Promoting Positive Behaviour
Promoting Positive Behaviour
Behaviour remains a key concern for many working or aspiring to work with
children and families. Written in a clear and readable style, this book considers
theory and recent research on children’s social and emotional development,
KAREN MORRIS
highlighting the implications for effective practice in promoting positive behaviour.
The book encourages you to gain a broad picture of factors underpinning more
challenging behaviour and to work positively and sensitively with children and
families. By carefully considering the factors which support or undermine the
fulfilment of children’s basic psychological needs, fresh light is shed on some
children’s behaviour. The approach also generates constructive ways of addressing
unmet needs and considers the child’s voice throughout.
With an emphasis firmly on finding ways to promote positive behaviour
and to equip you with effective strategies to help the child, the book
includes:
> A strong evidence base
> A rich range of helpful strategies
> Consideration of a range of viewpoints, including those of parents and
practitioners
> Links with the Early Years Foundation Stage
Morris
If you are working or training to work with young children this book has a highly
practical emphasis that will help you use theory to devise effective strategies to
support children.
Karen Morris is Senior Lecturer and Programme
Leader for the foundation degree and
undergraduate degree in Childhood Studies at
the University of Winchester, UK.
VISIT…
Downloaded by [ Faculty of Nursing, Chiangmai University 5.62.156.86] at [07/18/16]. Copyright © McGraw-Hill Global Education Holdings, LLC. Not to be redistributed or modified in any way without permission.
Promoting Positive Behaviour
in the Early Years
Promoting Positive
Behaviour in the
Early Years
Karen Morris
Open University Press
McGraw-Hill Education
McGraw-Hill House
Shoppenhangers Road
Maidenhead
Berkshire
England
SL6 2QL
email: [email protected]
world wide web: www.openup.co.uk
All rights reserved. Except for the quotation of short passages for the purposes
of criticism and review, no part of this publication may be reproduced, stored
in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic,
mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, without the prior written
permission of the publisher or a licence from the Copyright Licensing Agency
Limited. Details of such licences (for reprographic reproduction) may be obtained
from the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd of Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street,
London, EC1N 8TS.
ISBN-13: 978-0-335-26298-4
ISBN-10: 0-335-26298-8
eISBN: 978-0-335-26299-1
List of figures xi
List of tables xiii
Acknowledgements xv
1 Introduction 1
My family has always been my greatest support. Particular thanks are due to
Tim who behaved impeccably despite the impact writing the book made on our
home life.
Penny Lawrence was interested and helpful throughout. The case snippet on
Jordan (Chapter 7) was influenced by her work.
Thanks are also due to Cain Richards who helped with the initial litera-
ture search as part of the University of Winchester Research Apprenticeship
Programme.
1
Introduction
the complexities of why children behave in certain ways, they will find that they
can generate helpful strategies to move challenging situations forward.
Much of the concern about, and interest in, children’s behaviour relates to
aggressive and anti-social behaviour, often termed externalising behaviour.
Of equal importance in this book are internalised behaviour difficulties,
shown by children who are seriously withdrawn and anxious. The book is rel-
evant for an understanding of both these types of behaviour but in a framework
that sees these extremes of behaviour as deriving from the same processes as all
behaviour, and responsive to the same approaches as less extreme behaviour.
The stance in this book is that behaviour (both norms and extremes) can best be
understood using a framework that considers children’s needs and what might
be appropriate expectations of them, drawing on knowledge of how behaviour
develops.
This chapter introduces some of the key concepts underpinning the approach
to behaviour taken throughout the book. It is a whistle-stop tour of ideas, plant-
ing seeds for the remainder of the book. The ideas will be revisited and explained
in greater depth in subsequent chapters.
A positive approach
When professionals ask to see parents about their children’s behaviour and when
children and young people’s behaviour is featured in the media, it is usually
unwanted behaviour that is discussed. The implied model of children is often a def-
icit one. In other words, the underlying assumption is that the problem is located in
the child who lacks the desired behaviours. The child and (often) the parents are
blamed for the undesired behaviour which is viewed entirely negatively.
In contrast, the approach in this book is a positive one. The emphasis is on
finding ways to promote positive behaviour and, where challenging behaviour
is evident, to equip those involved to find effective strategies to help the child.
Behaviour is seen not as an ingrained and essentially unchangeable aspect of a
child but as something that has been, and can be, influenced by many aspects
of the environment, including the physical environment, the emotional environ-
ment and the reactions of adults and peers.
First, we need to develop a shared understanding of what is meant by ‘posi-
tive behaviour’ (see Task 1.1).
Positive behaviour is not entirely synonymous with what is colloquially
referred to as ‘good’ behaviour, which tends to mean behaviour that makes
life easy for adults. A child who is very compliant can be at increased risk of
abuse. Behaviour has to be judged in context and taking account of a child’s
developmental level and experience. The emphasis in this book is on supporting
young children in ways that are likely to promote positive behaviour and well-
being over the long term. It is accepted that young children need to learn how
to behave acceptably and may be clumsy in their attempts to do so. They may
also deliberately test the boundaries of what is acceptable. The early years are a
INTRODUCTION 3
Comments
• Babies need to alert their carers if they are uncomfortable. Crying is
their means of doing so. This is a typical and ‘healthy’ behaviour, there-
fore can be classified as positive.
• A 1-year-old should be attached and very upset to be taken from a par-
ent by an unfamiliar adult. Ella’s behaviour is not positive.
• Olivia is testing the boundaries but wants to maintain a close relation-
ship nevertheless. Her behaviour can be construed as positive and part
of learning to accept the boundaries.
• Jack may have picked up fears about getting dirty from adult reactions.
His behaviour seems constrained and may not be positive.
• Zachary should be allowed to decide whom he feels comfortable to hug
and kiss. His views should be respected. His behaviour can be con-
strued as positive.
• Lucas is an immature negotiator. Learning to negotiate is positive. He
now needs to learn to distinguish between contexts where negotiation
works and those where it is less appropriate.
upsetting for parents of a Down syndrome child. How did the correlation
arise? It is possible that it was a pure fluke. Another possibility is that,
asked if they had had an electric shock in pregnancy, mothers of Down
syndrome, searching for an explanation of their child’s syndrome, remem-
bered or even thought they remembered, receiving an electric shock.
Other mothers would not have ‘raked over’ their memories of the preg-
nancy searching for explanation of things not proceeding as they hoped.
They may well have forgotten any electric shocks suffered.
The next time you read of a correlation and are tempted to assume a
causal relationship between factors, think ‘chocolate and Nobel Prize’, ‘elec-
tric shock and Down syndrome’ and pause before jumping to conclusions.
children and others at school entry with regards to behavioural issues than
Australia, Canada and the USA (Bradbury et al. 2012). These statistics do not
reveal why poverty and disadvantage are associated with behavioural difficul-
ties. Such statistics can be very useful as a broad guide to how to best allocate
scarce resources to the neediest. They should not be used to stereotype or form
negative expectations of large sections of our communities. The approach taken
in this book is one of considering any child and family individually where there
is concern about behaviour.
well-being when in an environment or situation that enables the three key needs
for relatedness, autonomy and competence to be met. If someone is thwarted in
having any or all of these needs met, there will be a psychological cost, often
displayed in behaviour, which may take the form of anxiety, lack of empathy,
aggression or myriad other forms. In this model, much unwanted behaviour is
interpreted in terms of people compensating or developing defences to cope with
the thwarting of their needs for relatedness, autonomy and competence. The
model will be further explained and used to illuminate behaviour and to gener-
ate positive approaches throughout the book. The model raises the following key
question in terms of promoting positive behaviour: what can be done to further
help the child meet their needs for relatedness, competence and autonomy?
Relatedness Autonomy
Competence
• Think of some examples of how your own needs in these three spheres
are satisfied.
• Does your work enable satisfaction of these needs?
Key points
• Positive behaviour is promoted by providing environments that enable peo-
ple to meet their psychological needs.
• Positive behaviour does not just mean complying easily with adults.
• Nurture is usually more important than nature in the genesis of problem
behaviour. Much behaviour is learned or shaped by circumstances.
• Theory can provide constructive ways of viewing behaviour and generating
strategies to support positive behaviour.
• Correlations between factors should be treated with care as they do not nec-
essarily mean that one factor has caused the other. Understanding cause
and effect is important when devising effective strategies. Experiments and
longitudinal studies are to be favoured when trying to establish cause and
effect.
• As they grow, children internalise the behavioural expectations of their com-
munities. The way in which this internalisation takes place has long-term
consequences for their behaviour and well-being.
• Self-determination theory, as developed by Deci and Ryan (2000), posits
that environments that support the needs for relatedness, autonomy and
competence will promote healthy internalisation and positive behaviour.
Part 1
Relatedness
‘I belong’
Relatedness Autonomy
Competence
Related individuals:
• Feel unconditionally loved and cared for and love and care for others uncon-
ditionally.
• Depend on important others.
• Have a sense of security and of mattering.
• Feel linked to the values of their main social groups, being influenced by
them and influencing them.
• Consider others without compromising their sense of self.
• Value co-operation and collaboration.
Relatedness is not:
• Total dependence on someone else (after infancy).
• Always being part of a group and avoiding solitary activities.
INTRODUCTION TO PART 1 17
Structure of Part 1
• Chapter 2 – ‘The value of a secure attachment bond: “I need to feel safe, spe-
cial and valued”’. This chapter explores the origins of attachment theory and
both the immediate and long-term benefits of secure attachment. It looks at
how parents and those working with children can promote secure attach-
ment through sensitive, responsive care.
• Chapter 3 – ‘Understanding insecure attachments and ways to help: “Under-
stand that this behaviour started as self-protection”’. This chapter develops
understanding of the different ways that children try to meet their need
for relatedness in less than ideal circumstances and the impact on their
well-being and behaviour. It helps adults to recognise these patterns and
discusses ways to help those at risk of insecure attachments.
Each child must be assigned a key person. Their role is to help ensure
that every child’s care is tailored to meet their individual needs …, to
help the child become familiar with the setting, offer a settled relation-
ship for the child and build a relationship with their parents.
(DfE 2014b)
If you want children who thrive in all respects, including displaying positive
behaviour, the best single piece of advice is to provide babies and young children
with consistent, responsive caregiving. This means caregiving that is delivered
by one or a consistent small group of adults, with the adults being attuned to the
baby or child, picking up on the range of cues the child offers – subtle cues that
the baby is interested and would like to interact, or is tired and would prefer to
rest (Sroufe 1990) as well as the more obvious cues such as crying. Such care-
giving enables babies to form and maintain a secure attachment bond with the
caregivers. By dint of being soothed and having their needs met by their caregiv-
ers, the scene is set for two very important developments. First, babies who are
soothed and comforted learn over time to stabilise (regulate) their own emotions.
Emotional regulation is important for positive behaviour. Second, when babies
and children are responded to in a way that meets not just their physical needs but
recognises their individuality, their likes and dislikes, the times when they want
to interact and play, their emerging sense of humour and their idiosyncrasies,
then they develop the sense that they matter, that they are special and valued.
This too is important for behaviour. Children who are supported in understand-
ing and valuing themselves develop a clear yet positive sense of self, which is
a foundation for autonomy. They also tend to understand and value others too.
Early relationships form the template for future relationships. The child who feels
secure and valued has a tremendous foundation for the joys and challenges of
relationships throughout life. Such a child is much more likely to be happy, to give
and receive appropriate care, to take others’ feelings into account and generally
to behave positively than is a child with less fortunate early experiences. These
secure foundations produce a dizzying array of benefits (Figure 2.1).
Some bold and sweeping claims have been made in the paragraph above.
They are the claims of modern attachment theory, ‘perhaps the most widely-used
and influential explanatory framework in psychology’ (Meins 2014). In order to
apply these ideas to benefit children and families, it is important to understand
attachment theory in some depth (see Box 2.1). Attachment theory has had a
major influence on the EYFS (DfE 2014b) and has informed the Key Person
THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND 21
More
positive
self-
concept
More curious,
More
aiding
competent
language and
in forming
cognitive
friendships
development
Secure
More able
attachment Fewer
to tolerate behavioural
frustration problems
More Greater
advanced confidence in
emotional tackling day-
understanding to-day
and regulation problems
Figure 2.1 Some of the ‘dizzying array of later outcomes’ associated with secure
attachment
Source: Based on Thompson (2013).
Key influences
• Personal experience. Bowlby saw relatively little of his own mother
during infancy, being raised at a time and within a social class where
most of the childcare was delegated to nannies. He was very fond of
his nanny, Minnie, and devastated when she suddenly left and he
received no explanation why (Bowlby 2005: xi).
• His work with ‘maladjusted’ boys. Bowlby noted that some were
‘clingy’, staying near him as much as possible. Others were remote
(Continued)
22 THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND
Note: Bowlby has been criticised for his emphasis on the mother as the primary
attachment figure. Modern attachment theory is much less gendered.
Note: the strongest attachment need not necessarily be to the mother but often is.
figure close) are only evident at times and particularly when threat is perceived.
States such as hunger or tiredness can increase attachment behaviours, as can
‘life events’ such as the arrival of a sibling or any other change in the emotional
dynamics of home. With age, the form of attachment behaviours varies though
even in adulthood, trauma often triggers an urgency to contact or be with our
parents or other primary attachment figures.
them for two hours every fortnight, for up to nine months. In the Bal-
timore project, she again worked with 26 families, this time recruiting
them before the birth of the baby and making 18 visits, each of four
hours, during the baby’s first year. Ainsworth made detailed obser-
vations, recording events in context and noting mothers’ comments
on their babies as well as their actions towards them. She gathered
rich data that yielded insights into the natural development of infant–
mother interactions in two very different parts of the world. She rec-
ognised that all mother–baby pairs formed bonds but that there were
qualitative differences in the nature of the bonds.
• She famously devised an experimental paradigm called the ‘strange
situation’ which is a telling snapshot that seems to capture qualities
of the bond developed between babies and their mothers over the first
year of life. The design of the ‘strange situation’ and Ainsworth’s inter-
pretations of the findings were informed by her extensive observational
‘fieldwork’ which, to her, was the most significant part of her work.
The detailed naturalistic studies showed that secure attachment, the type of
attachment believed to be healthiest for children’s development, emerged when
mothers had been sensitive to their baby’s signals from birth. Large amounts
of close bodily contact, mutual enjoyment of feeding and the mother’s ability
to comment on the tiny details of her baby’s reactions were all associated with
secure attachment. Where babies and mothers had enjoyed this close, sensitive,
attuned relationship in the early months, the babies were generally content at
the end of the first year. Babies who had been held and successfully soothed fre-
quently in the first months sought less contact with their mother at the end of the
first year but the contact that took place was warm and both took pleasure in it
(Ainsworth and Bell 1970). These babies already had the foundations for secure
attachment and positive behaviour.
Meins et al. (2012) found that ‘mind-mindedness’ was the best predictor of
secure attachment. ‘Mind-minded’ described mothers who accurately ‘read’ the
minds of their 8-month-olds and commented appropriately (for example, ‘you want
the rattle’ to a child looking at a rattle and obviously pleased to have it passed to
her , or ‘you’re hungry’ to a child who was then offered milk which was willingly
received). Through mothers’ mind-mindedness, securely attached children build
a positive and predictable internal model of the mother as comforting and them-
selves as worthy of comfort and attention to their needs and desires. Through the
experience of having their feelings and emotions correctly interpreted and regu-
lated by the mother, they learn over time to regulate their own emotions.
Ainsworth herself drew a distinction between warmth and sensitivity,
describing warmth as a general characteristic but sensitivity (responsiveness/
26 THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND
‘mind-mindedness’) as being in tune with the baby, recognising the baby’s initia-
tives and letting the baby set the pace and style of interaction (Ainsworth and
Marvin 1995: 11). A warm carer might have positive but similar interactions with
all children but one who is ‘mind-minded’ would be observed to vary the tempo
and style of interaction to suit the signals coming from a particular baby.
In talking about responsive and sensitive care, it is important to be aware of
the concept of the ‘good enough’ parent or carer, a term usually ascribed to Win-
nicott (1965) and capturing the reality that parents and carers do not have to be
100 per cent responsive or mind-minded in order for children to achieve secure
attachment. Tronick’s (2007) research suggests that even sensitive mothers ‘read’
their baby’s cues correctly first time only in about a third of instances. Misread-
ing cues and then a correcting interpretation are more common than not. With
sensitive care, babies learn that miscuing in social relationships is normal and
need not be worrying. Baby and carer can persist in expressing themselves to
achieve understanding and to ensure babies’ needs are met.
Imitation is something that many adults instinctively do with babies. Reddy
(2008: 45) describes imitation as a ‘psychological door through which one is
immediately led into a world of intentional relations with another person’. Par-
ents and practitioners can usefully adopt imitation as a conscious strategy to
help them act in tune with a baby.
In a secure relationship, a child develops trust. The attachment figure pro-
vides a secure base from which a growing child can explore the world. Attune-
ment works both ways. The child is attuned to the attachment figure, picking up
subtle cues from the attachment figure about the safety or otherwise of the envi-
ronment. The way children look to their parents or carers to gauge what they
can do is known as social referencing (see Box 2.3). The attached toddler acts
as though literally attached to the preferred carer by an elastic rein, venturing to
the farthest reaches of the elastic’s reach when all is well but seeking proximity
(closeness) to the attachment figure when there are signals given out that all is
not well or when an external threat is perceived. Secure attachment promotes
exploration and learning.
