), Shimp, T. A., & Kavas, A. (1984). The Theory of Reasoned Action Applied to Coupon
288–302. Usage. Journal of Consumer Research, 11(3), 795–809.
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. (1986). Fairness as a Constraint on Profit Shimp, T. A., & Moody, M. P. (2000). In Search of a Theoretical Explanation for the
Seeking: Entitlements in the Market. The American Economic Review, 76(4), 728–741. Credit Card Effect. Journal of Business Research, 48(1), 17–23.
Kang, H., Hahn, M., Fortin, D. R., Hyun, Y. J., & Eom, Y. (2006). Effects of Perceived Singh, J. (1988). Consumer Complaint Intentions and Behavior: Definitional and
Behavioral Control on the Consumer Usage Intention of E-Coupons. Psychology & Taxonomical Issues. Journal of Marketing, 52(1), 93–107.
Marketing, 23(10), 841–864. Stephen, A. T., & Lamberton, C. (2016). Knowledge Generation Initiative -
Kehr, F., Kowatsch, T., Wentzel, D., & Fleisch, E. (2015). Blissfully Ignorant: The Effects Understanding Digitized Customers in Digitized Environments: Introducing Six
of General Privacy Concerns, General Institutional Trust, and Affect in the Privacy Research Priorities for Academic-Industry Collaboration. Retrieved from http://
Calculus. Information Systems Journal, 25(6), 607–635. www.msi.org/articles/report-outlines-challenges-of-understanding-digitized-
Kimes, S. E. (2002). Perceived Fairness of Yield Management. Cornell Hotel and customers/.
Restaurant Administration Quarterly, 43(1), 21–30. Streitfeld, D. (2000, September 22). On the Web Price Tags Blur: What You Pay Could
Kung, M., Monroe, K. B., & Cox, J. L. (2002). Pricing on the Internet. Journal of Product & Depend on Who You Are. The Washington Post. Retrieved from https://www.
Brand Management, 11(5), 274–288. washingtonpost.com/archive/politics/2000/09/27/on-the-web-price-tags-blur/
Kuo, A., Rice, D. H., & Fennell, P. (2016). How Fitting! The Influence of Fence-Context Fit 14daea51-3a64-488f-8e6b-c1a3654773da/?utm_term=.56a68dc4b4ea.
on Price Discrimination Fairness. Journal of Business Research, 69(8), 2634–2640. Tsai, D., & Lee, H.-C. (2007). Will You Care When You Pay More? The Negative Side of
Lii, Y.-S., & Sy, E. (2009). Internet Differential Pricing: Effects on Consumer Price Targeted Promotions. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 16(7), 481–491.
Perception, Emotions, and Behavioral Responses. Computers in Human Behavior, 25 Tyler, T. R. (1989). The Psychology of Procedural Justice: A Test of the Group-Value
(3), 770–777. Model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(5), 830–838.
Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The Social Psychology of Procedural Justice. New York, Venkatesan, R., & Farris, P. W. (2012). Measuring and managing returns from retailer-
NY: Plenum Press. customized coupon campaigns. Journal of Marketing, 76(1), 76–94.
Mahadevan, J. (2010). Wahrgenommene Preisfairness bei Personenbezogener Victor, V., Fekete Farkas, M., & Lakner, Z. (2019). Consumer Attitude and Reaction
Preisdifferenzierung (Doctoral Dissertation). Vallendar: WHU - Otto Beisheim School of towards Personalised Pricing in the E-Commerce Sector. Journal of Management and
Management. Marketing Review, 6(2), 140–148.
Malc, D., Mumel, D., & Pisnik, A. (2016). Exploring Price Fairness Perceptions and Their Wang, Y., & Krishna, A. (2012). Enticing for Me but Unfair to Her: Can Targeted Pricing
Influence on Consumer Behavior. Journal of Business Research, 69(9), 3693–3697. Evoke Socially Conscious Behavior? Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 433–442.
Malhotra, N. K., Kim, S. S., & Agrarwal, J. (2004). Internet Users’ Information Privacy Weible, R. J. (1993). Privacy and Data: An Empirical Study of the Influence and Types and
Concerns (IUIPC): The Construct, the Scale, and a Causal Model. Information Systems Data and Situational Context upon Privacy Perceptions (Doctoral Dissertation).
Research, 15(4), 336–355. Starkville, MS: Mississippi State University.
Mallinckrodt, B., Abraham, W. T., Wei, M., & Russell, D. W. (2006). Advances in testing Weisstein, F. L., Monroe, K. B., & Kukar-Kinney, M. (2013). Effects of Price Framing on
the statistical significance of mediation effects. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53 Consumers’ Perceptions of Online Dynamic Pricing Practices. Journal of the Academy
(3), 372. of Marketing Science, 41(5), 501–514.
Marmorstein, H., Grewal, D., & Fishe, R. P. (1992). The value of time spent in price- Wu, C.-C., Liu, Y.-F., Chen, Y.-J., & Wang, C.-J. (2012). Consumer Responses to Price
comparison shopping: Survey and experimental evidence. Journal of Consumer Discrimination: Discriminating Bases, Inequality Status, and Information Disclosure
Research, 19(1), 52–61. Timing Influences. Journal of Business Research, 65(1), 106–116.
