115609_Chapter 6 Smaller
115609_Chapter 6 Smaller
3. Let's talk
3. Reading and writing are the major focus; little or no systematic attention
is paid to speaking and listening.
4. Vocabulary selection is based solely on the reading texts used, and words
are taught through bilingual word lists, dictionary study, and memorization.
1. Get it right from the beginning
Characteristics of the Grammar Translation approach (Cont.):
5. The sentence is the basic unit of teaching and language practice. Much
of the lesson is devoted to translating sentences from and into the target
language.
6. Accuracy is emphasized.
Its purpose:
They may answer comprehension questions based on the passage, often in their L1.
The teacher draws attention to a specific grammar rule that is illustrated by the text
(e.g. a certain verb form).
Following this, the students are given an exercise in which they are asked to practice
the grammatical rule by filling in the blanks with the appropriate verb form in series
of sentences that may or may not be related to the text they have read and
translated.
1. Get it right from the beginning
The Audiolingual Method:
• Even though the focus is on the oral language, students rarely use the
language spontaneously.
S1 And uh, in the afternoon, uh, I come home and uh, uh, I uh, washing my dog.
T I wash.
S1 My dog.
S1 No. [ben]
Research findings
The grammar translation approach is useful for the intensive study of grammar and vocabulary and
for understanding important texts.
The audiolingual approach was used successfully with highly motivated adult learners in training
programmes for government personnel in the United States.
However, there is little research to support such approaches for students in ordinary school
programmes who have different levels of motivation and aptitude.
The traditional grammar translation and audiolingual methods frequently failed to produce fluency
and accuracy in second language learners.
Supporters of the communicative based approaches were against the structure based approach.
Why? What is their argument?
1. language is not learned by the gradual accumulation of one item after another (e.g. Isolated
vocabulary lists and grammatical rules)
2. errors are a natural and valuable part of the language learning process.
3. the motivation of learners is often ruined by an insistence on correctness in the earliest stages of
second language learning.
Research findings
They argue that allowing learners too much 'freedom' without correction
and explicit instruction will lead to early fossilization of errors.
1. Get it right from the beginning
Research findings
• It is difficult to test the hypothesis that focus on form in the early stages of second language learning will, in
the long run, lead to better results than those achieved when the focus is on meaning in the early stages.
• To test that hypothesis, it would be necessary t0 compare groups that are similar in all respects except for the
type of instruction they receive.
• However, it is not easy for researchers to find proper comparison groups. Why?
On the one hand, there are many parts of the world where one finds only structure-based approaches to
language teaching. In these settings, there are no classrooms where the teaching places the primary
emphasis on meaning in the early stages of learning.
On the other hand, in other parts of the world, it is very difficult to make comparisons with classrooms
that are primarily structure-based because such classes simply do not exist (communicative teaching is
used instead)
1. Get it right from the beginning
Research findings
None the less, some findings from second language classroom research can help us in
assessing the effect of instruction that is strongly oriented to the 'Get it right from the
beginning' approach.
These include :
Procedure:
A 'culture' group
A CONTROL GROUP.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 13: Grammar plus communicative practice
Procedure (Cont.):
• All groups received about four hours per week of audio lingual instruction where the focus was on the
practice and manipulation of grammatical forms.
The 'culture' group had an hour devoted to activities, conducted in English, designed to 'foster
an awareness of the French language and culture through films, music, and art'.
The control group had an hour in the language laboratory doing grammar and pronunciation
drills similar to those they did in their regular class periods.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 13: Grammar plus communicative practice
Procedure (Cont.):
• Tests to measure learners' linguistic and communicative abilities were administered before and after instruction.
The tests of linguistic competence included a variety of grammar tests, teachers' evaluations of speaking
skills, and course grades.
The tests of communicative competence included measures of fluency and of the ability to understand and
transmit information in a variety of tasks, which included:
Findings:
1. There were no SIGNIFICANT DIFFERENCES between groups on the linguistic competence measures.
2. However, the communicative group scored significantly higher than the other two groups on the four
communicative tests developed for the study.
Conclusion:
1. Second language programmes that focus only on accuracy and form do not give students sufficient
opportunity to develop communication abilities in a second language.
2. Opportunities for freer communication did not cause learners to do less well on measures of linguistic
accuracy.
2 Just listen ... and read
• This proposal is based on the hypothesis that language acquisition
takes place when learners are exposed to comprehensible input
through listening and/or reading.