The ‘visual cliff’ is an illusory cliff. Babies are placed on a flat, stable
transparent surface about one metre above the floor. There is a cloth
underneath with a chequerboard pattern. Half-way across the surface, the
cloth drops to the ground creating the impression of a steep drop or cliff.
The transparent surface continues and the baby is quite safe to crawl on
the whole surface despite the appearance of the drop.
Sorce et al. (1985) used the visual cliff with 1-year-olds. Previous
work had established that babies of this age perceive the drop and stay
THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND 27
on the side they perceive as safe. In this experiment, parents were asked
to give babies different cues. Some were asked to be reassuring and to
smile and indicate to the child that it was fine to venture onto the ‘cliff’;
others were asked to look anxious and deter children from doing this.
74 per cent of babies went on the cliff when the parent smiled.
‘Social referencing’ is the term used to describe how people take cues
from others for how to behave. Babies were powerfully influenced by
signs from their parents that the cliff was safe.
In settings too, children show social referencing. Singer and de Haan (2007) have
carried out extensive research in a variety of day-care settings and report how
children tend to look towards the teacher when they are not sure about things or
need help. They also glance towards the adult periodically in a way that suggests
they find it reassuring to check that the teacher is watching over them.
There has been some consideration of gender roles in promoting children’s
exploration. Richard Bowlby, son of John Bowlby, has questioned whether John
Bowlby underestimated fathers’ importance in promoting exciting play and
exploration in young children. Richard Bowlby (Newland and Coyl 2010) sug-
gested that mothers might take the lead in providing a secure base for children
but that their emphasis on safety and calm, controlled activities, though impor-
tant, might be a little restrictive for the growing, curious infant. He suggested
that mothers’ approach needs to be leavened by the more exciting rough-and-
tumble play, and generally more daring activities of fathers. Many of us can eas-
ily picture a doting Dad throwing up and catching a giggling infant or chasing a
squealing toddler, while making loud, mock scary noises.
The possible importance of men in satisfying children’s needs for a racier,
riskier, more exhilarating type of interactive exploration raises questions for
many settings catering for young children. There are very few men working with
young children.
The role of fathers in children’s development has been an important area of
research (Lamb 1978; Grossmann et al. 2008; Bretherton 2010). A parallel can be
drawn between the move away from the initial emphasis on (female) gender in
28 THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND
attachment theory and the way thinking has developed in ‘father’ research. Ini-
tial claims of a crucial role of certain ‘father’ activities have now been replaced
by a systems approach that considers the range of support and styles available
to children in a particular family (or setting). Fathers’ involvement is beneficial
for children on many fronts, including positive behaviour (Lamb 2010). Sensitive
fathering has been found to have the same key elements as sensitive mother-
ing. Both involve recognising and responding to children’s needs; emotionally
engaged interaction (sharing fun, frustration and other feelings); and promot-
ing children’s exploration, primarily in directions initiated by the child. Sensi-
tive parenting and care are gender-neutral. Children benefit from having both
parents involved in their care. A systems approach recognises a range of rea-
sons why this is the case, ranging from the support, emotional and financial, that
parents may provide for each other, through to the benefit to the child of having
involved caregivers with different interactional styles (Lamb 2010).
For settings, while it would be desirable to have more men, there are also other
implications of this research. Children are likely to benefit from different styles
of play and exploration. They need carers whose primary style is calm, caring
and nurturing. They also benefit from those who tend to be more rambunctious,
adventurous and active. Not all practitioners need to adopt the same style, in fact,
it might be unhelpful to do so. Be aware of the value of discussing these issues in
settings. Individuals feel comfortable with different ranges of behaviour and some
feel threatened by the idea of altering their behaviour or of accepting different
behaviour in those around them. Many settings recruit people with similar quali-
ties to those already working there. Newcomers model their behaviour on what
they observe. The result of these influences may be a staff team that feels very
comfortable with one another but that is depriving children of beneficial access to
a variety of interaction styles. In the worst cases, children’s risk taking and explo-
ration may be limited as a result of the dominance of what is perceived as a caring,
nurturing ethos but is so extreme that some children’s drive towards the novel and
exciting is not being encouraged. An optimal environment includes diverse inter-
personal styles to stimulate children’s natural exploratory tendencies.
Box 2.4 indicates some of the features of a setting that would indicate it
takes attachment seriously.
Adult attuned
and responsive
to baby
Baby continually
Baby's internal
learning through
working model:
interaction and
'I can rely on
action. Baby and
adults and I am
adults delight in
loveable'
development
Baby is confident
Baby and adults
in interacting
enjoy interaction
with others and
and engage in it
secure enough
often
to explore
be refined over time, there are some useful established facts that show support
for Bowlby’s ideas in terms of neuroscience:
Gerhardt (2004) writes very powerfully about the biological basis of attachment
and explains how social interaction with carers literally builds the structure
(connections) within a baby’s brain. She describes the patterns of biochemical
reactions that, over time, form a baby’s (and later the child’s, then the adult’s)
habitual response. Caring behaviours such as making eye contact, genuine smil-
ing, stroking, taking pleasure in each other’s company and being soothed release
oxytocin and other ‘feel good’ chemicals which promote the growth of neural
connections and dampen down stress. By contrast, threat, fear and negative
emotions release cortisol and other stress hormones which inhibit neural growth
and are toxic for well-being if they remain in the body too long. In Gerhardt’s
explanation of secure attachment, the carer recognises when the baby is upset
or frightened and engages in the soothing behaviours which help to stop the
release of stress hormones and to dissipate their effects as they are replaced by
the ‘feel good’ chemicals. Babies, who are thus helped to cope with and manage
their feelings, learn, over time, to cope more independently. The pattern of being
able to maintain a reasonable emotional (and chemical) equilibrium is laid down
and, in the process, the baby develops an internal working model of the self as
safe, special and valued.
Among the studies providing support for these ideas is one by Gunnar et al.
(1992). They found heightened cortisol level in infants of 9 months of age half an
hour after they had been separated from their main carer. If the substitute carer
was interactive and responsive, cortisol levels reduced but they remained high in
children with a carer who responded only to the baby’s crying and did not inter-
act otherwise, simply getting on with other tasks.
evolved to increase the young’s chances of survival. This remains a useful way
to view attachment (and other) behaviour. The human baby is dependent on oth-
ers for a long period of time. The ‘unfinished baby’ (Gerhardt 2004) can grow and
develop in a way that adapts to its surroundings. This is also consistent with socio-
cultural models which emphasise that the form of development (in this case, the
nature of the attachment bond) is determined by the interpersonal environment.
Reflection 2.1
Key points
• Responsive ‘mind-minded’ parenting and caregiving by a limited number of
people are important for the development of secure attachment which is
associated with the best outcomes for children.
• A secure attachment to a practitioner (secondary attachment figure) does
not threaten a child’s attachment to the parents (primary attachment fig-
ures). The child benefits greatly from strong attachments to both.
• The Key Person role is about relationships. The quality of the relationship is
determined by the extent to which the adult pays heed to the child’s lead.
Eye contact, facial expression, speed of movement and speech and tone of
voice convey as much about the mutuality of a relationship as what is said.
• The more ‘irritable’ a young baby, the more the adult needs to soothe. Tiny
babies cannot be ‘spoiled’.
• Imitation is a powerful way of connecting to babies and children.
• Maintaining watchfulness over babies and young children is extremely
important. They are attuned to the adults’ reactions and will explore more if
the adult is watching and is comfortable with what they are doing.
• Be wary of overwhelming parents with feelings that they should always
respond and always interpret a baby’s signals correctly. Embrace the con-
cept of ‘good enough’ parenting for your interactions with parents.
• Use knowledge of attachment theory to try to help parents and children
towards secure, fulfilling relationships and to support similar relationships in
settings. Avoid making diagnoses or judgements and think instead in terms
of how to promote security, positive interaction and close, responsive rela-
tionships for all children, in the setting and at home.
34 THE VALUE OF A SECURE ATTACHMENT BOND
Further reading
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Practitioners might find it helpful to reinforce the messages in this chapter by revisiting
the PSE sections of Development Matters. The columns on ‘what adults could do’ and ‘what
adults could provide’ are ways of translating attachment theory into practice.
Gerhardt, S. (2004) Why Love Matters: How Affection Shapes a Baby’s Brain, Hove: Brunner-
Routledge.
An easy-to-read book that carries a very powerful message about the importance of
responsive early care.
3
Understanding insecure attachments
and ways to help
‘Understand that this behaviour started as
self-protection’
• Mother and infant were alone in the playroom. Mother placed the child by
the toys and herself went to a chair across the room (3 minutes).
• A stranger entered, sat (1 minute), talked to the mother (1 minute), then
knelt on the floor to interact with the child (1 minute).
• Mother left, obeying an agreed signal.
• Stranger attempted to engage the child if s/he was not playing (3 minutes).
• Mother returned (Reunion 1) (3 minutes).
• Stranger left unobtrusively during Reunion 1.
• Mother left. The child was alone for up to 3 minutes (if not distressed).
• Stranger returned (3 minutes).
• Mother returned (Reunion 2).
mother’s presence as she would provide them with a safe base in the unfamiliar
playroom. When she was not there, they would find the unfamiliar room a little
threatening and play would subside, to be replaced with attachment behaviours
such as crying and searching. The ‘strange situation’ did indeed demonstrate these
factors. What proved to be most informative, however, was the range of responses
to the two reunions with the mother. These responses Ainsworth was able to cat-
egorise into three main patterns, which correlated with what she knew of the same
infants and mothers in their home situations and with her analyses of the interac-
tion patterns observed naturalistically in both the Ugandan and the Baltimore stud-
ies. Later Mary Main, a colleague, added a fourth category, which will be included
in the discussion (Main and Sullivan 1990). The ‘strange situation’ has been repli-
cated extensively and proved to be a reliable indicator of attachment quality or
type. Note the different behaviours associated with the attachment styles when we
discuss them below. Interpreting behaviour through an ‘attachment lens’ can pro-
vide clues to whether a child’s need for relatedness has been met in a healthy way.
Secure attachment
Securely attached infants take their cue from their mothers that it is fine to play.
They are often distressed when their mother leaves but show obvious relief and
pleasure in her return (the reunions). They seek proximity (closeness) to their
mother when uncertain. When the stranger enters, they often approach their
mother but they quickly pick up her cues that there is nothing to fear and are happy
to resume play in the stranger’s presence while their mother remains in the room.
making demands on an adult) does not necessarily mean that all is well with a
child. Practitioners should be alert for children who seem to lack emotion and
shun connection with people.
Parent's
attunement to baby
is erratic.
Baby's internal
Child is wary and
model: 'There is no
distrustful of others
certainty that my
and lacks
needs will be met. I
confidence in
need to grab
defining and
attention when I
meeting own needs.
can.'
Opportunities for
warm, stress-free Infant expends
interaction are energy in watching
limited. So too are adults and making
opportunities for bids for attention.
exploration.
Practitioners will notice and be concerned about the behaviour of these children.
They are likely to be angry and aggressive, controlling and unpredictable. Safe-
guarding concerns need to be at the forefront of everyone’s minds.
• The possibility of a one-to-one Key Person working with only this child.
• Additional training or reading about disorganised attachment for as
many members of staff as possible.
• A support network for the one-to-one Key Person, possibly daily short
meetings with the manager and room staff to talk though progress, inci-
dents and strategies. Both practical and emotional support will be crucial.
Note that all forms of attachment indicate a bond with the caregiver(s). Evolu-
tion has equipped babies to attach to their caregivers in the way that is likely to
help them to survive longest. Avoidantly attached children try to minimise their
demands on the caregiver whom they sense has limited capacity to help them – it is
a survival strategy. Similarly, ambivalently attached children watch their caregiv-
ers very carefully as they fear that their needs will not be recognised if they do not
remain alert to opportunities to ‘present’ them. Children are presumed to develop
these styles because they are their best chance of having their needs met in the cir-
cumstances in which they find themselves. They are simply trying to protect them-
selves. When they later encounter different, more favourable, contingencies, as in
attending a high quality childcare setting, they may struggle to accommodate. Pat-
terns of behaviour are tied to patterns of physiological responses and can be hard
to alter. Children’s challenging behaviour is often a symptom of their confusion.
The descriptions of the insecure categories have indicated their associations
with behaviour problems. Table 3.1 shows the links between attachment styles
and behaviour.
but this may mask underlying problems, for example, Dettling et al. (1999) found
elevated cortisol levels (a sign of stress) in 3- and 4-year-olds in nursery though
their behaviour caused no concern. Rapid turnover of staff, long work shifts and
tokenistic implementation of the Key Person requirement are among the corre-
lations of low quality childcare. Managers should guard against these factors,
including ensuring that work patterns are not so extreme that practitioners tend to
become listless and relatively unresponsive towards the end of long shifts. Chapter
2 looked in depth at ways in which settings can promote secure attachment.
Depression (and other risk factors) may affect not just parents but also prac-
titioners. Managers should be alert to any such factors and discuss and acknowl-
edge the possible effort that affected practitioners may need to exert, and the
support they may require, to deliver suitably responsive care.
Key points
• Ainsworth’s ‘strange situation’ led to the identification of patterns of
insecure attachment, each with implications for the internal working
UNDERSTANDING INSECURE ATTACHMENTS AND WAYS TO HELP 45
Further reading
Bomber, L.M. (2007) Inside I’m Hurting: Practical Strategies for Supporting Children
with Attachment Difficulties in Schools, London: Worth Publishing.
Bomber, L.M. (2011) What about Me? Inclusive Strategies to Support Pupils with Attach-
ment Difficulties Make It through the School Day, London: Worth Publishing.
Geddes, H. (2006) Attachment in the Classroom: The Links between Children’s Early
Experience, Emotional Well-Being and Performance in Schools, London: Worth
Publishing.
Heather Geddes and Louise Bomber are two writers who address the impact of insecure
attachment on behaviour in schools and strategies to help. Their work is focused on
school-aged children but might nevertheless be of interest for they give clear depictions
of the ongoing challenges faced by children with insecure attachment. They explain the
difficulties empathetically and advocate positive practical approaches.
Part 2
Autonomy
‘Being me’
Relatedness Autonomy
Competence
Autonomous individuals
• Know their own minds and feel able to be their genuine selves.
• Make their own choices.
• Act in a manner consonant with their beliefs and values.
• Are open to their emotions but not at their mercy.
• Cherish their relationships and feel any compromises they make for the sake
of relationships are outweighed by their value.
• Can negotiate flexibly, maintaining a focus on priorities and compromising
on other matters.
• Have a sense of vitality and growth.
INTRODUCTION TO PART 2 49
Autonomy-supporting environments
• Are respectful of everyone.
• Acknowledge feelings (and help people to recognise their own feelings).
• Allow choice.
• Require tasks for a good reason and make the reason clear.
• Allow people a say (and act upon that say).
• Recognise the contributions people make.
• Model successful conflict resolution and negotiation.
• Allow people to try things and accept that mistakes will happen and can be
learning experiences.
• Allow risk taking.
• Are set up so that people can take some initiative.
• Give appropriate opportunities for responsibility.
• Provide ‘structured freedom’.
as the source of action’. They see autonomy as the opposite of coercion. Coercion
comes in myriad forms. It is easily recognised in (the fear of) harsh punishment but
it can also be experienced in other ways, including fear of loss of love and pressure
of others’ expectations. Over-protection and intrusive helpfulness (so-called ‘heli-
copter parenting’) can be coercive as they constrain people’s options.
The degree of autonomy experienced by children impacts upon how they
internalise the expectations and demands that are increasingly placed on them
as they grow up. Deci and Ryan argue that socialisation and internalisation
are natural processes. Provided their psychological needs are met, children’s
sense of relatedness will lead them to internalise the ‘demands’ of their culture
(the things that they have to do that are not intrinsically appealing).
Autonomy develops over time as children learn about themselves and about
the society in which they are growing up. Some ‘aggressive’ and ‘defiant’ behav-
iour (terms used in the psychological literature and hence used here) is perhaps
usefully construed as part of the natural development of autonomy. Other, more
persistent and extreme behaviour could be interpreted by the Deci and Ryan
(2000) model as a reaction against an environment that undermines the develop-
ment of autonomy. A child who is very aggressive towards adults and children
may be reacting to a harsh, coercive and punitive environment.
One of the major insights offered by the model is that some children whose
behaviour is not seen as causing concern may be storing up problems for the future
because their sense of autonomy is not being nurtured. People at different ends of
the autonomy continuum might behave equally ‘well’ but the motivations behind
their behaviour might have vastly different consequences for their well-being
and their behaviour in the future. Think of the case where children say ‘sorry’ to
another child after a dispute because the other child’s upset reaction makes them
realise that they acted unfairly and the child’s reaction is legitimate. This is com-
pletely different from a case where a child says ‘sorry’ out of fear or social expecta-
tion yet is really the wronged partner but cannot express this or has no expectation
of being listened to. The former is autonomous behaviour with children choosing
freely to adopt the social custom of apology; the latter behaviour is ‘controlled’,
motivated by others’ expectations but not ‘true’ to the child’s inner self.
There are various different strands of research with a bearing on how chil-
dren develop autonomy. They offer different perspectives though there is some
overlap and certainly some commonality in the lessons offered for effective
autonomy-supporting practice. The structure of Part 2 on autonomy is as follows:
• Chapter 4 – ‘Boundary setting: “I need to explore and find out about the world
and myself within managed boundaries”’. This chapter emphasises the
importance of parents setting boundaries for children. It explores the lit-
erature on parenting styles to help practitioners understand how particular
patterns of child behaviour result from particular styles. Practitioners are
in a strong position to support parents with setting boundaries. The chapter
also suggests ways in which autonomy can be supported within the setting.