Marn, M. V., & Rosiello, R. L. (1992). Managing Price, Gaining Profit. Harvard Business Xia, L., Monroe, K. B., & Cox, J. L. (2004). The Price Is Unfair! A Conceptual Framework
Review, 70(5), 84–94. of Price Fairness Perceptions. Journal of Marketing, 68(4), 1–15.
Martin, W. C., Ponder, N., & Lueg, J. E. (2009). Price Fairness Perceptions and Customer Youyou, W., Kosinski, M., & Stillwell, D. (2015). Computer-Based Personality Judgments
Loyalty in a Retail Context. Journal of Business Research, 62(6), 588–593. are More Accurate than Those Made by Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy
Maxwell, S. (1999). The Social Norms of Discrete Consumer Exchange: Classification and of Sciences, 112(4), 1036–1040.
Quantification. American Journal of Economics and Sociology, 58(4), 999–1018. Zaichkowsky, J. L. (1985). Measuring the Involvement Construct. Journal of Consumer
Maxwell, S. (2002). Rule-Based Price Fairness and Its Effect on Willingness to Purchase. Research, 12(3), 341–352.
Journal of Economic Psychology, 23(2), 191–212. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and
Maxwell, S., & Garbarino, E. (2010). The Identification of Social Norms of Price Truths about Mediation Analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.
Discrimination on the Internet. Journal of Product & Brand Management, 19(3),
218–224.
Gerrit Hufnagel is a former research and teaching assistant at the Institute of Market-Based
Mende, M., Scott, M. L., & Bolton, L. E. (2018). All That Glitters Is Not Gold. Journal of
Management at the Munich School of Management within Ludwig-Maximilians-University
Service Research, 21(4), 405–420.
in Munich. He earned his Ph.D. from Ludwig-Maximilians-University in 2020. His research
Mikians, J., Gyarmati, L., Erramilli, V., & Laoutaris, N. (2013). Crowd-Assisted Search for
is focused on personalized price discrimination and customer-centric growth. Gerrit
Price Discrimination in E-Commerce. In K. Almeroth (Ed.), CoNext 2013 - Conference
Hufnagel presented his work at renowned international conferences such as the Australian
on Emerging Networking Experiments and Technologies (pp. 1–6). New York, NY: ACM.
& New Zealand Academy of Management (ANZAM) Conference, the Global Marketing
Milne, G. R., Pettinico, G., Hajjat, F. M., & Markos, E. (2017). Information Sensitivity
Conference (GMC), or the Association for Marketing & Health Care Research Conference
Typology: Mapping the Degree and Type of Risk Consumers Perceive in Personal
(AMHCR), where he was awarded with the best student paper award.
Data Sharing. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 51(1), 133–161.
Niemann, A., & Schwaiger, M. (2016). In Consumers’ Expectations of Fair Data Collection
and Usage - A Mixed Method Analysis (pp. 3646–3655). Piscataway, NJ: IEEE. Manfred Schwaiger is full professor of business administration, head of the Institute of
Obermiller, C., Arnesen, D., & Cohen, M. (2012). Customized Pricing: Win-Win or End Market-Based Management, and Dean of Studies at the Munich School of Management
Run? Drake Management Review, 2(1), 12–28. within Ludwig-Maximilians-University in Munich, for which he has been serving as a dean
Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and Intention in Everyday Life: The Multiple from 2003-2005. He earned his Ph.D. (1993) and his post-doctoral degree (“Habilitation”,
Processes by Which Past Behavior Predicts Future Behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 1998) from Augsburg University. His main research interests cover the management of
124(1), 54–74. intangible assets (esp. reputation and trust), return on marketing, consumer behavior,
Phelps, J., Nowak, G., & Ferrell, E. (2000). Privacy Concerns and Consumer Willingness corporate communications, and market & trend research. His academic work has been
to Provide Personal Information. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 19(1), 27–41. published in leading journals in the field (e.g. Strategic Management Journal, Journal of
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS Procedures for Estimating Indirect the Academy of Marketing Science), and has been awarded with a number of best paper
Effects in Simple Mediation Models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & awards. Manfred Schwaiger has been a member of the Board of Directors of the German
Computers, 36(4), 717–731. Academic Association for Business Research (VHB), and he still serves as a member of the
Priester, A., Robbert, T., & Roth, S. (2020). A special price just for you: Effects of International Advisory Board of EMLyon, Grande École. Moreover, he is editorial review
personalized dynamic pricing on consumer fairness perceptions. Journal of Revenue board member of the Journal for Public Policy and Marketing, the Journal of Advertising,
and Pricing Management, 1–14. and the International Journal of Advertising.
Richards, T. J., Liaukonyte, J., & Streletskaya, N. A. (2016). Personalized Pricing and
Price Fairness. International Journal of Industrial Organization, 44, 138–153.
Louisa Weritz is research and teaching assistant as well as doctoral candidate at the
Rondan-Cataluña, F. J., & Martin-Ruiz, D. (2011). Moderating Effects in Consumers’
Institute of Market-Based Management at the Munich School of Management within
Perceptions of Price Unfairness. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 10(5), 245–254.
Ludwig-Maximilians-University in Munich. She received a Bachelor of Science and Master
Rosencrance, L. (2000). September 11). Customers Balk at Variable DVD Pricing: Amazon.
of Science in Marketing and Management from Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich.
com Claims It was Part of a Test. Retrieved from https://www.computerworld.com/
Her research is focused on pricing strategies in e-commerce and customer trust.
article/2597065/retail-it/customers-balk-at–-variable-dvd-pricing.html.
365