collabra_2021_7_1_29766
collabra_2021_7_1_29766
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits.
Collabra: Psychology, 7(1). https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.29766
Personality Psychology
Collabra: Psychology
Vol. 7, Issue 1, 2021
Personality traits are stable, but also amenable to change 2012). Fewer studies have investigated the associations be-
(Roberts et al., 2006, 2017). Starting from early childhood, tween parenting and child Big Five personality traits, which
several factors are thought to influence personality devel- are relatively enduring, automatic patterns of thoughts,
opment, such as life events and long-term person-environ- feelings, and behaviors manifested in specific contexts
ment transactions (Fraley & Roberts, 2005). Influences on (Roberts, 2009). Most of these studies have examined the
personality encompass factors ranging from family to peer, links between parenting and child Big Five traits using
school, neighborhood and cultural contexts. Due to the cross-sectional methods, while very few studies have em-
amount of time and energy invested in raising a child, it ployed longitudinal methods.
is logical to consider parents-child relationships as central We extend past research using data from a longitudinal
to the development of child personality. Starting from in- study of children transitioning to adolescence to examine
fancy, parents organize the child’s home and environment, the co-development of four parenting dimensions and child
help the child regulate their affect and moral actions, teach, Big Five personality traits. The parenting dimensions in our
and provide opportunities for children to learn (Bornstein, study, which are parental academic involvement, parental
2001). Parents are the most consistent people with whom structure, parental cultural stimulation, and parental goals
children spend their time. In short, all signs point to the are not widely explored in the literature. We focus on the
idea that parents play a crucial role in child personality de- Big Five personality traits for multiple reasons. The Big Five
velopment, but is that really the case? model is among the most widely used and well-established
Past studies have examined the associations between model of personality trait structure; therefore, it is a useful
parenting and an array of child outcomes such as depres- framework for conducting systematic research. Moreover,
sion (e.g., McLeod et al., 2007), behavioral problems (e.g., the Big Five are used to explore child, adolescent, and adult
Pachter et al., 2006), temperament (e.g., Kiang et al., 2004), personality, which facilitates comparisons across develop-
and academic achievement (e.g., Cheung & Pomerantz, mental periods. Furthermore, we know very little about the
a [email protected]
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
factors associated with differential development of the Big practices (Darling & Steinberg, 1993). This means that
Five traits during childhood and adolescence, a gap in the parental goals should be examined as a separate component
literature that we aimed to fill. In the present study, we to wholly understand parental socialization (Darling &
use a longitudinal design to study the association between Steinberg, 1993).
parenting and child personality to better understand di-
rectional associations. Specifically, assessing parenting and Theoretical Models of Parent-Child Relationships
child personality across years allows us to understand their
developmental trajectories, as well as how their trajectories Parental socialization models propose a wide variety of
are related to each other. Longitudinal designs are also ca- different mechanisms for how parents influence child per-
pable of delineating the bidirectional associations, which sonality development. For example, Social Learning Theory
are characteristic of parent-child relationships. Above and (Bandura & Walters, 1963) proposes that children learn be-
beyond these strengths, our study uses a sample size with haviors through observation and imitation. Bandura sug-
adequate statistical power, and uses data from multiple in- gested that children tend to observe behaviors, and sub-
formants to eliminate shared method variance. Below, we sequently encode and imitate them. Moreover, Attachment
review the literature on dimensions of parenting, theoreti- Theory (Bowlby, 1969) proposes that the child’s early expe-
cal models of parent-child relationships, and existing liter- riences with parents (or caregivers in general) shape their
Collabra: Psychology 2
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Existing Studies on the Associations between correlations were found between initial levels of child
Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits agreeableness and changes in overreactive parenting, as
well as positive correlations between initial levels of over-
Although the associations between parenting and a wide reactive parenting and changes in child’s agreeableness and
range of child characteristics and outcomes have been in- emotional stability. Also, researchers found that increases
vestigated in prior research, less has been done on the lon- in overreactive parenting were related to decreases in child
gitudinal associations between parenting and child Big Five agreeableness and emotional stability. Despite its relatively
personality traits. Some studies have examined these as- sophisticated design, the main limitation of the study was
sociations using cross-sectional data. For example, it was that it included only 290 participants, which may not pro-
found that parental warmth was positively associated with vide adequate statistical power to detect complex, multi-
child extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, open- variate associations such as these. The second study (N =
ness to experience, and negatively associated with child 400-500) used a latent difference score model to investigate
neuroticism (Fadda et al., 2015; Lianos, 2015; Nyhus & bidirectional associations between parental warmth, over-
Webley, 2013). Furthermore, it was found that parental be- reactivity, and the child’s Big Five traits from age 8.5 to 10.5
havioral control was positively correlated with child consci- (van den Akker et al., 2014). It was found that changes in
entiousness (Nyhus & Webley, 2013). parental warmth were positively associated with changes
Collabra: Psychology 3
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
1 Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities of the scales before and after removing the negatively worded items are reported on Open science Frame-
work https://osf.io/qnjp3/?view_only=a2eaa14f0ae640b1ab7572026e4821e7).