52 INTRODUCTION TO PART 2
captures the way most mothers were, in effect, treating children like young
apprentices to a culture and helping them gradually develop the behaviours to
fit in. Reasonable boundaries and rules provide the ‘structured freedom’ that pro-
motes autonomy. Accepting that children need support and will only gradually
become consistent in adhering to rules is another autonomy-supporting factor.
For children, appropriate, managed boundaries fulfil several important pur-
poses. They supply them with a safe and predictable framework for playing and
living. At the same time they provide areas for children to test out their influ-
ence and power, a natural part of being curious and learning about oneself and
relationships. Boundaries also give children the message that certain types of
behaviour are expected here – it prepares them for restrictions they will come
across in different places and gives them a prototype against which to compare
and contrast other systems they encounter in the future. Managed boundaries
show children that someone is noticing them and, though there will be occasions
when they wish this was not so, the underlying message they absorb is that they
are worth noticing, they matter.
Parenting/child-rearing styles
‘Parenting styles’ capture patterns of parent behaviour. The patterns are made
up of a number of different parental behaviours and the motivations that seem
to underlie them. The styles are composite categories, amalgams of different ele-
ments. They stereotype the different styles to some extent but are useful to set
the scene and give a broad analysis of how different child-rearing approaches
promote or hinder autonomy and positive behaviour in the long term. In this
chapter, the emphasis is placed on boundary setting for children, an issue of
crucial importance in promoting positive behaviour but something that can be
problematical for parents and often plays a part in children’s behavioural dif-
ficulties. Many loving parents fail to put in appropriate boundaries, possibly
construing themselves as kind and tolerant. Boundaries, appropriately set and
managed, play a vital role in helping children feel safe and promoting autonomy.
Children thrive when life is predictable and there is the safety of adults being in
overall control. The discussion on attachment theory stressed that babies who
are soothed and comforted go on to regulate their own emotions. Similarly, chil-
dren brought up with reasonable boundaries learn to impose boundaries on their
own behaviour. They are less likely to mismanage the freedoms of adolescence
than their peers brought up with unduly restrictive boundaries or virtually no
boundaries.
Research on parenting styles is relevant for practitioners. If they understand
the types of behaviour associated with each style, this can provide a basis for
interpreting children’s behaviour and for working with families to promote an
alternative style, when appropriate. Where families have established few bound-
aries, one of the most important contributions practitioners can make is to help
parents understand the value of boundary setting and to support them in the
BOUNDARY SETTING 55
difficult task of putting in limits when a child has become accustomed to oper-
ating without them. A further reason for practitioners to know about parenting
styles is that the style associated with best outcomes for children is also relevant
for best practice in education and childcare settings.
Landmark work on families and boundaries was carried out by Diana Baum-
rind in 1966 but the basic outline of her findings and insights remains current, with
subsequent research refining rather than radically revising her ideas. Baumrind
(1966) reviewed 12 studies into parent behaviour and its effects on children. She
selected studies where the children had been observed directly and on a number
of occasions. Some had been observed at home or in school, others in laboratory
settings. The parents had been interviewed about their attitudes and behaviour.
Some studies also involved direct observation of the parents’ interaction styles.
From analysing the studies, Baumrind concluded that parenting styles could be
divided into three broad types (though she recognised that a given parent would
probably use different styles at different times): authoritarian; permissive; and
authoritative.
Authoritarian parenting
Authoritarian parents are highly controlling of their children, setting firm bound-
aries to suit their own desires and/or beliefs and not seeing any need to take
the children’s views into account. Boundaries are strictly enforced, sometimes
by threat of, or actual, physical punishment. This is the ‘spare the rod and spoil
the child’ view of parenting where children are believed to have to shape up to
high expectations for their own good. Authoritarian parents prize obedience and
conformity. They (or the higher sources that guide them, for example, religious
ideas) know what is right and children are discouraged from debating, avoiding
or transgressing the rules. The rules are the rules and need no explanation. The
authoritarian style is harshly undermining of autonomy.
Children in authoritarian households are likely to be expected to help sub-
stantially with jobs around the house. Often there is no choice over the types of
jobs carried out or when they are done.
Authoritarian parenting is associated with children who look to adults to
organise their lives. They lack opportunities to develop autonomy. ‘Good’ behav-
iour is enforced and children brought up by authoritarian parents are often com-
pliant. Particularly when harsh physical or psychological punishment has been
involved, children tend to respond in one of two ways. Their response might be
hostile withdrawal (retreating into themselves, not engaging with others or with
activities, being nervous, possibly flinching if someone makes a sudden gesture
in their direction). On the other hand, children treated in this way may ‘act out’
when away from their parents, hitting and trying to control other children, using
some of the tactics they have themselves experienced. There is evidence that
coercive, authoritarian parenting of babies is associated with challenging behav-
iour by the child later on (for example, Tremblay et al. 2004).
56 BOUNDARY SETTING
Permissive parenting
Baumrind’s characterisation of permissive parenting, while based on research, is
influenced by the sociocultural environment in which she was working – 1960s
America, the ‘Swinging Sixties’, a time of experimentation with relationships and
of rapid change in society.
Permissive parents are portrayed as loving and giving, to a fault. They
are accepting of their children and act as though they want to be their friend
rather than their parent. Their underlying view (of which they may not be con-
scious) is of themselves as someone to help their children by granting their
wishes to a large degree. They do not see their key roles as being to provide
a role model for their children or to guide and mould their behaviour. They
make few demands on their children, wanting to provide them with space and
opportunity to be themselves and to grow into their own aspirations. These
parents may intend to afford children autonomy but the evidence of the effect
of this style is otherwise. Permissive parents avoid exerting control but will
try to reason with children. If that fails, they may use what Baumrind terms
‘manipulation’, for example, diversionary tactics. Permissive parents tend not
to set explicit boundaries for children. Children hold a great deal of control in
households where the dominant style is permissive. What they lack is a devel-
opmentally appropriate level of structure to provide the levels of predictability
and security that might enable them to exercise this control in a constructive
and autonomous fashion.
Permissive parents typically expect their children to do few chores in the
household as they grow up.
Permissive parenting is associated with children who find it hard to under-
stand that rules in settings and schools apply to them as well as everyone else.
These children may defy rules, ignore what adults say and argue with those who
try to get them to conform to behavioural expectations. They can be lacking in
initiative and tend to expect adults to solve their problems for them. Practition-
ers and teachers (and, sometimes, parents) may view such children as ‘difficult’
and unruly. The children lack the confidence, the security and the positive, goal-
focused approach that might have been fostered in them. They tend to expend
a great deal of energy trying to sort out the sometimes overpowering extent of
their control. They can be confused by the conflicting expectations placed on
them at home and elsewhere.
Regarding ‘permissive’ approaches, it is important for both parents and prac-
titioners to be aware that when an adult is present but does not intervene to set
appropriate boundaries on behaviour, children seem to assume that what they
are doing is being condoned – the chances of them repeating behaviour with
negative consequences for others are higher than if no adult had been present
(Siegel and Kohn 1959; Singer and de Haan 2007: 44). This emphasises the impor-
tance of adults maintaining appropriate boundaries. Not to do so is a missed
opportunity for promoting positive behaviour but, more than this, it may worsen
behaviour.
BOUNDARY SETTING 57
Authoritative parenting
Authoritative parents see their role as guiding children and, over time, they hope
to help their children develop into young people who manage their own behav-
iour, taking into account its effects on others. These parents set boundaries but
not in an arbitrary fashion. They are happy to articulate the reasons for the
boundaries and, as children grow up, they will be open to discussion and flex-
ibility on some boundaries. Crucial boundaries, for example, to do with safety,
might be varied with the age of the child but be otherwise non-negotiable.
Authoritative parents typically expect children to take on some responsibil-
ity for jobs in the home but may offer a degree of choice concerning the jobs and
some flexibility in when they are carried out.
Children of authoritative parents are those that are best equipped to develop
autonomy and flourish in the settings and schools of Western democracies. This
parenting style is associated with responsible children, who conform the majority
of the time but may speak up if they feel something is unfair. They tend to have con-
fidence in the value of their ideas and opinions but listen to, and value, other opin-
ions too. These children usually co-operate with others. They play an active role in
solving any problems they face but they will seek adult support appropriately.
There is some evidence to suggest that authoritative parenting enables
babies with ‘difficult temperaments’ to overcome the association between this
temperament and behaviour difficulties in pre-school (Paulssen-Hoogeboom et
al. 2008). Differences in children’s temperaments exist and babies and young chil-
dren considered to have a ‘difficult temperament’ are those who tend to display
powerful negative emotions such as sadness, frustration and anger. Authoritative
58 BOUNDARY SETTING
parenting helps such children. This links with evidence that responsive caregiv-
ing mitigates against ‘difficult temperament’ resulting in externalising behaviour
difficulties (Kochanska and Sanghag 2013).
Baumrind’s categories of parental attitudes and behaviour and their effects
on children are useful for informing practice and reflection. Two notes of caution
need to be sounded. The first is the likely cultural specificity of the ideas. Our
settings and schools prize certain characteristics and, in our culture, a particu-
lar parenting style (authoritative) promotes children’s likelihood of developing
the required characteristics. In other cultures, the findings are more ambivalent.
The second note of caution is somewhat related to the first but also raises the
issue of ‘values’ versus ‘research evidence’. Some people are shocked to find that
Baumrind is not completely against physical punishment. Whilst abhorring harsh
punishment, she believes there is evidence that mild physical punishment may
have some benefits. Here her views are at odds with prevailing norms in the
world of British education. We perhaps need to acknowledge that there is an
important place for ‘values-led’ approaches as well as for evidence-based ones.
Hopefully the two approaches work in tandem but this is not always the case. (A
strong argument can, however, be made that ‘benefits’ achieved by mild physical
punishment can be better achieved in alternative ways.)
In her discussion of parenting styles and their effects, Baumrind makes
implicit reference to a hostility/warmth continuum. Hostile control by a parent is
the authoritarian style. Firm control by a warm parent is the authoritative style.
The context of particular actions/attitudes – the relationships within which they
occur – are all-important. This is why there is an overlap between relatedness
and autonomy (see Figure P2.1). Just as both parent and baby thrive and benefit
from secure attachment in the baby stage, so too does authoritative parenting
protect both from the long-term damage that can occur if boundaries/behav-
ioural limits are not negotiated in a way that meets both sets of needs. Within the
authoritative relationship, children have a strong sense of security and belonging
in the home and a strong drive to please their parents. Authoritative parents are
attuned to a child’s feelings and developing competencies, while also recognis-
ing their own needs and those of all in the household. Both parent and child
derive pleasure from their relationship. These factors hold fundamentally true
even though children – as a natural part of their curiosity and learning – will test
any boundaries they face.
In later reviews of parenting styles, researchers have identified an over-
protection/coercive control continuum in addition to the hostility/warmth one
(Parker 1983; Chorpita and Barlow 1998). These continua relate well to the ideas
in this book regarding the environments that promote satisfaction of the need
for autonomy. Both ends of the overprotection/control continuum are associated
with limiting autonomy. Over-protective parents limit children’s opportunities to
learn about the consequences of their behaviour and about how to manage the
risks they take. This may be a parental practice that is increasing among recent
generations of parents. Certainly, providers of early years education and care
BOUNDARY SETTING 59
and local authorities responsible for playgrounds have been influenced by health
and safety concerns and awareness of legal repercussions should anything go
wrong. Reasonable precautions must be taken but, equally, children need devel-
opmentally appropriate opportunities to stretch themselves and exercise their
capabilities to the limit.
course, rules may seem reasonable to their originator and appear arbitrary to
those expected to follow them! Explaining rules and boundaries to children sup-
ports effective internalisation and autonomy – children can, for example, choose
to be kind to other children, recognising how being unkind to them would make
them feel. This is much more autonomous than acting in ‘a kind way’ to avoid
negative consequences of not doing so. Involving children in deciding the rules
in a setting can be an important part of supporting autonomy.
Key points
• Authoritative parenting (and childrearing practice) promote the development
of autonomy.
• ‘Structured freedom’ involves providing reasonable behavioural boundaries
with the reasons explained to the child, as well as opportunities for develop-
mentally appropriate choice and risk-taking.
• Some parents may benefit from advice and support regarding boundary-set-
ting. Acknowledge that putting in boundaries late is hard work and support
parents with this task, stressing the long-term benefits for all the family.
• Harsh, coercive control impedes the development of autonomy but so too
does over-protection.
BOUNDARY SETTING 61
Further reading
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Practitioners might find it helpful to look at the section in Development Matters on PSE:
Managing Feelings and Behaviour and to find the links with setting boundaries.
Roberts, R. (2006) Self-Esteem and Early Learning: Key People from Birth to School, 3rd
edn, London: Paul Chapman.
Chapter 6 is on the topic of adults setting limits.
5
Emotions
‘I need to understand my own feelings
and how to use them constructively’
Despite the evolutionary argument for the importance of emotion, the Western
philosophical tradition has tended to regard emotions as an inconvenience and
as vastly inferior to reason. Emotions have been viewed as unpredictable and
often damaging – closely allied to sin. The ideal, in this tradition, has been to
keep emotion strictly controlled, and often suppressed, and to concentrate on
developing reasoning abilities. While some recent changes in these attitudes can
be discerned, this is essentially the heritage that has shaped British society (the
tradition of the ‘stiff upper lip’). It is a heritage that leaves its imprint on how
parents bring up their children and on our education system.
Reflection 5.1
Basic emotions
Most textbooks assert that there are six basic emotions and that babies
of any culture will show the facial expressions for these by the time they
are 7 months old. The six basic emotions are considered to be: happi-
ness, sadness, anger, disgust, surprise and fear.
Recent work (Jack et al. 2014) suggests that there may be only four
basic emotions: happiness, sadness, ‘approach and deal with’ (which
within the first year separates into anger and disgust) and ‘avoid’ (which
within the first year separates into surprise and fear).
Later-appearing emotions
These later-appearing emotions evident by 24 months are: shame, guilt,
jealousy, embarrassment and pride.
While the ability to reflect on others’ emotions and consider them
explicitly develops significantly with age, research suggests that babies
are responsive to others’ emotions, and the emotional climate, from
birth. Emotion is communicated in myriad ways, including touch, tone of
voice and facial expression. The baby’s early understanding of emotion is
‘interactive’ rather than ‘abstracted’ (Reddy 2008: 79). In the context of
engaged interaction, a 2-month-old responds to emotional cues.
Positive emotions have the same role now as in our evolutionary history.
They power the virtuous cycle of positive relationships within which people
can flourish. This has been discussed in detail with regard to attachment theory
and its implications, including children’s need to feel special, noticed and val-
ued in settings. Positive interactions and emotions are what makes or breaks
relationships. Seligman (2011: 48), looking at adult relationships, points out
that ‘how you celebrate is more predictive of strong relations than how you
fight’. Having arguments may at times be healthy for relationships but what
EMOTIONS 65
Key points
• Emotions are part of what makes us human and from birth babies are sensi-
tive to the emotional tone around them.
• Emotions serve purposes and denying them is unhealthy and impedes
development of autonomy.
• Positive emotions support relationships, learning and creativity. Having fun
with children is one of the most fruitful things parents, practitioners and
teachers can do.
• If children’s behaviour is a cause for concern, try to build up their positive
experiences in addition to tackling the behaviour.
• Promote positive behaviour by helping children to learn to use negative emo-
tions as an early warning system so that their behaviour is not emotionally
hijacked.
• The five steps of ‘emotion coaching’ (Gottman with Declaire 1997) provide a
useful practical framework for supporting children’s emotional understanding.
Further reading
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Practitioners might wish to reinforce the messages in this chapter by looking through
the PSE sections of Development Matters and noting the guidance on the development of
emotions.
Faber, A. and Mazlish, E. (1980) How to Talk so Kids Will Listen and Listen so Kids Will
Talk, New York: Avon Books.
Gottman, J. with Declaire, J. (1997) The Heart of Parenting: How to Raise an Emotionally
Intelligent Child, London: Bloomsbury.
Both the books listed above are full of wonderful examples to guide adults with an emotion
coaching approach. These authors were all students of Dr Haim Ginott whose influence
shines through in their work.
Oatley, K., Keltner, D. and Jenkins, J.M. (2006) Understanding Emotions, 2nd edn, Oxford:
Blackwell.
This large textbook provides a wealth of information on all aspects of emotions and emo-
tional development.
6
Unique but belonging
‘I want to be me but I still need to be loved’
Discovering difference
Toddlers are busy little learners. They generally take great delight in their
increasing accomplishments. Walking confidently, running, climbing, reaching
for things, exploring – these can be heady pleasures. Falling and hurting them-
selves, being stopped from doing what they want – these unleash distress and
frustration. Many toddlers live on an emotional rollercoaster, they are particu-
larly passionate little people. In their second year, children usually delight in
doing things on their own, a sign of burgeoning autonomy. Less endearing for
the adults supporting them, but equally a sign of developing autonomy, is tod-
dlers’ tendency to resist being helped. Hair washing, nappy changing and other
necessary tasks which the younger baby co-operated with may now become bat-
tlegrounds (see Box 6.1). Toddlerhood is when children start to realise that their
wants and desires may clash with those of other people. They are at the begin-
ning of a long learning project – how to balance individuality with social accept-
ance, how to do their own thing but retain the benefits of belonging.