• Stephen Krashen
• Krashen's hypothesis that the one essential requirement for second
language acquisition is the availability of comprehensible input is
explored in the instructional setting described in Example 3, p. 144.
2 Just listen ... and read
• 'Just listen ... and read' is a controversial proposal for second language
teaching.
• It not only says that second language learners do not need to drill and practice
language in order to learn it, but also that they do not need to speak at all.
• However, graded reading materials specially designed for adult ESL learners are
increasingly available.
• Marlise Horst (2005) used simplified readers in a study of vocabulary development among adult
immigrants who were enrolled in an ESL programme in a community centre in Montreal, Canada.
Procedure:
• The twenty-one participants represented several language backgrounds and proficiency levels.
• In addition to the activities of their regular ESL class, students chose simplified readers that were
made available in a class library.
• Over a six-week period, students took books home and read them on their own.
• Horst developed individualized vocabulary measures so that learning could be assessed in terms of
the books each student actually read.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 15: Reading for words
Findings:
1. There was vocabulary growth attributable to reading, even over this short period.
Conclusion
• As we saw in Chapter 4, when we interact in ordinary conversations, we tend to use mainly the 1,000 or
2,000 most frequent words.
• It is in reading a variety of texts that students are most likely to encounter new vocabulary.
• The benefit of simplified readers is that students are likely to encounter a reasonable number of new words.
• This increases the likelihood that they can figure out the meaning of new words (or perhaps be motivated
to look them up).
• If the new words occur often enough, students may remember them when they encounter them in a new
context.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 16: Total physical response
• One of the best-known variations on the 'Just listen ... and read'
proposal is the second language teaching approach called 'Total
Physical Response' (TPR).
• In TPR classes, students (children or adults) participate in activities in which they hear a series 0f
commands in the TL.
• e.g. 'stand up', 'put the book or the table', 'walk to the door'
• At a more advanced level, they may act out skits as the teacher provides a description of an event or
encounter.
• For a substantial number of hours of instruction, students are not required to say anything. They
simply listen and show their comprehension by their actions.
• When students begin to speak, they take over the role of the teacher and give commands as well as
following them.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 16: Total physical response
Although Krashen has expressed his enthusiasm for this approach to teaching,
it differs from his comprehensible input hypothesis in one important way.
• In TPR instruction, the vocabulary and structures learners are exposed to are
carefully graded and organized.
• The material gradually increases in complexity so that each new lesson builds on
the ones before.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 16: Total physical response
• Asher's research showed that students could develop quite advanced levels of
comprehension in the language without engaging in oral practice.
• It is clear that there are limitations to the kind of language students learn to
produce in such an environment.
• Nevertheless, Asher's research shows that for beginners, this kind of active
listening gives learners a good start.
The emphasis is on getting the learners to notice language forms in the input, not on
getting them to practise producing the forms.
• Martha Trahey and Lydia White (1993) carried out a study with young French-
speaking learners (aged 10-12) in INTENSIVE ESL classes in Quebec.
• These students were in ESL classes in which instruction was communicative and
task-based.
Procedure:
• For approximately ten hours over a two-week period, learners read a series of short
texts in which they were exposed to literally hundreds of instances of adverbs in
English sentences-so many that the investigators referred to this study as an 'input
flood'.
• There was no teaching of adverb placement, nor was any error correction provided.
• Instead, students simply read the passages and completed a variety of
comprehension activities based on them.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 17: Input flood
Findings:
• Although learners benefited from this exposure to sentences with adverbs in all the correct
positions, their learning was incomplete.
• They improved in their acceptance of sentences with word order that is grammatical in
English but not in French ('The children quickly leave school').
• However, they continued to accept sentences that are grammatical in French but not in
English ('The children leave quickly school').
• The students' inability to recognize that adverbs in this position are ungrammatical in English
suggests that the input flood could help them add something new to their interlanguage, but did
not lead them to get rid of an error based on their first language.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 17: Input flood
• This is discussed in more detail in the section 'Get it right in the end'.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 18: Enhanced input
• In a study involving enhanced input, Joanna White (1998) examined the acquisition of possessive
determiners (specifically 'his' and 'her') by French-speaking learners in intensive ESL classes aged
11-12.
Procedure:
Students received approximately ten hours of exposure to hundreds of possessive determiners through
a package of reading materials and comprehension activities provided over a two-week period.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 18: Enhanced input
There was a major difference between this study and Trahey and White's input flood. What is it?