2 Rieger (2018) used the same child personality scales in the TRAIN dataset. He also omitted the reverse coded items. To check for robust-
ness, he re-ran the analyses with the complete set of items. The results remained unchanged.
3 We reran the analyses with personality scales that include all the items. Results were similar. They can be found on Open Science Frame-
work https://osf.io/qnjp3/?view_only=a2eaa14f0ae640b1ab7572026e4821e7
Collabra: Psychology 4
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Collabra: Psychology 5
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
to rule out the possibility that the association between par- to listwise or pairwise deletion methods (Graham, 2009).
enting and child personality traits is due to parents’ per- FIML estimates the model parameters using all available
sonality traits. We also controlled for multiple testing by data. The function COMPLEX in Mplus 5.1 (Muthén &
adjusting the alpha level of statistical significance using Muthén, 2007) was used to account for the nested structure
Bonferroni’s correction. The conventional alpha level was of the data. Model fit was inspected using chi-square, RM-
divided by the total number of tests (.05 / 20 = .003). A sen- SEA, and CFI statistics. Good model fit was inferred when
sitivity power analysis was conducted to compute the raw χ2 is low and not statistically significant, RMSEA is below
correlations between parenting and child Big Five traits that .06, and CFI is above .95 (L.-T. Hu & Bentler, 1999).
can be detected for power of .80. The analysis revealed that
effect sizes with magnitudes of .06 could be detected, given Results
our sample size and adjusted alpha level. Descriptive Statistics
All longitudinal analyses were conducted using Mplus
5.1 software (Muthén & Muthén, 2007). The analytic plan Table 1 provides the means and standard deviations of
was preregistered on the Open Science Framework parenting, child personality, and control variables. Table 2
(https://osf.io/ shows correlations between observed parenting and child
4
qnjp3/?view_only=a2eaa14f0ae640b1ab7572026e4821e7). personality variables across time. In total, the magnitudes
Example scripts of the analyses are also available on the of the correlations between parenting variables and child
Open Science Framework. Missing values were handled us- personality were small, averaging .05.