Adults can do much to promote positive behaviour if they understand the
range of behaviour typical for this age group and adopt autonomy-supporting
approaches, maintaining warm relationships through the struggles that develop-
ing autonomy may entail (see Box 6.1). Readers can remind themselves of the
principles of autonomy supporting environments by looking back at the intro-
duction to Part 2.
• ‘Let’s get these toys off the floor. Granny could easily trip over them
and fall’ (giving a reason).
• ‘Shall I put the shampoo on your hand to rub in or do you want me
to do it? Shall I hold the flannel over your eyes or will you?’ (offering
limited choice).
UNIQUE BUT BELONGING 71
• ‘You’re tired and just want to sleep. We need to get you washed and
changed. Let’s do it as quickly as possible. Can you make it easy for
me to help you? Will it help if I sing? (acknowledging feelings).
• ‘The cars are all like Chitty Chitty Bang Bang. They can fly into the
cupboard’ (making a game, appealing to the child’s interest).
• ‘Right we need boots, coats and hats on to go to the shop. You decide
the order to put them on’ (choice of sequencing).
• ‘Brushing those teeth up and down, top teeth and bottom teeth. Keep
your smile bright and white, like the dentist said’ (giving reason).
• ‘We’ll all need warm sweaters for later because it’s going to be cold.
Which do you want to pack – your red or the blue?’ (limited choice).
• ‘It stings when I clean your cut? Poor you. I need to do it so it gets
better quickly’ (acknowledging feeling/giving reason).
What does research tell us about typical behaviours in the early years?
Aggression
• Only 28 per cent of toddlers show little or no aggression.
• At 17 months, most children display aggression towards adults, sib-
lings and peers.
(Continued)
72 UNIQUE BUT BELONGING
• Most of these show slightly increasing aggression over the period until
they are 31⁄2 years old. Their levels of aggression then drop before
they are 5 years old.
• 14 per cent of children become much more aggressive over the same
period and these are the children at serious risk of long-term problems
and poor outcomes (Tremblay et al. 2004).
• 2- and 3-year-olds used coercive methods in 91 per cent of clashes
in childcare settings, and in 42 per cent of cases this was physical
aggression (Singer and de Haan 2007).
(Singer and de Haan 2007) on two fronts. Not only are toddlers very interested
in other people’s reactions but, despite initiating or prolonging conflicts, they
usually want to restore close relations with the important people in their lives.
Relatedness is important and can help both child and adult recover from some
of the ‘fireworks’ of behaviour typical of this period. Toddlers often cry and seek
hugs and reassurance after conflicts that they themselves have escalated. This
is a healthy sign.
with positive behaviours coming to the fore by the time of school entry. In the
Dix et al. study (2007), the defiant children in warm relationships with their par-
ents made many positive overtures to their parents quite separate from their defi-
ant behaviour. These findings and ideas suggest that for many children crucial
learning takes place through conflict in the toddler and early pre-school period.
Most of them internalise social demands and can avoid or handle much conflict
without aggression by the time they enter school. The persistence of a high level
of aggressive and defiant behaviour at school entry is less typical and much
more worrying because it is associated with poorer behavioural and other out-
comes over the long term. This developmental pattern is associated with lack of
warmth between parent and child and with lack of engagement with others in
co-operative play.
Judy Dunn, professor at King’s College, London, is well known for her
naturalistic studies of children and families at home. Just as Mary Ains-
worth carried out naturalistic studies of mothers and babies, throwing
light on attachment, Judy Dunn studied families at home and provided a
wealth of valuable detail about children’s social and emotional develop-
ment. In her seminal Cambridge studies, she and her colleagues tracked
the development of over 50 children and their families by observing and
audiotaping them in their homes for two-hour periods at intervals when
the second-born children were aged 1–3 years old. Observations were
systematic, with a running record made every ten seconds and behaviour
later categorised and coded.
Dunn (1988) tracked how mothers naturally altered their response
to their children as the children’s abilities increased. Maternal responses
to crying reduced after the first year but this was balanced by moth-
ers became increasingly responsive to their children’s attempts to use
language during the second year. She noted that mothers increasingly
mentioned mental states (feelings, beliefs, thoughts, motivations) in their
conversation with their second-born children when these children were
aged 13–18 months. The children began commenting on people’s men-
tal states at about 18 months and at the same period, mothers were
referring to people’s feelings, the consequences of actions and socially
acceptable behaviour in 33 per cent of instances of conflict. As the chil-
dren’s language and understanding developed, their mothers talked about
‘mental states’ and their relation to behaviour even more often – in just
over half of the instances of conflict when the children were 2 years old.
Dunn argues that the mothers were not just responding to children but
UNIQUE BUT BELONGING 75
What can adults do to promote children’s learning over what can be a period
of conflict? The importance of establishing and maintaining reasonable bounda-
ries was discussed in Chapter 4. The manner in which adults respond to con-
flicts is important. Autonomy-granting approaches need to be employed within
the context of strong relationships and mutual respect. Adults should aim to be
calm but firm and to remember that young children need support, guidance and
repetition to learn. Inductive socialisation practices (see Box 6.4) are believed
to be helpful. This refers to providing behavioural guidance with explanation
and reasoning. There is also a great deal of evidence for the benefits of tuning
children into others’ perspectives and feelings. Adult use of ‘mental state’ talk
(talk about what people feel, think, know, remember and believe) is associated
with children’s development of ‘Theory of Mind’ (understanding of people’s dif-
fering viewpoints) (Dunn et al. 1991; Ensor and Hughes 2005; Harris et al. 2005;
Harris 2006). As children develop this understanding of other people’s emotional
lives, and provided they are in a context of warm relationships, they are likely to
behave in a positive and autonomous fashion (Ensor and Hughes 2005). Induc-
tive approaches foster empathy and are associated with self-determined auton-
omy. Contrasting approaches such as punishment and threat result in children
focusing on external consequences that will affect them. The result is more likely
to be controlled autonomy (Krevans and Gibbs 1996), outward behaviour that
conforms to social expectations but which is not fulfilling the child’s psychologi-
cal needs and so increases the risk of later behaviour or psychological problems.
You want to play with the ball? But Billy was playing with it – you’ve
interrupted his game and he’s upset. It’s not fair just to grab. Give
Billy the ball back. Now, how could you get to play with the ball?
(prompting solutions encourages a child to rehearse words for asking
to join in or for asking to have the next turn).
Kara was in your way but you can’t just push her out of the way.
Look, she got a shock and she knocked her arm and it’s hurting.
What should you do? (prompts apology). Remember to ask people
nicely if they can move. What could you say? (encourages a child to
rehearse a request to move).
Children who are hurt or have their rights infringed by others should be encour-
aged to voice their feelings to the child concerned. (‘That hurt!’, ‘I was playing
with that. It makes me cross that you grabbed it away. Give it back’, ‘Hey, I was
playing with Luke. It makes me angry that you’re taking him away and not letting
me play too.’)
Helping children to be assertive without retaliating physically is a key skill
for autonomy but one that is likely to need practice.
The level of induction and mental state talk used in sorting out conflicts will
vary according to the age and understanding of the children. Box 6.4 gives an
outline of the approach. Conflicts provide excellent opportunities for training
in negotiation. Adults should model negotiation in adult-to-adult differences of
opinion and in clashes with children. Part of the authoritative style (see Chapter
4) involves some flexibility on less important aspects of what the adult decrees,
whilst remaining firm on the important points. If parents need to hurry out with a
child and tell her to get ready quickly and leave all her toys behind, but she argues
to bring lots of toys with her, they might, for example, agree that she can choose
one toy to bring as long as she is ready soon. Many young children quickly grasp
the idea of negotiating with their parents and become rather inventive and adopt
a ‘bargaining’ approach in their attempts. Adults need to be patient and to avoid
letting a power struggle develop as the conversations can be rich learning experi-
ences for children. Some teachers, however, react badly to children’s attempts to
negotiate with them. Parents of keen little negotiators may need to prepare their
children for the fact that there can be different expectations of compliance in dif-
ferent places, but equally teachers of young children may need to reflect on how
group settings do not always have practices that encourage fledgling autonomy.
Children’s disputes with one another may be hotspots for learning the skills
of negotiation. Adults should avoid intervening too early if no one is in danger
of being physically hurt. Children learn from settling their own disputes. Singer
and de Haan found that 79 per cent of disputes they observed in group settings
UNIQUE BUT BELONGING 77
were settled without adult involvement. (Some minor disputes were not noticed
by the adults in charge.) There will, however, be times where adult intervention
is necessary and coaching in negotiation is helpful. Box 6.5 outlines the process.
• Calm the children first. They may not be ready to enter into discussion
until intense emotions have subsided.
• Ascertain what happened from each child’s viewpoint – avoid questions
such as ‘Why did you hit him?’ Instead ask more open questions, such
as ‘Alfie, tell me what happened’ (some children will find this too open
and you might need to make guesses – phrase them tentatively so
that children are free to correct your interpretation: ‘Alfie, did you want
a turn on the bike?’).
• Try to limit dispute over the details. Encourage each child to give an
account that reveals what they want now as well as what happened.
Try not to jump to conclusions and assume one child is to blame.
• Give clear messages about the unacceptability of any behaviour that
hurt others or damaged property. ‘Mia, hitting is never the right way
to go about things. You need to say sorry to Alfie for hurting him.’ You
may also need to tackle any behaviour that provoked the unacceptable
behaviour. ‘Alfie, Mia shouldn’t have hit you and she has said sorry
but it wouldn’t have happened if you hadn’t stopped her riding the
bike. That’s what made her angry. What do you think you should do?’
(prompt apology and/or offer to let her back on the bike).
• Ask children for solutions: ‘So, is there any way you can both get
what you want? Is there anything you can do to feel happier with each
other?’ Children who are close friends are more likely to find ways to
play together after disputes (Dunn 2004; Singer and de Haan 2007).
It is not necessary that children should always do so.
Licht et al. (2008) report some research based on observing children in day-
care centres in Switzerland when they were 8, 14 and 22 months old. They noted
that the majority of conflicts between young children were driven by one child’s
desire to continue an activity when another child was curious to use the same
equipment or toy. The researchers classified the motivations behind such con-
flicts as ‘interrupted activity’ or ‘exploration’. Children wanted to play with toys
and the other child was an obstacle to this but the focus was on playing with the
toy, not on a ‘mine’ versus ‘yours’ dispute with attendant emotions regarding the
other child. Anger was not seen in incidents classified this way. Only at 22 months
were conflicts observed that were classified as ‘possession’ disputes, with children
more interested in possessing the object, getting or keeping it away from another,
78 UNIQUE BUT BELONGING
than in playing with it. Also at this age, some conflicts classified as motivated by
‘dominance’ began to appear. These too were emotionally intense. The child did not
necessarily want the object but wanted to control what was done by the object – to
make the other child carry out the requisite action. Children exerting their power
over one another in this way is probably more common than practitioners and
teachers realise (Singer and de Haan 2007; Grieshaber and McArdle 2010). Such
disputes are common among siblings too. Even more unfortunately, some adults
enter into power disputes with children (Dreikurs et al. 1971). In addition to dealing
with disputes as they arise, using the approaches already outlined, it may be impor-
tant to check whether more could be done to enable those ‘abusing’ their power
to feel more secure and valued as unique individuals (an important part of auton-
omy). Can anything be done to strengthen their relationships? Have they wide and
appropriately challenging opportunities for competence? Can older and physically
stronger children be given responsibilities in relation to younger and less strong
children (Singer and de Haan 2007)? If a child seems to want to dominate, the adult
might play with the child, giving the child a chance to take the powerful role while
s/he assumes the less powerful role (for example, adult as baby or pupil?) (Singer
and de Haan 2007). Siblings often vie for parents’ attention and interest, and this
can be particularly the case if they are the same gender and close in age (or ‘multi-
ples’ – the same age). There can be a need to help children forge a positive identity,
authentic to themselves and not just formed in reaction to siblings (whether in
imitation or as a complete contrast to a sibling.) ‘Emotion coaching’ (Chapter 5)
is one aspect of support. Part 3 (Competence) provides some additional pointers.
Task 6.1
children get caught in a vicious cycle of action and reaction that compounds
their initial difficulties. Adults who normally adopt autonomy-supporting meth-
ods can find themselves becoming more authoritarian in the face of the child’s
reaction to them. Aggression, when not leavened by prosocial behaviour at other
times, often results in peer rejection (Volling et al. 1993). These children may
therefore have limited access to join pretend play and other peer activities that
might support the development of self-determination (Figure 6.1).
A different but equally vicious cycle could work in the case of children who
are rather passive and compliant, whether related to parental depression, over-
protection or other reasons (Figure 6.2). Here a passive child complies easily
and people do not realise that this comes from copying others or just doing what
is expected. Adults do not recognise that the child has not developed a secure
and autonomous sense of self and therefore do not provide support, time and
82 UNIQUE BUT BELONGING
Stifling support of
child.
Child appears
Child is amotivated
passive and
(apathetic, listless)
compliant. Adults
or compliant to
unaware of the
gain approval or
pressure they are
rewards.
exerting.
opportunity for this to form. Such children do not face up to stressful situations
in an active way, thus missing out on the autonomy-building effects of successful
‘coping’ (Skinner and Edge 2002).
Key points
• When children delight in doing things for themselves, this is an early sign of
autonomy.
• Toddlerhood to school age is a crucial time for establishing the likely long-
term pattern of a child’s autonomy, whether this is self-determined (health-
iest) or controlled (less healthy).
• Toddlers start to realise differences between their desires and those of oth-
ers. How adults handle the resultant clashes is important for the form of
autonomy that develops.
UNIQUE BUT BELONGING 83
• In the home situation, children under about 21⁄2 years old who resist help,
object to normal care routines and defy their parents’ requests may be show-
ing normal early signs of developing autonomy. This is particularly the case
if they want to be cuddled and comforted by parents after major disputes
and if they generally have a warm relationship.
• Physical aggression is common among 1–31⁄2 -year-olds, including aggres-
sion between young children in group settings.
• Using incidents as opportunities to help children understand the perspec-
tives of others is helpful, as is general ‘mental state’ talk in the course of
everyday events and sharing books.
• Autonomy-granting support treats children respectfully, recognising feelings
and offering reasons and limited choices.
• It is helpful to model and support children with acceptable ways of achieving
their ends, such as ways of joining in, ways of negotiating, ways of apologis-
ing or making amends.
• Overprotection and intrusive helpfulness can be coercive and inhibit autonomy.
• Quiet, compliant children who copy others or who always do what is expected
of them merit further consideration. Check that they can express their feel-
ings, preferences and opinions and gradually support them in doing so, if
necessary.
• Value pretend play as a vital context for children to learn about expressing
their own needs while taking others’ perspectives on board.
Further reading
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. and Kuhl, P. (2001) How Babies Think, London: Orion.
Whitebread, D. (2012) Developmental Psychology and Early Childhood Education,
London: Sage.
These two books, though very different, both present recent research findings in a very
readable manner.
Part 3
Competence
‘I can’
Relatedness Autonomy
Competence
Competence is the final of the three universal and lifelong needs to be examined.
We all acknowledge the value of being competent in a range of spheres. We recog-
nise the privileged position of adults who find an occupation that draws on their
strengths and enables them to develop in additional ways. Our education system
aims to promote children’s learning and abilities – to develop their competence.
Unfortunately we also know that many of the children who were eager to
learn as toddlers and young children lose their inquisitiveness and thirst for
knowledge at some stage. Many sadly do not achieve levels of competence com-
mensurate with the long years they spend in the education system. Some find
alternative paths to attaining and feeling competent; others do not.
In self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan 2000, 2002), the interest is in
developing a continuing love of learning and belief in one’s competence. As in
the earlier parts of the book, we will again begin with a list of bullet points which
summarises (adult) competence as portrayed in self-determination theory. This
will provide the context for our consideration of how to foster the natural incli-
nation towards competence in children and how to avoid practices, often well-
intentioned, which undermine competence in the longer term.
Competent individuals
• Feel capable of having an effect on the physical and social environments
and attaining valued outcomes.
• Enjoy engaging in interesting activities and exercising all their capacities.
They grow and develop a bedrock of competence through doing so.
• Respond positively to novelty and also to setbacks and challenges.
• Are motivated to carry out less interesting but important activities and tasks.
Competence is not
• Only doing things where you are confident of quick success.
• Being brilliant at everything you do.
Competence-supporting environments
• Model a learning approach.
• Have structure – are responsive, predictable, contingent and consistent.
INTRODUCTION TO PART 3 87
communication and in literacy and numeracy) are especially prized in our soci-
ety and educational system. Lagging behind in the acquisition of these specific
skills tends to pose a particular threat to perceived competence and there is an
association with behavioural issues.
Actual skill and perceived competence are not the same. People may be very
highly skilled at, for example, a sport but feel incompetent because they are
never ‘the best’ in a cold, highly competitive environment. Someone with objec-
tively lower skill levels might actually feel more competent and autonomous. The
section on competence examines the types of interactions and environments that
foster a realistic yet positive sense of perceived competence. It outlines general
approaches associated with maintaining the willingness to ‘have a go’ and keep
on learning that too many children lose as they grow up.
Competence and relatedness need to work hand in hand to promote posi-
tive behaviour. Children need to have the requisite skills and understanding to
be able to interact positively with others and to behave in a socially acceptable
manner. They also need to have the relationships with others that motivate them
to do so. Dunn’s research has repeatedly demonstrated that children demonstrate
higher levels of prosocial behaviour in warm relationships (Dunn 1988; Cutting
and Dunn 2006). Competence on its own does not necessarily promote posi-
tive behaviour, for example, some bullies are extremely adept at ‘reading’ and
exploiting social situations (Sutton et al. 1999).