Every time a possessive determiner appeared in the texts, it was in bold type,
underlined, italicized, or written in capital letters.
The hypothesis was that this would lead the learners to notice the possessive
determiners as they read the texts.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 18: Enhanced input
Procedure (Cont.):
White compared the performance of learners who had read the typographically
enhanced passages with that of learners who read the same texts without
enhancement.
Finding:
Both groups improved in their knowledge and use of these forms but there was
little difference between them.
2 Just listen ... and read
Study 18: Enhanced input
• That is, even though the two forms were highlighted by the use of bold
type, capital letters, etc., students did not learn how to choose the
possessive determiner to match the gender of the possessor.
• In subsequent research, White found that learners made more progress when
they were given a simple rule and then worked together to find the correct
form to complete stories that had blanks where the possessive determiners
belonged (Spada, Lightbown, and White 2005).
2 Just listen ... and read
Interpreting the research
• The evidence also suggests, however, that comprehension-based learning may best
be seen as
• They argue that when learners are given the opportunity to engage in
interaction, they are compelled to 'negotiate for meaning' (to express and
clarify their intentions, thoughts, opinions, etc.) in a way that permits
them to arrive at mutual understanding.
3 Let's talk
• According to the interaction hypothesis, the negotiation leads learners to acquire the
language forms- the words and the grammatical structures-that carry the meaning they
are attending to.
• This is the theoretical view underlying the teacher-student behaviour in the transcript
from Classroom B and from the student-student interaction in Communication task A
in Chapter 5.
• Example 4, p. 150
3 Let's talk
• How different these examples are from the essentially meaningless
interaction often observed in classrooms where the emphasis is on 'getting it
right from the beginning'?
• But do they lead to successful language acquisition? Note, for example, that,
although the conversation proceeded in a natural way, the student in
Example 4 never did find out what 'feed' meant.
3 Let's talk
Research findings
• In the mid-1990s researchers began to explore the effects of interaction on second language
production and development over time.
• Most of these studies are motivated by Michael Long's (1996) updated version of the interaction
hypothesis.
Compared with the original version (Long 1983) stating that conversational interaction promotes
second language development, the updated version integrates:
• In a longitudinal study with adult ESL learners in Australia, Naomi Storch (2002) investigated the patterns
of pair interaction over time and whether differences in the nature of the interactions led to differences in
second language learning.
• Within her data, she identified four distinct patterns of interaction. What were they?
1. 'Collaborative' interaction consisted of two learners fully engaged with each other's ideas
3. 'dominant-passive' interaction consisted of one learner who was authoritarian and another who was
willing to yield to the other speaker;
4. and 'expert-novice' interaction consisted of one learner who was stronger than the other but actively
encouraged and supported the other in carrying out the task.
3 Let's talk
Study 22: The dynamics of pair work
• Then she examined whether that language knowledge was maintained in a subsequent
task.
Findings:
learners who participated in the collaborative and expert-novice pairs maintained more
of their second language knowledge over time.
Conclusion:
• The expectation of this approach is that students can get 'two for one',
learning the subject matter content the language at the same time
4 Two for one
• In immersion and CLIL programmes, students (or their parents) choose to receive content-
based instruction in a second language.
• In some countries, the only language of schooling is the language of a previous colonial
power. (e.g. African countries - French, Dutch, English, etc)
• In others, educational materials are not available in all local languages, so one language is
chosen as the language of education (e.g. Arabic materials in Iraq even for Kurdish speakers).
• In countries of immigration, students often have access to schooling only through the
majority language. (e.g. Kurdish immigrants studying in Australia)
4 Two for one
Research findings
1. It increases the amount of time for learners to be exposed to the new language.
1. Students may need several years before their ability to use the language for
cognitively challenging academic material has reached an age-appropriate level.
2. For students from minority groups, this delay can have lasting negative effects on
their first language as we saw in the discussion of subtractive bilingualism.
• Researchers supporting this view also claim that certain other aspects of
language (e.g. individual vocabulary items) can be taught at any time.
5 Teach what is teachable
• In Example 6, we see a teacher trying to help students with the word order of
questions.
• The students seem to know what the teacher means, but the level of language
the teacher is offering them is beyond their current stage of development.
• Students are asking Stage 3 questions, which the teacher recasts as Stage 5
questions.
Students in intensive ESL (11-12 year-old French speakers) interviewing a student who had been in the same
class in a previous year.