ing full information maximum likelihood (FIML) procedure,
which provides less biased parameter estimates compared
4 The preregistered hypotheses and analytic plan are found in the Introduction and Methods documents on OSF.
Collabra: Psychology 6
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Table 2. Correlations between Parenting and Child Personality Variables in TRAIN Dataset
E1 E2 E3 E4 A1 A2 A3 A4 C1 C2 C3 C4 N1 N2 N3 N4 O1 O2 O3 O4
Inv1 .09 .03 .07 .07 .06 .06 .06 .03 .12 .12 .07 .10 .01 -.03 -.06 -.03 .07 .04 .03 .02
Inv4 .10 .06 .08 .09 .08 .11 .05 .08 .12 .12 .11 .12 .08 -.02 -.03 -.04 .08 .05 .06 .07
Inv=Involvement; Str=Structure; CS=Cultural Stimulation; PG= Parental Goals; E=Extraversion; A=Agreeableness; C=Conscientiousness; N=Neuroticism; O=Openness to Experience
Bold Font: p <.001
Collabra: Psychology 7
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Table 3. Model Fit Indices of Bivariate Latent Growth Models Fitted to TRAIN Dataset
Results of Longitudinal Analyses was small-to-medium, corresponding to .45 and .60 stan-
dardized units of change across time respectively. The vari-
Details of measurement invariance tests are provided in ances of the parenting slopes represent the existence of
Appendix B. These analyses showed that changes in model inter-individual differences in changes in parenting. The
fit indices of child personality variables across the nested variance of the slope of parental academic involvement was
models were not larger than the recommended cutoffs, and relatively larger than the variance of parental structure,
therefore, the child Big Five domains exhibited strong in- which means that there was more variation in changes in
variance across waves. As for the parenting variables, parental involvement trajectories across the two assess-
changes in the fit indices between metric and scalar models ment points. Notably, the mean slopes of parental cultural
were slightly larger than the recommended cutoffs, except stimulation and parental goals were not statistically signif-
for parental goals. This means that full scalar invariance icantly different from zero.
was not supported. Instead, partial scalar invariance held In addition to providing information about changes in
after freeing one or more parameters in each model. parenting, Table 4 shows the results of changes in children’s
After measurement invariance was tested, univariate Big Five traits across time. As children were getting older,
growth models were fit to the child personality variables to they became less conscientious and less open to experience,
examine whether linear or quadratic trends were a better as shown by the negative slopes. The rates of change were
representation of the data. None of the quadratic models small, ranging between -0.05 and -0.08. These numbers cor-
showed significant improvement over the linear models respond to .37 and .31 standardized units of change across
based on Chen’s (2007) criteria. Therefore, linear models for time. Interestingly, the mean slopes of child extraversion,
personality variables were retained. As for parenting vari- agreeableness, and neuroticism were not statistically sig-
ables, linear trends were assumed as two assessment points nificantly different from zero.
were available only. Results of the univariate analyses are It is important to note that the examination of individual
reported in Appendix C. differences in change necessitates the existence of reliable
Following that, second-order bivariate latent growth variance in change. The slope variance parameter is typi-
models were fit to the data. Table 3 provides model fit in- cally inspected for statistically significant variance to justify
dices for each model. All models demonstrated good fit. Af- examining the correlations of change over time. In the cur-
ter testing model fit, the means and variances of the bi- rent case, all parenting and child personality variables ex-
variate growth models were examined. The first and second hibited statistically significant variance in slopes over time,
columns of Table 4 provides the means and variances of ini- justifying the examination of predictors and correlates of
tial levels of parenting and child personality variables. We individual differences in change.
also formally tested for change over time by examining the Next, we tested the concurrent associations between
average slope values for each variable, as shown in the third parenting and child personality (intercept to intercept cor-
and fourth columns of Table 4. Results show that parental relation) and the associations between changes in parent-
academic involvement and parental structure decreased ing dimensions and changes in child personality over time
across time as shown by the negative slope estimates (slope (slope to slope correlations). We follow two approaches for
= -0.21, p < .003; slope = -0.17, p < .003, respectively). This interpreting the results of the longitudinal analyses. In the
means that as children were growing up, parents were be- first approach, we highlight the statistically significant re-
coming less involved in their children’s academic environ- sults only, and ignore statistically non-significant ones. In
ments and provided them with less structure. The magni- the second approach, we interpret the effect sizes regard-
tude of the rates of change in these parenting dimensions less of whether they reached statistical significance or not.