(Adamson and Frick 2003) and indicates that babies are quickly aware of behav-
iour that contravenes their expectations.
The importance for well-being of having an environment that is predictable
and over which one has some control has been known for some time. Classic
animal experiments highlighting the importance of control were carried out by
Seligman and colleagues in the 1960s (Overmeier and Seligman 1967; Seligman
and Maier 1967). Ethical considerations would prevent such experiments nowa-
days. In brief, the experiments involved comparison of two groups of dogs (A
and B) subjected to electric shocks. Each dog in group A was matched to one
in group B. In part one of the experiment, the shocks occurred randomly but
group A dogs could end the shock by pressing a lever. The matched dog in group
B received shocks at the same time and for the same duration as the group A
dog but it was only the group A dog that had control (by lever pressing) over
the length of shock. In part two, all the dogs were given shocks but it was now
possible for all to escape the shocks by jumping away. Group A jumped to avoid
the shocks but group B did not. Their experience of lack of control in part one
resulted in them failing to exercise control in part two – they had learned to be
helpless (incompetent). Lack of control – rather than just exposure to electric
shock – seemed to be the vital factor associated with animals becoming with-
drawn and unable to cope. Possible parallels with abused children are disturbing.
In babies as young as 12 months, there is some experimental evidence that
control may diminish fear of a potentially frightening object. Gunnar (1978, 1980)
compared 1-year-old babies’ reactions to a walking robot toy that was potentially
frightening. Some children were able to control the toy’s movement and noise by
hitting a panel to activate it; other children could not. The children with control
seemed less frightened by the toy and approached and touched it more often.
In toddlerhood, children begin to learn and understand that people can have
different feelings and responses to the same thing. Securely attached toddlers
have the security to explore how far they can challenge their caregivers’ restric-
tions and the reactions they can evoke. Children from chaotic homes lack the
predictable backdrop to their days and there are fewer patterns for them to
detect, often resulting in less security. If carers are erratic, sometimes bestow-
ing attention and at other times distant and unresponsive (as associated with
ambivalent attachment, see Chapter 3), children’s own actions are less predict-
able in their effects. A smile that may engage attention on some occasions can
meet with disinterest or irritation on others. The link between cause and effect is
unclear and so anxiety rather than competence is engendered.
Just as we discussed ‘good enough’ parenting, so too should we think in
terms of ‘good enough’ predictability of routines and reactions in fostering chil-
dren’s competency. Rigid routines can be controlling and complete standardi-
sation of reactions would be unnatural. Both would be unhelpful for a child’s
development. The brain is a good enough pattern detector to recognise broad
patterns within minor everyday variations. The minor everyday variations may
in fact help foster resilience (Chorpita and Barlow 1998). Children grow into
HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS 93
their culture and much of what many parents and carers instinctively provide
for children is appropriate for developing their competence and promoting posi-
tive behaviour.
There are, however, some children who are much more dependent than most
on fixed routines. Whereas most children thrive on some novelty and variety, a
few children become exceedingly anxious about even very slight changes. Such
children benefit from warnings of the transitions within the usual day (for exam-
ple, a bell five minutes before children have to come in from the garden, a five-
minute sand timer counting down the final five minutes of a favourite activity).
More major changes, such as an outing, need careful preparation. Some practi-
tioners find an ‘Oops card’ on the visual timetable can be useful to alert a child
to last-minute changes, such as a change in the plan to use the outside area if
workmen arrive to fix the fence.
Reflection 7.1
• What do you enjoy doing? If you were given a day free from duties and
mundane tasks, how would you spend it?
• How often do you get the chance to do activities purely for enjoyment,
activities that are intrinsically motivating for you?
Intrinsically motivating activities are those that people choose to do for their
own pleasure. For adults, these are often hobbies, though fortunate adults find
parts of their paid employment intrinsically motivating. People may put a great
deal of effort into intrinsically motivating activities and can become engrossed,
whether it is researching a family tree, tending an allotment or playing with their
children. Intrinsic motivation is associated with engagement, deep learning,
persistence, creativity and feelings of well-being. Csikszentmihalyi (1975) coined
the term ‘flow’ to capture how outside concerns drop away and involvement in
the activity becomes everything when one is deeply engrossed in intrinsically
rewarding activities. There is an overlap between the ideas of intrinsic motiva-
tion and recreation (literally re-creation, being created again), with the latter
term capturing the replenishing effects of following one’s own interests. Rec-
reation has been crowded out of some adults’ lives, probably with detrimental
effects to their health and well-being.
It is vital for children to have access to the opportunities for competence (and
autonomy and learning) afforded by their intrinsic motivation. Young children do
not have the ‘executive functions’ (Chapter 9) required to fall in with someone
94 HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS
beneficial for a child at a given stage of development and which have the most
impact on subsequent development. Barring adverse experiences or additional
needs, children tend to be intrinsically motivated by the leading activity for their
developmental stage. Emotional interaction is held to be the leading activity for
babies; exploring and playing with objects is the leading activity for toddlers;
and imaginative play holds this status for pre-schoolers. Not surprisingly from
an evolutionary perspective, there appear to be links between what children are
intrinsically motivated to do, the activities that will help them develop compe-
tence and the activities that are most beneficial for their development (the ‘lead-
ing activities’). By and large, one can work with children’s natural drives rather
than struggling to impose developmentally less appropriate styles of learning.
The depiction of emotional interactions with caregivers as the leading activ-
ity of infancy is consonant with the exploration of attachment theory in Part 1
and with the discussion above about how contingencies in social interaction con-
tribute to feelings of competence.
Toddlers love cause-and-effect toys and benefit from the opportunities to
explore a range of objects. The ‘heuristic play’ approach (Goldschmied and Jack-
son 2004) stresses that everyday objects made from a range of natural materi-
als provide richer learning experiences than do plastic toys. Treasure baskets
(Goldschmied and Jackson 2004) represent one way of ensuring that children
benefit from a variety of textures and other sensory properties in their play.
Exploring how objects work and what they can make objects do, is one of the
ways toddlers develop feelings of competence in the physical world. A parallel
strand of learning about social competence comes from others’ reactions to their
behaviour – what they are allowed to do and what adults prefer them not to do
and how far they can go in ‘testing’ the adult’s prohibition. Children are primed
to learn and to revel in stretching themselves. They need opportunities to be out
and about, exploring the physical world and opportunities for sustained inter-
action with others. Challenge plays a sometimes overlooked role in well-being.
Adults may concentrate so much on trying to change the behaviour of a child
who causes concern that they lose sight of the child’s overall needs. Stimulus,
excitement and challenge should be available to all children.
Imaginary play – the leading activity in the pre-school period – is considered
by many psychologists and educators to be a hugely important vehicle for learn-
ing, developing among other things, symbolic understanding (Leslie 1987), under-
standing of rules (Vygotsky [1930] 1978), perspective taking (Evangelou et al.
2009: 22), flexibility of thought, social skills including co-operation (Goswami
and Bryant 2007: 12), emotional understanding and self-regulation (Galyer and
Evans 2001; Savina 2014). The pre-schooler who engages in intense imagina-
tive play is also laying down sound foundations for the development of literacy
(Bergen and Mauer 2000). Imaginative play is thus a prime context for the devel-
opment of competence in a host of spheres, many of them with links to behaviour.
This will be further explored in Chapter 9. For most children, and this includes
children of school age, play is an intrinsically motivating activity. When trying
96 HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS
to give a child the maximal opportunities to engage in activities that are intrinsi-
cally rewarding – part of any behaviour plan for a child causing concern – play
may often be an activity to facilitate.
What about children who show limited intrinsic motivation? Occasionally
children may show little motivation to do anything; they may appear listless and
apathetic. This merits some investigation. It is important to establish the devel-
opmental history of such children, to check that they have not been traumatised
and that they have received sufficient stimulation. Practitioners may have to be
inventive in finding ways of stimulating and extending the child’s interest. It is
likely that other professionals should be involved to make suggestions and moni-
tor the child’s development.
Other children may show strong intrinsic motivation but only for a restricted
range of activities. Here the challenge is to try to interest the child in a wider
range of activities, sometimes by using the preferred activity as a ‘way in’. A child
who loves the train set might be enticed to wider interests if a train is taken with
him to ‘observe’ or if the activity involves making a junk model structure for the
trains. Sometimes a child’s limited and intense interest will be unusual, for exam-
ple, tapping on parts of the wall or removing the top of toilet cisterns to play with
the ballcock. If normal, mild discouragement (with reasons given) to desist from
such an activity is ineffective, it may be wise to consider to what extent arrange-
ments can be made to permit the activity safely. Tapping on the wall may not be
really problematic; playing with ballcocks is much more likely to be. Even in this
case, it may be possible to capture some of the interest through water play involv-
ing a clean ballcock and opportunity to try to make it work as a valve. Children
with restricted interests should be monitored carefully and other professionals
involved if efforts to extend their interests meet with limited success.
drawing activity was found for the children in the control groups. Contingent
reward for an intrinsically motivated activity reduces the frequency with which
children engage in it.
Deci and Ryan later made sense of this finding in terms of how contingent
rewards affect feelings of autonomy (psychological control) which, as we have
seen, feature centre-stage in self-determination theory. Contingent reward for
an intrinsically rewarding activity can dent that intrinsic motivation by making
people feel more externally controlled.
A ‘growth mindset’
Reflection 7.2
Think about the last time you received praise. What were you praised for?
When was the last time you praised someone and what did you praise?
How important do you think praise is for yourself and for children?
The use of positive language with children is accepted normal practice in care
and education settings. Practitioners are generally aware that the ‘right’ way to
promote positive behaviour is to reinforce/praise good behaviour in preference to
punishing unwanted behaviour. Practitioners can model and recommend a posi-
tive approach to parents or carers who may seem critical of children, for example,
they ‘catch them being good’. This technique involves withholding attention from
98 HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS
minor incidents of unwanted behaviour and switching attention and taking care
to make positive comments when the child behaves well. It is very important
for there to be warmth in the relationships between children and those caring for
them and the balance of positive to negative comments directed at a child can be
one measure of this. Indeed, children from lower socio-economic classes tend, on
average, to receive more parental criticism and less praise than those from higher
socio-economic classes (Hart and Risley 1995) and a common and crucial thread
of programmes aiming to help is to encourage parents and children to enjoy each
other’s company more. Increased use of praise can be both a technique to promote
happier interaction and a sign that it is taking place. So important is this consid-
ered that Leicester runs a ‘five a day’ programme, echoing the well-known public
health campaign to eat five servings of fruit and vegetables a day, encouraging
parents to praise their children at least five times a day (Sutton 2012).
Praise has a role but an increasing body of research encourages us to con-
sider whether we are balanced and discriminating enough in the praise we offer
children. From the 1970s onwards the self-esteem movement was powerful, influ-
encing many parents and practitioners alike (Noble and McGrath 2013). With
its roots in Carl Rogers’ idea (1961: 283) that children need unconditional posi-
tive regard (to be loved irrespective of what they do), the self-esteem movement
banished criticism and ladled praise indiscriminately on children. The view was
that children need high self-esteem in order to thrive. Low self-esteem was seen
as the root of many problems, from fear of ‘having a go’ at new experiences, be
they social encounters or learning challenges, to lacking confidence in one’s own
judgement and being unduly influenced by peers. Low self-esteem was regarded
as a contributory cause of underachievement and behavioural issues. Praise was
thought to foster high self-esteem which in turn would promote achievement on
all fronts. Exceedingly limited evidence has been found for the supposed ben-
efits of high self-esteem (Baumeister et al. 2003) and there have been extensive
critiques of a bland, pervasive praise strategy. This has led to a more nuanced
understanding of praise and its effects.
Stanford psychologist Carol Dweck has been particularly interested in the
mental representations children form of their competency and autonomy and
how these impact over time on their well-being and behaviour. She coined the
term ‘mindset’ to capture differing patterns of beliefs or mental representations
(internalisations) that people form. She argues that people tend to view the
world through either a ‘fixed’ or a ‘growth’ mindset (Dweck 2008) and has devel-
oped questionnaires that can reliably identify the nature of an individual’s mind-
set. Those with a growth mindset feel that what they do makes a difference: it
is worthwhile for them to strive to achieve the behavioural and other standards
they set for themselves; none of their abilities is set, they can all be developed
further with effort (Table 7.1). They are self-motivated, purposeful people who
are not only willing to face challenges but positively embrace them. In a vast
body of experimental work (for example, Diener and Dweck 1978; Mueller and
Dweck 1998), Dweck has demonstrated that failure undermines performance for
HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS 99
people with a fixed mindset but enhances it for those with a growth mindset. She
has also shown how interventions (such as teaching about the brain as a muscle
that can grow with exercise, or telling inspirational stories of the striving behind
individuals’ achievements) can influence people towards a growth mindset. A
growth mindset is a vital component of competence. It also links clearly with the
characteristics of effective learning in the EYFS.
Table 7.1 Beliefs and behaviours associated with fixed and growth mindsets
despite often behaving ‘well’ and achieving at high levels. The high level of ‘per-
son praise’ they have achieved is regarded as contributing to the problem.
The type of praise or feedback that Dweck advocates is process praise
which comments on effort, perseverance and strategies (see Box 7.2).
• ‘You told Monika that it upsets you when she takes things without
asking – it’s good to tell her that so that she knows.’
• ‘Taking turns – that was a good way for you both to be able to play.’
• ‘You asked Elliot if you could go next – that was a good way to get the
bike.’
• ‘Ah, you’re matching the colours – that should work well.’
• ‘You’re looking carefully at the shapes – good.’
• ‘Pouring the water like that didn’t work? You’re trying another way now.
Good idea!’
• ‘You shared the banana pieces very carefully.’
• ‘You made a really tall tower using the big bricks at the bottom.’
Most of Dweck’s work has consisted of experiments. Only recently have she
and her colleagues published longitudinal naturalistic observational research
which investigates whether naturally occurring praise relates to mindsets in
the way their ideas would predict. Gunderson et al. (2013), under the guise
of studying language development, took videos of parent–child interactions
when children were aged 14, 26 and 38 months. These were analysed for
the amount and nature of spontaneously occurring praise. Later, when they
were 7–8 years old, children’s mindsets were ascertained using questionnaires.
The findings indicated that frequency of process praise correlated with a growth
mindset, suggesting that this type of praise may be particularly influential in
fostering competence. It was noted that while parents did not differ in rates of
praise for sons and daughters, more of that praise tended to be process praise
in the case of sons. This has also been found to be true of teachers (Dweck
et al. 1978).
With the young children in the Gunderson et al. (2013) study, ‘person praise’
did not in fact correlate with fixed mindsets, though the researchers had hypoth-
esised that it might. Parents used significantly less ‘person praise’ with their
38-month-old children than when they were younger. The authors speculate that
phrases they counted as ‘person praise’ (such as ‘good boy’) may be quite differ-
ent in terms on effects on different ages of children. Praise and feedback have to
be appropriate for children’s language comprehension and it may be that ‘good
boy’ and similar short expressions carry just a positive message for very young
children. It is possible that, as children’s language comprehension develops, ‘per-
HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS 101
son praise’ may affect their mental representations towards a fixed mindset over
time. As soon as their language comprehension allows, it may be particularly
important when correcting children’s unacceptable behaviour to give them ‘proc-
ess’ messages about the behaviour required rather than the ‘person message’ that
the child is ‘bad’. Children form ideas of goodness and badness from a very early
age (Dweck 2000: 96) and a fixed view of the self as ‘bad’ could impede the devel-
opment of positive behaviour.
Reflection 7.3
In addition to process praise and feedback, there are other approaches that
seem likely to foster positive reaction to challenge. The models children observe
are likely to be important, so modelling making mistakes and recovering from them
is likely to be helpful. As noted earlier, Dweck’s experimental interventions suggest
various helpful techniques such as telling stories of people who persisted despite
setbacks and teaching about the brain as a muscle that improves with exercise.
• Do you agree that boys tend to receive more process praise than girls?
• Audit the setting where you work for types of praise given to boys and
to girls. Are boys receiving more process praise?
• Can efforts be made to increase the process praise given to all, and
especially to girls?
• Think how the issues concerning praise can be shared with parents. Would
a group discussion on this topic be something they might engage with?
• Could you write a short leaflet summarising key messages?
102 HOW COMPETENCE DEVELOPS
Key points
• Predictable environments foster competence.
• Intrinsic motivation is important for well-being. All children should have ample
opportunities for intrinsically motivating activities and this is particularly true
for children whose behaviour is a cause for concern.
• Promote opportunities for children to find intrinsically motivating activities
by providing a wide range of activities to stimulate and interest them and
allowing extended periods of time on activities that fire their enthusiasm.
• Show a genuine interest in the process of what children do and ask open
questions and make process-related comments on the activities, concentrat-
ing on strategies, effort and persistence.
• Correct behaviour, where necessary, by giving clear process information
regarding the preferred behaviour. If there is criticism, focus on the behav-
iour rather than the child (avoid person criticism).
• Remember that intrinsic motivation can be easily dented. Use praise sparingly
and in a process manner when children are enjoying what they are doing.
• Model making mistakes at times.
• Tell stories, share books and recommend films that show characters suc-
ceeding through perseverance.