T Where did you put your poster when you got it?
S2 In my room.
Example 7
T 'Is your mother play piano?' OK. Well, can you say 'Is your mother play piano?' or 'Is your
mother a piano player?'
S2 No.
5 Teach what is teachable
In Example 8, the teacher draws the student's attention to the error and also provides the correct
Stage 4 question. This time, however, the feedback is not followed by an imitation or a reformulation
of the question, but simply by an answer.
Example 8
S2 Yes.
T You're saying 'is' two times dear. 'Is your favourite house a split- level?'
S 1 A split-level.
T OK.
5 Teach what is teachable
In Example 9 the student asks a Stage 3 question, and the teacher provides a Stage 4 correction that the
student imitates.
The interaction suggests that the student is almost ready to begin producing Stage 4 questions.
Note, however, that the student does not imitate the possessive 's, something that French speakers find
very difficult
Example 9
1. The 'Teach what is teachable' view suggests that while some features of language can be taught
successfully at various points in the learners' development (e.g. Isolated vocabulary items),
other features develop following a predictable order (e.g. Word order and grammaical
morphemes)
2. Furthermore, although learners may be able to produce more advanced forms on tests or in very
restricted pedagogical exercises, instruction cannot change the 'natural' developmental course.
The recommendation is to asses the learners' developmental level and teach what would naturally
come next.
Next, we are going to examine some studies that have tested this hypothesis.
5 Teach what is teachable
Study 28: Ready to learn
• Manfred Pienemann (1988) conducted a study investigating the acquisition of German as a foreign language
• They were taught the rules associated with Stage 3 and Stage 4 respectively.
• Group 1 was taught Stage 3 rules.
• Group 2 was taught Stage 4 rules.
• The instruction took place over two weeks.
5 Teach what is teachable
Study 28: Ready to learn
Findings:
1. The learners who received instruction on Stage 3 moved easily into this stage from Stage 2.
2. However, learners who received instruction on Stage 4 rules either continued to use Stage 2
rules or moved only into Stage 3.
That is, they were not able to 'skip' a stage in the developmental sequence.
Conclusion:
Pienemann interprets his results as support for the hypothesis that for some linguistic
structures, learners cannot be taught what they are not developmentally ready to learn.
6 Get it right in the end
Proponents of the 'Get it right in the end' position:
• recognize the importance of form-focused instruction, but they do not assume that everything has to be
taught.
• believe that many language features are acquired naturally if learners have:
• adequate exposure to the language
• and a motivation to learn.
• agree with advocates of the 'Teach what is teachable' position that some things cannot be taught if the
teaching doesn't take the student's readiness (stage of development) into account.
So, they view meaning-focused instruction (comprehension-based, content- based, task-based, etc) as
important for language learning
But they also believe that learners will do better if they also have access to some form-focused instruction.
6 Get it right in the end
Example 10
Examples 10, 11, and 12 are taken from a
T Another place to put our adverb?
classroom where a group of 12 - year-old French
speakers are learning English. Sl After makes?
T After makes.
In example 10, the following sentence has been
placed on the board, S2 Before good?
• argue that what learners focus on eventually lead to changes in their interlanguage
'systems.
• They claim simply that the learners need to notice how their language use differs
from that of a more proficient speakers.
6 Get it right in the end
• As we will see in the examples below, teachers who work in this approach look for the right moment to create
increased awareness on the part of the learner
Example 11
• The students are practising following instructions; one student instructs, others colour.
S2 Mom's.
• As we saw in Chapter 4, French-speaking learners of English have difficulty with 'his' and 'her' and with
determining grammatical gender in English.
• The teacher is aware of this and- briefly, without interrupting the activity-helps the learners notice the correct form.
6 Get it right in the end
Example 12
• The students are playing 'hide and seek' with a doll in a doll's house, asking questions until they find out
where 'George' is hiding.
• Although a model for correct questions has been written on the board, the game becomes quite lively and
students spontaneously ask questions that reflect their interlanguage stage.
T You said 'is' two times dear. Listen to you-you said 'Is George is in?' Look on the board. 'Is George in the' and
then you say the name of the room.
T Yeah.
Sl I win!
• Note that the teacher's brief intervention does not distract the student from his pleasure in the game, which
demonstrates that focus on form does not have to interfere with genuine interaction.
6 Get it right in the end
Presentation
6 Get it right in the end
Study 34: Focusing on gender in French immersion
• Birgit Harley (1998) examined the effects of instruction with very young children in French
immersion programmes.