Collabra: Psychology 8
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Intercept Slope
Mean Variance Mean Variance
Parental Involvement 3.35 .24 -.21 .22
Parental Structure 3.78 .05 -.17 .08
Parental Cultural Stimulation 1.52 .22 -.05 .13
Parental Goals 3.79 .36 -.08 .04
Child Extraversion 3.74 .21 -.001 .02
Child Agreeableness 3.71 .26 -.02 .03
Child Conscientiousness 3.79 .37 -.08 .04
Child Neuroticism 2.26 .32 -.02 .03
Child Openness to Experience 3.44 .32 -.05 .03
Column 1 in Table 5 shows the correlations between par- Regarding the correlations between changes in parenting
enting and child personality intercepts at Time 1. Following and changes in child personality, none of the correlations
the first approach for interpreting the results, only four cor- were statistically significant at p < .003. However, the mag-
relations were significant at the adjusted alpha level of p < nitude of several slope-slope correlations exceeded .10 in-
.003 out of 20 correlations. There was a significant positive cluding: changes in parental involvement and changes in
association between parental involvement and child consci- child conscientiousness (r = .12) , neuroticism (r = -.14), and
entiousness (r = .13, p < .003); a significant positive asso- openness to experience (r = .10); changes in parental struc-
ciation between parental structure and child agreeableness ture and changes in agreeableness (r = .12) and neuroti-
(r = .13, p < .003); an association between parental cultural cism (r = -.19); changes in parental cultural stimulation and
stimulation and child conscientiousness (r = .15, p < .003), changes in extraversion (r = -.15); changes in parental goals
and an association between parental goals and child agree- and changes in neuroticism (r = -.15).
ableness (r = .10, p < .003). No other statistically significant
findings at p < .003 were found, suggesting that there were Discussion
few associations between parenting and child personality at
Time 1. The main goal of our research was to explore the asso-
Focusing on the magnitudes of the associations rather ciations among four parenting dimensions and children’s
than their statistical significance, we found that all corre- Big Five personality traits. The present study has several
lations were small or very small. In general, the four sta- strengths. First, we used a large dataset (N= 3,880) that pro-
tistically significant results that we observed were small in vided adequate statistical power to detect effects if they ex-
size, ranging between .10 and .15. The average correlation isted. Second, we examined the associations between multi-
between parenting and child personality intercepts was .08, ple parenting measures and child Big Five personality traits.
and few correlations exceeded .10. Third, we went beyond traditional cross-sectional methods
through fitting bivariate latent growth models. These mod-
Intercept-Slope Correlations els captured an important aspect about parenting and child
personality, which is their changeability across time, and
We examined the correlations between intercepts and allowed for examining the correlations between these
slopes as shown in Table 5. None of the correlations be- changes. Furthermore, bivariate latent growth models pro-
tween parenting at Time 1 and changes in child personality vided information about the extent to which change trajec-
were statistically significant at p < .003. Similarly, none of tories were uniform or variable across individuals.
the correlations between personality at Time 1 and changes There are many theories that suggest parenting and child
in parenting were statistically significant at p < .003. development are related to each other, including Social
Focusing on the magnitudes of the associations instead Learning Theory, Attachment Theory, and the Psycholog-
of statistical significance, we found that the correlations be- ical Resources Principle. However, these theories do not
tween parenting at Time 1 and changes in child personal- make specific predictions about the links between parenting
ity between Time 1 and Time 4 were also very small in all and child personality development per se. We expected that
models, ranging from .02 to .07. Comparably, the correla- positive parenting practices would be associated with the
tions between personality at Time 1 and changes in par- positive development of child extraversion, agreeableness,
enting were also very small. Only the correlation between conscientiousness, emotionality stability, and openness to
changes in parental involvement and neuroticism exceeded experience, whereas negative parenting practices would be
.10. related to negative trait development. Controlling for child
Collabra: Psychology 9
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Table 5. Results of Correlations between Intercepts and Slopes of Parenting and Child Personality Variables in
TRAIN Dataset
Parental Involvement
i1 with i2 i1 with s2 i2 with s1 s1 with s2
Extraversion .09[.02,.16] -.04[-.11,.04] -.01[-.11,.08] .08[-.02,.18]
Agreeableness .11[.03,.19] -.06[-.17,.04] .01[-.09,.11] .06[-.07,.19]
Conscientiousness .13[.07,.19] -.03[-.11,.05] -.02[-.10,.07] .12 [0,.23]
Neuroticism -.02[-.09,.04] -.04[-.15,.07] .13[.04,.22] -.14[-.27, -.01]
Openness to Experience .05[-.02,.12] -.07[-.15,.02] -.002[-.09,.09] .10[-.01,.21]
Parental Structure
i1 with i2 i1 with s2 i2 with s1 s1 with s2
Extraversion .08[-.006,.16] -.06[-.16,.04] -.003[-.10,.10] .04[-.08,.17]
i1 = intercept of parenting variable; s1= slope of parenting variable; i2=intercept of child personality variable; s2= slope of child personality variable
Bold font: statistically significant at p < .003
age, gender, socioeconomic status, and parents Big Five few individual differences in how parenting practices
personality traits, along with corrections for multiple test- change over time.