Further reading
Dweck, C.S. (2008) Mindset. The New Psychology of Success, New York: Ballantine Books.
This book is written for a popular, and not just an academic, audience and presents Carol
Dweck’s research and ideas in an accessible manner.
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Development Matters unpacks the characteristics of effective learning, usefully highlight-
ing what adults can do and provide. Many of these ideas relate very closely to fostering
intrinsic motivation and growth mindsets and practitioners may find it helpful to revisit
the characteristics of effective learning, having read this chapter.
8
Communication skills and behaviour
‘I need to understand and make
myself understood’
weight to the argument that developing these skills is likely to help promote
positive behaviour.
Reflection 8.1
‘Motherese’
People tend to speak to babies in a simplified, singsong fashion which is known
as ‘motherese’ or ‘infant-directed speech’. Occasionally people disparage this
but ‘motherese’ occurs across cultures and has been found to promote children’s
verbal development (Parish-Morris et al. 2013). In baby rooms that are promot-
ing communication (and thus contributing to the longer-term likelihood of posi-
tive behaviour), one would expect to see practitioners conversing with babies in
infant-directed speech. They should also imitate babies’ babble as babies will often
respond to this by a turn-taking game in which they increase the babble during
their turns, again practising foundational skills for communication (Reddy 2008).
• Showing children what terminology means – take them to what you are talk-
ing about or, where feasible, bring the relevant objects to the children and
demonstrate any instructions you wish them to follow.
• Providing a simple commentary of the steps a child is following when learn-
ing a new routine or behaviour.
• A visual timetable, using photographs.
• Story sacks and song boxes with props.
• Makaton signing (always to be used with accompanying speech).
106 COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR
Often children may not catch or understand all that the adult says but will do the
expected thing because of visual cues (including copying other children). This is
fine. If, however, you begin to think that a child may understand very little lan-
guage and be relying entirely on other cues, you may want to investigate further.
A hearing test may be useful. You will need to test verbal comprehension by taking
away the visual and other cues (including the subtle cues such as looking at the
toys you mention) and seeing if the child can understand verbal directions. Chil-
dren may easily guess certain actions that you are likely to ask for with given toys
(for example, ‘feed the dolly’ if a doll, plate and fork are present). Ask for some less
predictable actions as well (‘put the fork under the plate’) to be sure you are assess-
ing language comprehension. It may be appropriate for a Speech and Language
therapist to become involved to carry out a detailed and specialist assessment.
‘Elaborative’ talk
While communication always takes place in a variety of forms, spoken language
gains in importance as children develop. Particular types of conversation help
children to internalise cultural messages in a healthy manner. Also, while family
members may be very attuned to each other without depending on words, verbal
COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR 107
• emotion coaching;
• mental state talk;
• inductive discipline.
These approaches both develop and rely on linguistic competence. A child who
cannot understand at age-typical levels may miss many of the nuances of the
messages about behaviour being conveyed in the classroom and elsewhere.
Behavioural problems may arise both because the child has not understood the
messages about others’ perspectives and feelings and so is not making the link
between behaviour and its impact on others and also because of frustration with
difficulties in understanding and in expression.
Reflection 8.2
When the volume control is silenced on the television but one continues
to watch, it is usually easy to pick up the emotional tone of what is going
on, whether people are calm or agitated, happy or angry. Try to imagine
what it would be like not to be able to read body language. What problems
would it create?
Many of us can think of a few people we do not quite ‘gel’ with, though
it is hard to say exactly why. Often it will be because we find their body
language hard to read, or ambivalent or just not in synchrony with our own.
For some people, it is the norm rather than the exception to miss or be baffled
by body language. They find the social world an unpredictable, confusing place
where they do not feel competent. Some would prefer to avoid it completely.
In discussing attachment, stress was laid on the importance of the attuned
responsiveness of adults. Babies were portrayed as socially communicative and
quick to establish how they can have an effect on adults’ behaviour through, for
example, crying and smiling (Table 8.1). This is true for the vast majority of babies.
108 COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR
Age Communication
2–5 days old Already I’m aware of who is paying me the most attention.
I’ll look more at someone who is watching me than at
someone whose focus is elsewhere.
2 months I can tell the difference between when your remarks are
directed to me and when you are talking to someone
else.
2 months Protoconversations. When I smile at you and coo, these
are my first conversations. I’ve yet to say words but
I’m communicating and I’m already grasping important
elements of conversation.
7–15 months I love to interact with you and I’ll smile, make noises and
use all sorts of strategies to engage you and keep your
attention.
9–18 months Joint attention becoming established.
I look where you look or where you point because
I expect to find something interesting. I’ll point things
out to you too because I like to share things that interest
me.
18–24 months Now I can take turns in conversation for an increasingly long
time.
Note: Babies are born with a disposition to look at human faces and to interact. The baby’s
voice aims to capture what experimental psychologists deduce from babies’ behaviour.
There are, of course, individual differences in how long and how intensely
babies like to interact. Some are significantly less drawn to communication or
are less competent at aspects of it than the vast majority. At the extreme, a few
appear to struggle with interaction and taking turns to the extent that attuned,
sensitive adults minimise the interactive demands they make on the baby to avoid
overwhelming a child who seems to find direct interaction aversive. Whereas
most babies are frequently communicating, ‘Notice me, play with me, respond
to me’, a few babies communicate ‘Feed me and meet my physical needs but oth-
erwise leave me be’. In much less extreme instances, there are babies who enjoy
interaction but do not ‘read’ body language or transmit their signals with the ease
of most babies.
Social (pragmatic) communication is a term used to capture the many
aspects required for successful conversation beyond the words involved. Social
(pragmatic) aspects include taking turns, acknowledging the partner’s contribu-
tion, signalling a change of topic and adapting communication to take account
of the partner’s status (for example, talking differently to a teacher and a peer).
COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR 109
Children whose social (pragmatic) communication skills are not at levels typical
for their age can inadvertently behave in ways that are considered rude or inept
and this can get them into trouble.
Adults need to be alert to the possibility that some children’s ‘poor’ behaviour
may result from difficulties with social (pragmatic) aspects of communication.
Often adults are irritated by behaviour that violates pragmatic rules. Sometimes
matters escalate, the adult becoming increasingly exasperated with a child who
does not modify behaviour in the expected fashion. Many children, when under
stress, find that their body language lets them down. They can grin with the
embarrassment or tension of being under an adult’s scrutiny for wrongdoing, a
reaction that tends to make their situation worse. For children with problems in
social (pragmatic) communication, giving out inappropriate signals is another
manifestation of their (often misunderstood) problem. For children who do not
pick up social communication cues and whose own body language does not ‘help
their cause’, the social world can be an unpredictable and confusing place.
These children benefit from intentions and requirements being made much
more explicit than usual. The adult may need to state, for example, ‘Dominic, I’m
not in the mood for people taking a long time to come to the carpet or asking me
questions about the summer at the moment. I will be short-tempered if you don’t
do what I ask immediately like everyone else’. Adults need to coach children
through situations in a clear, explicit way, for example, ‘When someone is angry
with you, and you are sorry, say “Sorry” and look down, keeping your mouth
straight’. Sometimes it is appropriate to involve peers in coaching – ‘If Ben comes
too close to you when talking, say “Ben, you’re too close. Step back a bit”’. Social
stories (see Box 8.1) may also be helpful.
When children are learning skills from the specific areas of the EYFS, such
as phonics or number bonds, we take it for granted that they need to learn a little
110 COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR
Social stories were devised and shared by Carol Gray (2001). They are a
positive, clear way of alerting children to behavioural expectations. They
should be personalised for a child, taking account of that child’s concerns.
Ideally they should be presented in a book and illustrated.
Here is a snippet from a story for a child who tends to shout when
conversing and to stand so close to others that they find it uncomfortable:
Miss Lewis will tell us when we have to be quiet and display the ‘quiet
working’ sign. The rest of the time it is fine to talk to other people.
I like to talk to people. It’s good to share ideas and ask opinions.
When I talk to people, I need to be one big step away from them.
Most people like their ‘personal space’. This is like an invisible bub-
ble around them. If I go closer than one big step, people feel I am in
their personal space and it makes them uncomfortable. I will stand
one big step away from people. They might remind me if I forget and
that is OK, I should just thank them, step back and carry on. I like
talking to people and get excited when talking and want them to hear
me. They can still hear me when I talk quietly. If I talk loudly, it can
hurt their ears. I will talk quietly and it will be comfortable for them.
We will have a good conversation.
Key points
• Communication influences all aspects of development. Sociocultural theo-
ries encapsulate this.
• Communication in the broadest sense (non-intentional as well as inten-
tional, non-verbal as well as verbal) is at the heart of socialisation and a
vital aspect to consider with respect to behaviour.
• Speech and language skills play an increasing role in socialisation as young
children develop. However, in optimum environments, these skills are being
nurtured from birth.
COMMUNICATION SKILLS AND BEHAVIOUR 111
Further reading
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Practitioners might find it useful to look at the section of Development Matters on ‘Per-
sonal, Social and Emotional Development: Making Relationships’. A quick glance at the
behaviours listed for babies highlights how interactive and communicative they tend to be
from the very beginning. The detailed sections in Development Matters on ‘Communica-
tion and Language’ are valuable for reminding practitioners of the range of communica-
tion skills that typically develop and ways to promote them.
Every Child a Talker (ECAT). ECAT was a communication programme promoted by the
government (DCSF 2008). Unfortunately it has now been archived although many practi-
tioners continue to find it useful. Some information can be accessed at: http://webarchive.
nationalarchives.gov.uk/20130401151715/https:/www.education.gov.uk/publications/eOr-
deringDownload/DCSF-00854-2008.pdf (accessed 30 October 2014).
Introduction
Emotions are our hot responses, valuable in many ways, as outlined in Chap-
ter 5. Having only these ‘hot’ responses would be very limiting (and indeed
‘emotion coaching’, discussed in Chapter 5, takes children beyond the ‘hot’
response). Our ability to function well in the world depends on being able
to moderate our ‘hot’ responses appropriately, being able to manage our
impulses, to wait and reassess, to deliberately attend to certain things by
choice, while blocking out distractions. If our emotions are like a thorough-
bred horse – hot-blooded, sensitive, fast, ‘highly strung’– we need to harness
these qualities by developing the skills of a capable jockey. The horse on its
own can run fast but is likely to veer off track and to be ‘spooked’ by noises
and distractions around it. The jockey takes the reins and together horse and
jockey may achieve great success.
Psychologists refer to the jockey skills as ‘executive functions’. Execu-
tive functions are essential if people are to have some choice and control over
their behaviour, rather than being purely reactive. They are essential for effortful
learning and for goal-directed activity. Hot emotional systems without jockey
skills are bundles of energy, emitting emotions and emotionally driven behaviour
all the time, flitting from one thing to another, grabbing what they want, pushing
others out of the way or possibly attacking them if they put up resistance. (Read-
ers may feel they recognise children in this category.) Jockey skills without a hot
system are (well-programmed) computers, rather than people. They are rational
but not human, lacking the intuition, the individual ‘gut reactions’ and drive that
characterise people.
Executive functions describe an important set of skills that emerge dur-
ing development and which are crucial for purposeful activity. Executive
functions include being able to stop or delay responses; being able to direct
COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 113
attention; being able to formulate and hold in mind a plan; and being able to
try alternative approaches if what you are doing is not working or the require-
ments of a task change. These skills are important for all of us to function
effectively. Children whose executive function skills are poorly developed for
their age will fall below age norms for behaviour and learning. Pre-schoolers
considered as exhibiting problem behaviours show poor executive function-
ing for their age (Hughes and Dunn 2000). Research suggests that executive
functioning plays a significant part in school readiness (Bierman et al. 2008;
Welsh et al. 2010).
The terminology used by different researchers varies but executive func-
tions can be considered in three main groupings (Diamond 2013):
Working memory
Working memory refers to the capacity to both hold and manipulate items in
short-term memory. Working memory is not just passive retention of material,
it is active engagement with the material, making connections with previous
experience and knowledge. If someone says the names of three things to you
and asks you to repeat them back, that is a short-term memory task. If, how-
ever, they name three things and you have to say them back in the order ‘most
like to least like’, it becomes a working memory task because you are required
to work out the order. Working memory is an important component of many
aspects of behaviour and learning. Deciding on and executing any sort of plan
requires working memory. Mulling over events or things that have been said
involves working memory if, as is usually the case, you are not just recall-
ing but trying to make sense of what has happened and planning whether and
how to respond. Working memory is involved in relating things that happened
earlier to things that have just happened, noting similarities and puzzling over
differences. It is part of the reflection that informs social understanding and
helps us formulate responses.
114 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
Inhibition
• Not surprisingly, children’s ability to wait for the two marshmallows (to
delay gratification) improved with age.
• As they grow, children develop their ideas about helpful strategies:
• Children aged 31/2 –4-years-old thought it would help them to look
at the marshmallows.
• 4- and 5-year-olds often realised that covering the marshmal-
lows or looking away from them would make the wait easier.
• Some 4-year-olds and many 5-year-olds spontaneously used
strategies such as repeating the contingencies to themselves
(‘If I wait until she comes back, I’ll get two’) or distracting them-
selves by jiggling their legs or twiddling with their hair.
Mischel and his team carried out some versions where children were
encouraged to think about the marshmallows in particular ways while
waiting:
• Some were encouraged to think about the lovely sweet taste and melting
softness in the mouth they would have when they ate the marshmallow.
• Others were encouraged to think about the marshmallows in terms of
aspects other than their taste and factors associated with eating them.
They might, for example, think of the marshmallows as soft pillows or
beanbag chairs for toy figures or as little clouds.
COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 115
Mischel’s daughters were close in age to the first cohort of young chil-
dren who took part in the marshmallow experiments. Ten years or so later,
when the girls were adolescents and chatting about friends and their varied
escapades, Mischel had a ‘light bulb moment’. He realised that it would
be fascinating to follow up the young children who had taken part in the
marshmallow experiments. He collected data from their high schools and
he and his research team continue to track some of original cohort now,
more than 40 years on. All the data from this longitudinal research show
that differences in delay in gratification at age 4 correlate with significant
outcomes over time. In high school the children who could delay gratifica-
tion as youngsters were socially and emotionally well-adjusted and achieved
well. Children with very short delay times at age 4 had significant problems
with behaviour and were failing academically in adolescence. This pattern
has continued in the subsequent follow-up studies. Ability to delay gratifica-
tion at age 4 seems to have huge implications for a child’s future behaviour,
achievement and overall adjustment (Mischel et al. 1989; Hammond 2011).
One form of inhibition is the delayed gratification called for in the marsh-
mallow experiment (see Box 9.1). Successful inhibition of many sorts is required
if children are to be able to get on with others and to learn effectively. In many
situations it is best to curb (inhibit) an immediate emotional response to a situa-
tion and give ourselves time to consider how to interpret it and how to react. In
order to complete activities efficiently, we need to focus and limit our responses
to distractions, for example, listening to a story requires maintenance of atten-
tion. Children who are learning about how to ask for something nicely rather
than grab may know cognitively what they should do but will only be successful
if they can manage to inhibit the impulse to grab.
Cognitive flexibility
Cognitive flexibility is the third building block of executive functions and the
latest of the three to develop. It refers to the ability to vary your approach to
things and entertain different perspectives. It is necessary when your usual or
previous response becomes ineffective but it is also helpful much more broadly.
It can enable people to find creative and better ways of doing things. In the realm
of behaviour, cognitive flexibility is associated with being open-minded in judge-
ments of people and recognising that situations can be interpreted and reacted
116 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
‘nature’ or ‘nurture’ alone but an interaction, such that the type of nurture influ-
ences which of the inbuilt potentials flourish and which fade. The executive func-
tions developed by children will depend on their biological make-up but also,
crucially, on the strategies modelled and encouraged by the people with whom
they interact and wider cultural aspects.
Vygotskians refer to the development of mental tools (or tools of the
mind) to describe and explain how children move from immediately reacting
to stimuli to being able to plan, focus and persevere. Mental tools and executive
functions can be seen as largely similar. In Vygotskian theory, the acquisition of
mental tools is crucial for self regulation of both behaviour and learning, and
comes about as a result of social interaction. It is through the process of interac-
tion that children learn, for example, ways of focusing their attention, inhibit-
ing their impulses and directing their behaviour and learning towards goals. The
quality and nature of the social interactions available to children can help or
hinder the acquisition of mental tools/executive functions.
Vygotsky believed that children first learn through active engagement (co-
construction) with others and later internalise the strategies that can support
their learning. In Part 1 there was reference to the idea that securely attached
infants are calmed by their carers and through this learn, over time, to calm
themselves. The idea here is similar. Mutual processes lay the foundations for
independent processes. When adults create the conditions for children to engage
in activities that interest and stimulate them, and when they or other children
support the child in developing strategies for persistence and constructive prob-
lem solving, then the child will, over time, adopt such strategies independently.
The facilitation role is not to do with the transmission of information. It is to
do with providing the degree of challenge and support appropriate to individ-
ual children engaging in activities that are meaningful to them. The Vygotskian
term, ‘zone of proximal development’ (ZPD) is used to capture the area of
learning where facilitation by more knowledgeable others is most effective. The
ZPD is just above where a child can function independently but where success
is still possible with sensitive support. It is through the process of interaction
that children learn, for example, ways of focusing their attention, inhibiting their
impulses and directing their behaviour and learning towards goals.