• Six classes of grade 2 children (7 or 8 years old) were given focused instruction on a
grammatical feature that is known to be a persistent problem for French immersion students-
grammatical gender.
• For 20 minutes a day over a five-week period these children carried out many activities based on
children's games that were modified to draw the children's attention to gender distinctions and
which required them to choose between feminine and masculine articles (une or un, la or le).
• Students were also taught how certain noun endings provide clues about gender (e.g. -ette in la
bicyclette for feminine, and -eau in le bateau for masculine).
6 Get it right in the end
Study 34: Focusing on gender in French immersion
• Learners who received instruction were much better at recognizing and producing
accurate gender distinctions for familiar nouns than those who did not receive
instruction.
• However, the instruction did not enable learners to generalize their learning to new
nouns.
• Harley's interpretation of this is that too much new vocabulary was introduced in the
later teaching activities and this meant that teachers spent more time teaching the
meaning of words than the noun endings and their relationship to gender.
• Therefore, the input on noun endings was simply not available in sufficient quantity
for the students.
6 Get it right in the end
• Many questions have been raised by the research to test the hypotheses that
the different proposals represent.
• We know that some exceptionally gifted learners will succeed in second language learning regardless of the teaching
method.
• In the schools of the world, grammar translation is no doubt the most widely applied method.
• Most of us have met individuals whose mastery of a foreign language developed out of their experience in such
classes.
• Similarly, audiolingual. instruction has produced highly proficient second language speakers.
• However, we also know-from personal experience and research findings- that these methods leave many learners
frustrated and unable to participate in ordinary conversations, even after years of classes.
• Grammar translation and audiolingual approaches will continue to be used, but the evidence - suggests that 'Get it
right from the beginning' does not correspond to the way the majority of successful second language learners have
acquired their proficiency.
• On the other hand, in throwing out contrastive analysis, feedback on error, and metalinguistic explanations and
guidance, the 'communicative revolution' may have gone too far.
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
• There is increasing evidence that learners continue to have difficulty wit basic structures
of the language in programmes that offer little or no form-focused instruction.
• This calls into question extreme versions of the 'just listen ... and read' and 'Two for One'
proposals.
• However, the research also shows that learners may make slow progress on
acquiring more accurate and sophisticated language if there is no focus on form.
• This is especially true in classes where students' shared language and learning
backgrounds allow them to communicate successfully in spite of their errors.
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
• Thus, programmes based on the 'Let's talk' approach are incomplete on their own,
and learners' gains in fluency and conversational skills may not be matched by
their development of more accurate and complex language.
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
• It is important to emphasize that the evidence to support a role for form-focused instruction
and corrective feedback does not suggest a return to the 'Get it right from the beginning'
approach.
• Research has shown that learners do benefit considerably from communicative interaction
and instruction that is meaning-based.
• The results of research in French immersion, content-based courses, and communicative ESL
are strong indicators that learners develop higher levels of fluency through primarily meaning-
based instruction than through rigidly grammar-based instruction.
• The problem is that certain aspects of linguistic knowledge and performance are not fully
developed in such programmes.
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
• Research investigating the 'Teach what is teachable' proposal is not yet at a point where it is
possible to say to teachers: 'Here is a list of linguistic features and the order in which they will
be acquired. You should teach them in this order'.
• The number of features that researchers have investigated in experimental studies within this
framework is far too small.
• On the other hand, there has been no strong evidence that teaching according to the
developmental sequences is necessary or even desirable or that it will improve the long-term
results in language learning.
• What is most valuable about this proposal is that it serves to help teachers set realistic
expectations about the ways in which learners' interlanguage may change in response to
instruction.
6 Get it right in the end
The implications of classroom research for teaching
• According to the 'Get it right in the end' proposal, classroom activities should be built
primarily on creating opportunities for students to express and understand meaningful
language.
• However, this proposal is based on the hypothesis that form-focused instruction and
corrective feedback are also essential for learners' continued growth and development.
• The challenge is to find the balance between meaning-based and form-focused activities.
• The right balance is likely to be different according to the characteristics of the learners.
• The learners' age, metalinguistic sophistication, prior educational experiences, motivation,
and goals, as well as the similarity of the TL to a language already known need to be taken into
account when decisions are made about the amount and type of form-focus to offer.
References
Lightbown, P. M., & Spada, N. (2006). How languages
are learned (3rd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.