ing, we found several interesting findings regarding the lon- Second, child personality also changed across time. Chil-
gitudinal associations between parenting and child person- dren perceived themselves as less conscientious and less
ality. open to experience. The decrease in child conscientiousness
First, parenting changed across time, which is consistent shows that adolescents were not developing in the direction
with results of previous studies (e.g., van den Akker et al., of maturation, which is consistent with findings of previous
2010). The general trend was a decrease in parenting behav- research (Atherton et al., 2020; van den Akker et al., 2014).
iors. As children entered adolescence, parents became less Another thing to note is that the rates of change and vari-
involved in their children’s academic lives and provided less ability in change were modest, which is also consistent with
structure. It is understandable that parents’ roles change past studies.
during this period because adolescence is characterized by Third, surprisingly, there was a preponderance of sta-
children’s striving for autonomy and independence (Galam- tistically non-significant results when examining the asso-
bos & Costigan, 2003). The strongest decrease was in ciations among parenting practices and child personality.
parental involvement in their child’s academics. Parental After adjusting the alpha level using the conservative Bon-
goals and parental cultural stimulation showed no mean- ferroni’s adjustment, only four correlations were statisti-
level change across time. An important finding to note is cally significant. The four significant correlations were be-
that the variances of parenting variables were generally tween initial levels of parenting and initial levels of child
small, ranging from .04 and .22, indicating that there were personality (i.e., concurrent associations) and their magni-
Collabra: Psychology 10
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
tudes ranged from .10 to .15. The correlations were between date genes” that explain large amount of variance in the
parental involvement and child conscientiousness; parental phenotype. It appears that an analogous situation holds
structure and child agreeableness; parental cultural stimu- for personality development in childhood and adolescence.
lation and child conscientiousness; parental goals and child Much like the threads of a tapestry, environmental factors
agreeableness. combine in an intricate and complex way to drive personal-
Because focusing on statistical significance only can ob- ity development, and each factor is an essential, yet small
scure important information about the phenomenon under thread that contributes to the tapestry that is personality.
investigation (Fraley & Marks, 2007), we also interpreted Third, and more practically, our findings should not dis-
the magnitudes of the associations regardless of their sta- courage research on, and the implementation of, parenting
tistical significance. In general, the associations between interventions. Effect sizes that are modest at the individual
initial levels or changes in parenting and child personality level could be consequential at the population level. The
were small or very small. The average correlations between modest change in parenting and child personality that par-
the initial levels of parenting and child personality was .08. enting interventions can do are important when multiply-
The average correlation between initial levels of parenting ing the effect by the number of people who underwent the
and changes in child personality was .04. The average corre- interventions. It could also be comforting for parents to re-
lation between changes in child personality and initial lev- alize that parenting goes both ways. The link between par-
Collabra: Psychology 11
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
the same level of granularity. Fifth, although using longi- has little contribution to make towards personality develop-
tudinal data helps us to infer some directionality, we did ment.
not randomly assign participants to experimental condi-
tions and therefore, causal inference in the strictest sense is
not possible. Sixth, although we controlled for parents’ per-
sonality traits to rule out the possibility that the association Contributions
between parenting and child personality traits is due to par-
ents’ personality traits, it could be that parents’ personality Contributed to conception and design: Mona Ayoub
is part of the process linking parenting and child personal- Contributed to acquisition of data: Richard Göllner, Ul-
ity (see Rohrer, 2018). rich Trautwein
Contributed to analysis and interpretation of data: Mona
Conclusion Ayoub, Bo Zhang
Drafted and/or revised the article: Mona Ayoub, Bo
The longitudinal associations between multiple parent- Zhang, Richard Göllner, Olivia E. Atherton, Ulrich
ing dimensions such as parental involvement, structure, Trautwein, and Brent W. Roberts
cultural stimulation, goals, and children’s Big Five person- Approved the submitted version for publication: Mona
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License
(CCBY-4.0). View this license’s legal deed at http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 and legal code at http://creativecom-
mons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode for more information.