Vygotsky’s contribution to the fields of psychology and education has been
immense. He died in 1934, aged only 37. His ideas have been developed and
applied by researchers from his native Russia and other countries and it is the
work of these Vygotskians, particularly Bodrova and Leong (2007) that usefully
links to executive functions and behaviour. Bodrova and Leong (2007) devel-
oped an approach known as ‘tools of the mind’ which has been enthusiastically
adopted in many pre-schools in the United States and which has attracted atten-
tion for the high level of children’s executive functioning (Diamond and Lee
2011) and their behaviour (Bronson and Merryman 2010).
The value of language as a mental tool was mentioned in Chapter 8 which
highlighted private speech as a means of the child beginning to internalise
118 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
Reflection 9.1
hear a word naming a food, then, after a few minutes, stop clapping for food and
clap only for colours. Planning and reviewing by children are, as previously indi-
cated, crucial activities for developing executive function.
Most of us notice how we struggle with demanding tasks when we are tired
or below par. Diamond (2013) notes that executive functions are adversely
impacted by factors such as lack of sleep, stress or insufficient exercise. When
children show delays in executive functioning, she urges an overall view of their
lifestyle before assuming that they have a specific problem with executive func-
tioning. Addressing sleep patterns or exercise patterns might in turn result in
improved executive functioning.
Diamond (2013) also reports the research finding that in many situations a
child’s impulsive response is wrong but, if forced to wait, the same child can often
achieve the correct response. The challenge for adults is to find ways of helping
children slow down their wild horses and exercise their jockey skills!
Diamond and Lee (2011) reviewed the literature concerning interventions
to promote the development of executive functions in children. Interestingly the
effective interventions are quite varied and include the following (not in rank
order for effectiveness):
120 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
• aerobic exercise
• martial arts
• mindfulness
• yoga
• CogMed – particular computer programmes designed to develop working
memory
• bilingualism
• the tools of the mind curriculum (Bodrova and Leong 2007) (see Box 9.2 for
related ideas)
• the Montessori curriculum (see Box 9.3)
• Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS) (Greenberg et al. 1995) –
a curriculum designed to develop children’s emotional and social understand-
ing through age-appropriate, fun activities and links to real-life situations.
Developing tools of the mind: some ways adults can support the devel-
opment of executive functions
• Talk through procedures, events and happenings with children as this
helps give a structure to their experience.
• Model ‘private speech’ (Chapter 8) when you demonstrate things for
children and encourage them to use private speech themselves.
• If a young child says, ‘No, no, no’ but then carries out the action that
s/he clearly has some awareness is wrong, interpret this as a sign
that the child is on the journey towards internalising the behavioural
requirements. Be positive, for example, ‘That’s right, Chris. We don’t
want to jump in the deep puddles before putting wellies on. Now you’ll
need to change into dry socks’.
• Encourage children to plan their play. At its simplest this starts with
children making a choice of which activity they want to do. Later this
develops into a drawn or written plan with children indicating what they
want to do, what they will need for it, who else will be involved and how.
To begin with, planning takes place immediately before the play. Later,
planning may stretch ahead.
• Accustom children to reviewing their play and activities before they
move on to something else. Again, gradually increase the challenge of
the ‘review’, working towards a continuous cycle of review informing a
new plan, and so on.
• Give young children roles – being as quiet as a mouse, as still as a
soldier on sentry duty.
COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS 121
• Provide visual cues for roles, for example, children hold a picture of an
ear when they are in pairs and it is their turn to listen, they have the
picture of a mouth when it is their turn to talk.
• When learning particular skills, get children to pick out the best exam-
ple of the skill in their own and other people’s work or behaviour.
• Use external mediators to help children regulate their behaviour, for
example, a carpet square to ‘contain’ a restless child for short periods of
sitting on the carpet. Make sure to use external mediators in a manner
where they are a tool to help the child along the way to internal control.
One of the first female doctors in Italy, Maria Montessori worked with
children with learning difficulties and developed methods for all children
that continue to be practised in many nurseries.
Montessori believed a major part of the teacher’s role lay in preparing
the environment to allow children to follow their individual interests and
learn from constructive activity. She advocated a peaceful, quiet, unclut-
tered environment. She developed ‘didactic apparatus’, carefully designed
to enable children to spot and correct their own errors and so to maximise
(Continued)
122 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
what they could learn from independent exploration (‘auto education’). Mon-
tessorian activities challenge children as they develop, for example, walking
along a line then doing this while carrying a spoonful of water without spill-
ing (Diamond and Lee 2011). Young children were viewed by Montessori as
powerful, self-motivated learners. A teacher should observe but intervene
only when necessary – ‘she may always be ready to supply the necessary
help, but may never be the obstacle between the child and his experience’
(Montessori [1914] 2005: 87). Montessori fostered purposeful activity,
often involving real-life activities, among children. She believed strongly in
children being given long periods of uninterrupted time to become deeply
involved in their activities. Hers was an individual approach with children
working alone or choosing to work with one or two others. Her approaches
let a child plan and assume control of learning. She promoted development
of children’s language and of their social and emotional understanding,
creating an ethos in which prosocial behaviour was valued.
Diamond and Lee (2011) report research showing that children from
Montessori schools surpassed children from other schools in their devel-
opment of executive functions.
It is interesting to note a contrast between the tools of the mind and the
Montessori approach. The former accords high priority to imaginary play for
pre-schoolers whereas the latter (in the original form) sidelines this form of play
as unhelpful. Nevertheless both approaches impact positively on executive func-
tioning. Executive functions can be developed in many ways, some of which
have not been fully researched yet, though there is growing interest in the pos-
sible role of music and singing (Winsler et al. 2011).
Consider the following questions to help you translate theory into practice:
Key points
• Going beyond reacting to immediate stimuli requires executive functions –
the ‘jockey skills’ for successful negotiation of the racecourse of life.
• Self-regulated behaviour and deliberate learning depend on executive
functions. They play an important role in school readiness and in life
trajectories.
• The executive functions fall into three main groups: working memory; inhibi-
tion; and cognitive flexibility. They start to develop in the order listed. Devel-
opment to mature levels takes a considerable time but there is a particularly
rapid period of development from 3–6 years of age.
• Lack of sleep, stress and insufficient exercise impede the development of
executive functions.
• Children’s experiences can promote the development of executive
functions.
• Vygotsky’s theory of sociocultural development of mental tools fits comfort-
ably with recent neuropsychological work on executive functions. The tools
of the mind curriculum translates Vygotskian theory into practice and has
been shown to be effective in helping children develop executive functioning
and self-regulation of behaviour.
• A range of other approaches or experiences, from traditional martial arts, to
mindfulness, to bilingualism, have been found to benefit executive function-
ing and it is likely that further effective approaches will be identified.
124 COMPETENCE AND EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS
Further reading
Bodrova, E. and Leong, D.J. (2007) Tools of the Mind: The Vygotskian Approach to Early
Childhood Education, 2nd edn, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill/Prentice Hall.
This is an extremely useful book. Chapter 5 on mediators explains their purpose clearly,
with many practical examples.
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS), London: Early Education.
Introduction to Part 4
The emphasis so far has been on children’s needs for relatedness, autonomy and
competence. In this final part we consider how we can take into account the
same psychological needs of parents and practitioners. This is never more impor-
tant than when trying to devise an effective positive behaviour plan for a child
whose behaviour is causing extreme concern.
Chapter 10 considers parents and Chapter 11 considers practitioners. Both
chapters contain a case study. In real life, ‘case studies’ are dynamic and all
involve children, parents and practitioners. The approach advocated in this book
is one of hypothesis testing and a collaborative process. It can be difficult to
capture within a static description of a case study the interplay of the different
elements and ideas as a situation unfolds and changes over time. In devising a
behaviour programme, practitioners need to liaise with parents and include their
views and ideas within the behaviour plan if it is to have maximum effectiveness.
For this reason it is important to avoid the impression that practitioners simply
inform the parents about the plan. Nevertheless, practitioners who are asking
parents in to discuss a child’s behaviour do need to prepare carefully for the
meeting if the time is to be used to good effect. Box P4.1 shows a possible outline
for such a meeting and Figure P4.1 shows a proforma that practitioners might
use to gather their ideas in advance of the meeting.
Thank everyone for coming and indicate your availability to parents in the
next few days once they have had a chance to reflect on the meeting.
Would they appreciate a phone call?
Note that the proforma highlights some of the key messages that have been
stressed about behaviour, such as the importance of building relationships and
the value of intrinsically motivating experiences. The case studies in the follow-
ing chapters use this proforma to capture the relevant information. Blank copies
of the meeting format and the proforma are also included as Appendices 1 and 2
to allow ease of photocopying for readers’ own use.
The case study in Chapter 10 involves a parent whose sense of competence
seems to be threatened by hearing about her child’s behaviour, a common sce-
nario. In Chapter 11 a teacher struggles with the feelings engendered by the diffi-
culty of forming a relationship with a child. The case studies try to capture some
of the process of interpreting behaviour through the self-determination model
(Deci and Ryan 2000), applying the model to the various ‘players’ and thinking
about the underlying needs yet translating the insights into constructive action
for those involved. Note when you read the case studies that the targets proposed
for children are not in terms of things they should stop doing (‘stop hitting’,
‘reduce tantrums’). Instead they are positive targets of new skills that should
help the child, such as early skills of negotiation and use of external mediators
to cope with transitions.
INTRODUCTION TO PART 4 127
Child: Parents:
Teacher/practitioners:
Child’s strengths
Intrinsic motivation/flow
Regardless of behaviour, we will make sure the child . . .
Relationship building
Ways in which we will try to build the relationship, irrespective of behaviour
Child’s target(s)
Positive, specific and achievable.
Figure P4.1 Proforma for preparing for meeting to discuss a child’s behaviour
10
Planning for positive behaviour
Parents’ viewpoints
Parents are a diverse group and it would be foolhardy to attempt any generalisa-
tions. The importance of keeping an open mind and avoiding hasty judgements
cannot be over-emphasised. Self-fulfilling prophecies, where teachers form a
negative judgement, and children and families ‘live down’ to it, are a real danger
(Rosenthal and Jacobson 1968; Hauser-Cram et al. 2003; Rosenthal 2003). Effective
work with parents involves establishing constructive dialogue (which includes
careful listening) and maintaining a focus on what is helpful for the child. The
Nigerian writer Chimamanda Adichie (online) talks powerfully about ‘the danger
of a single story’ and reminds us about the complexity, richness and strengths
that get lost when we resort to stereotypes. Practitioners need to be aware of the
danger of colluding with stereotypes of ‘uncaring’, ‘inadequate’ or ‘pushy’ parents.
The association between poverty and poor outcomes for children (including
behaviour problems) exists and it is important to acknowledge this when deci-
sions have to be made in targeting scarce resources. However, the demonisation
of sections of society through, for example, the government’s use of the term
‘troubled families’ (DCLG 2012) is not constructive. Poverty certainly makes peo-
ple’s lives harder but it is not invariably associated with children who misbehave
and underachieve. It is more likely that correlates of poverty, such as drug or
alcohol dependence or other mental health problems, account for the link with
poorer outcomes for children. Such problems may be less easily identified than
poverty. Again, even in cases of severe mental illness that might interfere with
bringing up children, families differ in the outcomes achieved as there may be
protective and supportive factors in the wider family. Belsky (1984) promotes a
model of parenting that considers not just the parents’ personalities and psycho-
logical well-being but also the social support they enjoy and the characteristics
of the child. While the research evidence suggests that all children can thrive,
given optimal care, it also acknowledges that some babies’ temperaments call
for greater internal resources from adults. Furthermore, there is likely to be a
question of ‘goodness of fit’ between children and adults, such that some pairings
find it particularly hard to attune. Adults and children in this situation need help
and support.
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PARENTS’ VIEWPOINTS 129
The focus of this book is on what adults can do to promote positive behav-
iour. Biological influences on behaviour have received less coverage. Increas-
ingly, as indicated in Chapter 1, the complexities of the interaction between
biology and environment are being unravelled but there is still much that is not
fully understood. An informed approach should use research knowledge to for-
mulate hypotheses about what behaviour might signify in terms of unmet needs
and to draw up plans accordingly. The initial hypothesis is a ‘best guess’. It might
not lead to the hoped-for success and another hypothesis might then need to be
considered and addressed. Parents should certainly not be blamed for a child’s
behaviour and the tentative nature of attempts to help should be acknowledged,
with parents treated as partners in trying to formulate a plan of action.
We will now consider each of the psychological needs in turn and how set-
tings might work with parents in ways that take account of the particular need
under consideration. Readers may find it useful to consult the bullet points in the
Introductions to the earlier parts of the book since they summarise the environ-
ments that are supportive or undermining for each of the psychological needs of
relatedness, autonomy and competence.
Relatedness
• Settings need to be open with, and welcoming of, parents from their initial
contact. Parents will come from a whole range of backgrounds and there
may be some with little understanding of early years education and care in
the UK. Some will have their own unfortunate memories of educational estab-
lishments. Many will be unfamiliar with the terminology of the EYFS (DfE
2014b) which practitioners may unwittingly use (e.g. ‘doing his learning jour-
ney’, ‘my key group’). Practitioners should try to be friendly and clear without
being condescending and should encourage parents to ask for clarification.
• Be informed about sources of support for families in the area and display
literature relating to these. Find out as much about them as you can by visit-
ing, talking to the professionals involved and/or to families from your set-
ting who have used the service. If you build relationships with parents and
suggest a particular service to them from a position of knowledge, there is
a greater likelihood that they will access it than if they simply see a poster
advertising the same service.
• Behaviour issues can be embarrassing for parents and other parents can
ostracise a parent whose child is hurting others. Children in the UK are not
legally obliged to attend school until the term following their fifth birthday.
Some parents of children who display challenging behaviour withdraw the
child from early education. This tends to compound any problems because
the child then misses the opportunity to mix with other pre-schoolers and
develop social and behavioural skills before school entry. Take a strong line
in your behaviour policy about every child’s right to a positive approach to
130 PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PARENTS’ VIEWPOINTS
any behavioural needs, assuring all parents that informed individual plans
will be drawn up and implemented as necessary and that they should discuss
any concerns with the manager. In some cases, managers have stamped out
unhelpful muttering among parents by calling a meeting to explain that an
action plan is in place and that trust and support from the community are
expected in helping any child with needs (a manager doing this would have
requested permission from the parent of the child whose behaviour is of con-
cern, and observed due confidentiality).
• Aim for balance in the information shared with parents of a child with con-
cerning behaviour. While incidents where others have been hurt must be
shared, ensure you do not give just the ‘bad news’. The child and parent are
to be valued despite certain behaviour being unacceptable. Ways of demon-
strating that value are likely to be individual to the case and it is important
to reflect on this and do it in a meaningful way.
• If a parent seems to be struggling to have a positive view of their child, ask
about exceptions to the negative picture: ‘When is Harry usually at his most
co-operative?’, ‘Are there times when you and Bethany have a lot of fun
together?’
Autonomy
• Consider acknowledging the sensitivity of discussing a child’s behaviour:
‘Behaviour can be such a sensitive topic, can’t it? No parent wants to have
to discuss how their child is hurting others. These things happen some-
times among young children and our interest is just in working with you
to help Madison enjoy pre-school and get on well with the other children.’
Competency
• Using a ‘story sack’ approach, prepare information and resources that par-
ents can borrow on a range of issues that link to behaviour such as ‘Good
nights – happy bedtime routines’, ‘A new baby’, ‘When someone close dies’.
Each pack should include brief but research-informed information for adults,
age-appropriate books and resources for adults and children to share and a
list of relevant local organisations.
• Raising an issue about a child’s behaviour almost invariably risks threaten-
ing a parent’s sense of competence. When people feel threatened, they tend to
become defensive and this can block open and constructive problem-solving.
Take particular care not to criticise, particularly at the early stages of working
with parents on their child’s behaviour. Effective approaches start with noticing
and commenting on positive aspects of parent–child interaction: ‘It’s lovely how
she’s always so pleased to see you when you collect her.’ ‘It’s great that you are
patient when she takes you round to see all the different things she has done.’
• If you invite parents in for extra meetings to discuss the child’s behaviour,
make sure they know what to expect – alert them if, for example, an Area
132 PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PARENTS’ VIEWPOINTS
Child: Parents:
Angelika, 4 Angie and Geoff
Teacher/practitioners:
Pauline, Key Person
Child’s strengths
Now settled well. Happy to play in a variety of areas and her achievements
are at the level expected. She is articulate and has a wide vocabulary.
Her physical skills (climbing, etc.) have really come on. She is socially
oriented, lively and wants to play with others.
Intrinsic motivation/flow
Regardless of behaviour, we will make sure the child . . .
Continues to have access to all the play opportunities and everything
available.
Relationship building
Ways in which we will try to build the relationship irrespective of behaviour
Pauline has a good relationship with A and will continue to have some
individual chats with her every day.
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PARENTS’ VIEWPOINTS 133
Through her observations, the SENCo thinks many of the disputes that
Angelika has with adults could be avoided by noting when she clashes with
other children and being aware of her increased susceptibility to enter into
disputes with adults in the period shortly afterwards. The SENCo feels it may
be possible to take preventative steps to minimise the likelihood of this chain
of events. Her initial hypotheses about Angelika’s behaviour with other children
encompass issues to do with autonomy and control and issues related to under-
standing others’ perspectives and finding compromises. The SENCo has gen-
erated some possible approaches that might help Angelika in particular while
being beneficial for all the children.