Collabra: Psychology 12
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as
(1978). Patterns of attachment. Lawrence Erlbaum context: An integrative model. Psychological Bulletin,
Associates, Inc. 113(3), 487–496. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.1
Atherton, O. E., Lawson, K. M., & Robins, R. W. (2020). 13.3.487
The development of effortful control from late Egberts, M. R., Prinzie, P., Deković, M., de Haan, A. D.,
childhood to young adulthood. Journal of Personality & van den Akker, A. L. (2015). The prospective
and Social Psychology, 119(2), 417–456. https://doi.or relationship between child personality and perceived
g/10.1037/pspp0000283 parenting: Mediation by parental sense of
Atherton, O. E., & Schofield, T. J. (2021). Personality competence. Personality and Individual Differences, 77,
and parenting. Handbook of Personality: Theory and 193–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.046
Research, 352. Fadda, D., Scalas, L. F., & Meleddu, M. (2015).
Ayoub, M., Gosling, S. D., Potter, J., Shanahan, M., & Contribution of personal and environmental factors
Collabra: Psychology 13
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Hu, Z., Wang, Z., & Xu, S. (2012). An infinitesimal Power, T. G. (2013). Parenting dimensions and styles: A
model for quantitative trait genomic value prediction. brief history and recommendations for future
PLoS One, 7(7), e41336. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ research. Childhood Obesity, 9(s1), S14–S21. https://d
al.pone.0041336 oi.org/10.1089/chi.2013.0034
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait Rieger, S. (2018). Facilitating the Understanding of
taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical Personality: The Usefulness of Unifying Two Existing
perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Approaches [Doctoral dissertation]. Universität
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. Tübingen.
102–138). Guilford Press. Roberts, B. W. (2009). Back to the future: Personality
Kiang, L., Moreno, A. J., & Robinson, J. L. (2004). and assessment and personality development. Journal
Maternal preconceptions about parenting predict of Research in Personality, 43(2), 137–145. https://do
child temperament, maternal sensitivity, and i.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.015
children’s empathy. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), Roberts, B. W., Luo, J., Briley, D. A., Chow, P. I., Su, R., &
1081–1092. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1 Hill, P. L. (2017). A systematic review of personality
081 trait change through intervention. Psychological
Lang, F. R., Lüdtke, O., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2001). Bulletin, 143(2), 117–141. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul
Collabra: Psychology 14
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
I have enough time and energy to Child Big Five Personality Traits
1. talk intensively about school day
I am someone who is
2. take care that child is doing his/her homework
3. go through schoolwork with child 1. is talkative, likes to talk (E1)
4. get involved in child school 2. tends to criticize others (A1R)
5. go to parents’ evenings 3. does the tasks thoroughly (C1)
6. study classwork with child 4. is eclectic (O1)
5. is depressed (N1)
Parental Structure 6. is original, develops new ideas (O2)
Collabra: Psychology 15
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Collabra: Psychology 16
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Measurement Invariance
Collabra: Psychology 17
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Collabra: Psychology 18
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
Appendix C
Table C. Model Fit Indices of Linear and Quadratic Univariate Models (TRAIN Dataset)
Linear Quadratic
CFI RMSEA SRMR CFI RMSEA SRMR
Child Extraversion .96 .03 .03 .96 .03 .03
Child Agreeableness .99 .02 .03 .98 .02 .03
Child Conscientiousness .95 .04 .04 .95 .04 .04
Child Neuroticism .96 .03 .04 .96 .03 .03
Child Openness to Experience .95 .03 .04 .95 .03 .04
Collabra: Psychology 19
Longitudinal Associations Between Parenting and Child Big Five Personality Traits
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS
Collabra: Psychology 20