Pauline has mentioned that Angelika’s mother, Angie, is very uncomfort-
able at hearing about Angelika’s behaviour. When Angelika’s behaviour was first
mentioned, Angie was both surprised and anxious. She commented: ‘She’s not
like that at home’ and ‘I want her to be happy here – maybe it’s not right for
her’. Pauline asked how Angelika is with other children at home. Angie said she
is fine with her friends’ children and her cousin but they are all several years
older. Pauline remembered that Angelika did not start nursery until she was
31⁄2 and that the settling-in period had been hard for both Angie and Angelika –
particularly for Angie, she thought.
The SENCo has thus been alerted to Angie’s sensitivities. She looks forward
to meeting Angelika’s father, as yet unknown to the staff. Both parents come
and the meeting is intense and revealing. Angie’s husband, Geoff, is older and
tells them he was married previously when a very young man and has two sons,
now grown-up. He says he was too young then and did not fully appreciate his
boys or give them enough attention. They both talk with pride of Angelika. She
was a premature baby born after in vitro fertilisation and needing weeks in the
neonatal care unit. Angie becomes very emotional, talking of her ‘miracle baby’.
Geoff says: ‘Angelika is Angie’s world, she’s our world. It looked like she might
not make it through and now she’s so bright – you said yourself how well she
talks. Angie’s done a great job with her.’
When they are asked about Angelika’s experience with other children, Geoff
says, ‘Well, Angie’s friends’ children are all that bit older. That’s what I was
telling you, we had to wait and of course we didn’t know if it would work out
right in the end but it did and we got our little girl’. Pauline asks if Geoff’s boys
played together well as young kids. ‘I think so, no worse than any others. But I
wasn’t there much, I worked long hours. Unfortunately those boys didn’t get the
time and attention Angelika does, that’s my big regret.’
The discussion of the problems around Angelika’s conception and birth
is emotional and takes some time but the parents seem to want to share these
details and the setting staff gain an insight into Angelika’s status as a ‘miracle
baby’ and how this may have affected how much control she experiences at
home. Angelika mixes with older children when with her parents and they are probably
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PARENTS’ VIEWPOINTS 135
more likely to allow her what she wants than are the peers at nursery. The
SENCo asks Geoff and Angie if they would like to take a break now and whether
they would be able to come back to continue the meeting in a couple of days’
time. This suits everyone and so is agreed.
When the meeting resumes two days’ later, there is a rapport between the
parents and staff because of the emotions shared in the first meeting. The
parents are less emotional now and ready to engage in problem-solving and con-
structive discussion regarding a plan to help Angelika. Geoff says he realised
that Angelika has things her own way a lot but he hadn’t seen that this could
cause her problems. He comments that his boys had to learn to compromise
and ‘rub along’ because they had little choice. Geoff can sees various issues at
home, such as bedtimes, and thinks that change could be in everyone’s inter-
ests. He and Angie talk about how little time they get together in the evenings
because it is so hard to settle Angelika. Angie says she doesn’t like rushing
Angelika or leaving her at night when she wants more stories, a drink or some-
thing else because she wants Angelika to know how loved she is. Angie hadn’t
thought that it might be in Angelika’s interests (as well as easier for them as
parents) to be firmer about bedtime. Angie and Geoff agree some steps they
will take to put in firmer boundaries at home. They are happy for the strategies
that the SENCo suggests to be tried at the setting.
With the setting and parents working together, concerns about Angelika’s
behaviour decrease over time. Angelika, with her good verbal skills and social
orientation, soon becomes adept and creative in proposing compromises! The
first serious consultation with both parents over Angelika’s behaviour took two
relatively long meetings but the SENCo values the relationship formed with
the parents through listening to their story and feels that the time has been
invested well to promote partnership working.
11
Planning for positive behaviour
Practitioners’ viewpoints
Relatedness
• Settings and schools need to create an ethos where there are positive
relationships between staff but where any member, regardless of age or
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS 137
Autonomy
• While it is important to avoid colluding with views engendered by the
frustration of a colleague who is struggling, the feelings of such a col-
league should be acknowledged. In Chapter 5 the purpose of emotions,
including ‘negative’ ones, was discussed. Dreikurs (Dreikurs et al. 1971:
39) points out that practitioners’ fleeting gut reactions to a child’s behav-
iour are informative. He uses a different framework from that employed in
this book, but it is possible to translate his ideas into the terminology used
here (Table 11.1).
• A manager or SENCo can use Dreikur’s ideas both to acknowledge a prac-
titioner’s feelings but also to discuss constructive ways forward. Note
that the ‘gut responses’ to the feelings all feed into vicious cycles that
are likely to amplify and perpetuate the unwanted behaviour. The adult’s
executive functions and professionalism are needed to break out of such
cycles and to chart a more positive course, focused on the child’s needs.
‘Reframing’ a situation that causes negative emotion, particularly anger,
is one of the healthiest ways of managing the emotion (Goleman 1996;
Mauss 2005).
• The adults working closely with a child, whether practitioners, teachers
or teaching assistants need to be involved in devising any behaviour plan
for a child. An external expert or a SENCo may usefully suggest certain
138 PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS
Competence
• Practitioners need opportunities to continue to learn about children’s needs
and approaches for meeting them. If they are working with a child with a
known or suspected learning difference (for example, an autistic spectrum
difference or an attention and hyperactivity difference), they are likely to
benefit from opportunities to learn more. Training courses, reading and
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS 139
visits to settings that have developed expertise in helping children with simi-
lar needs can all be helpful.
• If a member of staff already has experience of the approach under consid-
eration, provide opportunities for that person to work alongside the prac-
titioner with less experience, modelling the approach with the child and
coaching the colleague.
• Managers and SENCos have a useful role to play in helping a practitioner
prioritise how to implement any new ideas, ensuring that changes are man-
ageable and staggered if necessary.
Child: Parents:
Ethan 51⁄2 Mr and Mrs White
Teacher/practitioners:
Tamsin Green, teacher
Hilary, teaching assistant
Child’s strengths
Wonderful Duplo construction and advanced language for thinking
displayed while working on it.
Intrinsic motivation/flow
Regardless of behaviour, we will make sure the child . . .
Has uninterrupted opportunities each day to carry out an activity of his
choice (likely to be Duplo or other constructional materials).
Relationship building
Ways in which we will try to build the relationship irrespective
of behaviour
See below. Ethan seems to benefit from a low-key, indirect approach. All
staff to try this and Hilary, teaching assistant, to try to continue to give
him some 1:1 time each day.
(Continued)
140 PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS
The SENCo sees this as a complex case where both the parents’ and the
teacher’s feelings could become a barrier to moving the situation forward. He
is interested in the fact that Ethan attended a Montessori nursery and was not
flagged up as presenting any behavioural challenges. He realises he needs
to handle matters sensitively with all concerned to maximise the chances of
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS 141
effective joint working. He prepares for the meeting with parents particularly
carefully, almost preparing a script using the meeting format.
A final, but crucial, part of the SENCo’s preparation for chairing the meeting
with parents is to have an informal discussion with the teacher. Tamsin Green
has been teaching for three years and is a warm teacher, popular with children,
parents and her colleagues. She has tried very hard to form a relationship with
Ethan, talking to him and often smiling at him but he turns away and there is no
positive response. The SENCo wonders if Tamsin’s attempts to be friendly might
be having the opposite effect to that she intends, making Ethan more wary of
her. Ethan hides under the table regularly and Tamsin tries to coax him out, to
no effect. She has also tried telling him to come out using a firm voice but Ethan
stays put. The SENCo recognises that the situation with Ethan is the greatest
challenge Tamsin has faced so far in terms of forming a relationship with a child
in her class. The SENCo has noticed that this normally caring and warm teacher
(Continued)
142 PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS
is beginning to talk in terms of ‘something wrong’ with Ethan, rather than ‘ways
to help’ him. He realises he will need to work hard to keep Tamsin on board as
part of the solution to the situation.
The SENCo arranges to see Tamsin and, over tea and biscuits, encourages
the young teacher to talk about the uncomfortable feelings that Ethan is engen-
dering in her. He says to Tamsin that the cocktail of hurt, hopelessness and
anger she is experiencing is a testament to how much she has tried to build a
relationship with Ethan and to encourage him to meet behavioural expectations.
The SENCo says these feelings are what he would expect from a passionate,
caring teacher. The SENCo admits he is still puzzling over what to make of Ethan
but points out that Tamsin’s feelings might be saying something about Ethan’s
needs. Tamsin feels rejected by him and this might mean either that Ethan is
distrustful because of rejection experiences in his past or that he cannot really
understand relationships and is baffled and may be anxious about attempts
to form them. The SENCo comments that a child who hides under tables for
extensive periods is not a happy child and needs support. He says he is not
sure how best to understand Ethan but is struck by his relationship difficulties,
his anxiety and rigidity. They will need to meet his parents and then formulate
a plan. The SENCo says he is sorry that Tamsin is struggling with the situation
and that part of the plan for Ethan will include regular ‘debriefings’ for Tamsin
and her teaching assistant where, among other things, they can express their
feelings in a safe and confidential environment. For the meeting with Ethan’s
parents, though, the SENCo states that he is confident he can rely on Tamsin to
be her usual factual and positive self.
In the meeting, the SENCo uses his prepared notes to lead the meeting but
takes care to draw out the parents’ views and to include everyone in the plan-
ning. He describes his observations. Tamsin pre-empts one of the SENCo’s own
ideas by saying, ‘Ethan might find it easier if I’m less bouncy and “Tiggerish”
when I talk to him’. The SENCo feels that Tamsin is fully on board again and that
the extra meeting with her was worthwhile.
Ethan’s parents describe him as ‘self-contained’ and acknowledge that he
thrives on routine and needs time for individual, uninterrupted activity. At home
he is no problem but he dislikes anything that breaks normal routines. They
feel the individual approach at the Montessori nursery was helpful for him, as
was the generous amount of physical space. They know school is making him
anxious and support the ideas of an individual visual timetable and warning
of transitions. At home they have quite set routines (for example, the same
sequence of meals every week and set times for visiting grandparents) because
this works best for Ethan. One of Ethan’s older cousins has been diagnosed as
having Asperger’s syndrome and they say that they hoped Ethan was fine but
they have used some of the strategies Mrs White’s sister finds good for the
cousin diagnosed with Asperger’s syndrome because they found them useful.
The SENCo does not pursue the question of a possible diagnosis for Ethan at
PLANNING FOR POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR: PRACTITIONERS’ VIEWPOINTS 143
this stage but says that the school will monitor how he reacts to the behaviour
plan they put in place and the dialogue with the family will continue.
After the parents have gone, Tamsin says she knows little about Asperger’s
syndrome but would be interested in learning more. This the SENCo also sees
as a positive indication of Tamsin’s constructive interest in Ethan and he prom-
ises to pass her some relevant literature, thinking that she will feel more com-
petent for informing herself. He stresses that she should avoid diagnosis and
concentrate on understanding which strategies might help and why.
Appendix 1
Format for meeting with parents
Meeting format
• Thank attendees for coming, particularly parents.
• Introductions.
• Purpose: to share information about (child’s name)’s behaviour with a
view to drawing on everyone’s views to find things we might be able to
do to help.
• Pleased to have (child’s name), outline strengths. Put behaviour in context
(e.g. ‘quite rare but still concerning’, a couple of times a week which, while
concerning means there are more days that are fine than not’).
• Ask parents ‘How are you feeling about (child’s name) at nursery?’ How
is (child’s name) at home? If relevant: and why do you think there is a
difference?
• Outline the concerns, giving information from observations.
• Suggest initial hypothesis, appropriately worded.
• Invite comments.
• Revise or refine hypothesis in the light of the discussion.
• Suggest ideas about ways forward:
• whole group level;
• changes in the physical environment;
• changes in how adults react;
• strategies to support the child (mediators?);
• positive target for child.
• Ask for ways parents can help.
• Ask for additional suggestions or modifications to suggestions.
APPENDIX 1 145
• Stress that you will be providing positive experiences for the child not con-
tingent on behaviour (and be prepared to justify).
• Agree review arrangements.
• Thank everyone for coming. Indicate availability for parents in the coming
days after they have had a chance to reflect on the meeting. Would they
appreciate a phone call?
Appendix 2
Proforma for preparing for meeting
to discuss a child’s behaviour
Child: Parents:
Teacher/practitioners:
Child’s strengths
Intrinsic motivation/flow
Regardless of behaviour, we will make sure the child . . .
Relationship building
Ways in which we will try and build the relationship, irrespective of behaviour
Child’s target(s)
Positive, specific and achievable.
Figure A2.1 Proforma for preparing for meeting to discuss a child’s behaviour
Glossary
Glossary words are shown in bold on their first occurrence in the text.
Attachment bond – the emotional tie between two people. The first and most
important attachment bonds are between and baby and the main carer(s).
Behavioural learning theory (behaviourism) – a school of psychology which
looks at the external and the measurable, at behaviour rather than cogni-
tions or feelings. The ways stimuli are presented and the effects that fol-
low behaviours (contingencies) influence learning in specifiable ways (for
example, behaviours that are rewarded tend to be repeated).
Contingent reward – a reward of the type ‘do x and you will be rewarded by y’.
The reward is usually presumed to increase the likelihood of someone doing
x. When an activity is intrinsically rewarding, the addition of a contingent
reward can dent motivation in the medium to long term.
Correlation – when there is a demonstrated relationship between two (or more)
factors, they correlate. An example is that responsive, attuned caregiving
correlates with secure attachment. A correlation does not necessarily indi-
cate that one factor causes the other.
Differential susceptibility – this is the idea that individual differences that have
tended to be associated with less good outcomes may not always result in
these. There are some indications that characteristics that in suboptimal envi-
ronments are considered risk factors may actually be positive characteristics
in more favourable environments, resulting in better-than-average outcomes.
Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS) – the statutory standards for the learn-
ing, development and care of children from birth to 5 years old in England. All
schools and Ofsted-registered early years providers must follow the EYFS,
including childminders, pre-schools, nurseries and school reception classes.
Elaborative talk – talk that goes beyond description and explores reasons.
Elaborative talk by an adult helps a child to understand events in context
and to evaluate them. It also supports memory of events.
148 GLOSSARY
them willingly most of the time. With ‘controlled’ internalisation, the picture
is less healthy. The child behaves from fear, from duty or a desire to retain
love – though the behaviour may appear ‘good’, the child feels like a puppet
on a string. Controlled internalisation puts children under strain and inter-
feres with the development of autonomy.
Internalised behaviour difficulties – behaviour that is problematic and
directed against the self. In the extreme this includes self-harming behav-
iour or eating disorders. Very high levels of anxiety, social withdrawal and
an extremely poor self-image are other examples.
Intrinsic motivation – motivation that comes from within and is not depend-
ent on external recognition or rewards. Intrinsically motivated activities are
those that someone chooses to do for the pleasure involved in doing them.
Joint attention – when adult and child share attention to an object, for exam-
ple, when a child looks to see what the adult is looking at or when a child
directs the adult’s attention to an object by pointing towards it.
Key Person – it is a statutory requirement in England that a child attending a
childcare and education setting be allocated a Key Person. This is in recogni-
tion of the importance of attachment. The Key Person’s most important role
is to form a close relationship with the child and the family.
Leading activity – a term from Vygotskian psychology, the leading activity
for a developmental level is the one which will have the most impact for
development.
Longitudinal study – in a longitudinal study, researchers follow children over
time and continue to collect data. Some longitudinal studies have followed
children well into adulthood. Studying the same children over time provides
much richer and often more meaningful information than do studies con-
ducted at a single point in time. Longitudinal studies may indicate the long-
term effects of particular strategies or factors.
Mediator – a Vygotskian term for an object that helps a child regulate an aspect
of behaviour.
Mental state talk – talking that refers to what people feel, think, know, remem-
ber and believe.
Mental tools (‘tools of the mind’) – a Vygotskian term to describe the men-
tal processes that children develop which enable them to direct their own
learning and behaviour rather than just being at the mercy of immediate
environmental influences. The tools of the mind are culturally determined.
Language is one of the most powerful tools of the mind.
Meta-emotion philosophy – one’s general belief system about feelings and
emotions and the degree to which they should be accorded importance.
Mind-minded – a term to describe adults whose behaviour indicates that they
take account of children’s minds, for example, they notice and correctly
interpret and comment on a pre-verbal child’s reactions.
Mindset – an internalised set of ideas through which people interpret their expe-
riences. Dweck (2008) contrasts a growth mindset (where effort can always
150 GLOSSARY
Adamson, L. and Frick, J. (2003) The Still-Face: a history of a shared experimental para-
digm, Infancy, 4(4): 451–73.
Adichie, C. (n.d.) TED talk: the danger of a single story. Available at: www.ted.com/talks/
chimamanda_adichie_the_danger_of_a_single_story (accessed 19 August 2014).
Ainsworth, M.D.S. (1978) Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange
Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ainsworth, M.D.S. and Bell, S.M. (1970) Attachment, exploration and separation: illustrated
by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation, Child Development, 41: 49–67.
Ainsworth, M.D.S. and Marvin, R.S. (1995) On the shaping of a theory and research:
an interview with Mary D.S. Ainsworth (Fall 1994), Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 60(2/3): 3–21.
Allen, G. (2011) Early Intervention: The Next Steps. An Independent Report to Her Maj-
esty’s Government, Available at: http://media.education.gov.uk/assets/files/pdf/g/
graham%20allens%20review%20of%20early%20intervention.pdf (accessed 2 February
2012).
Bakermans-Kranburg, M., van IJzendoorn, M. and Juffer, F. (2003) Less is more: meta-
analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood, Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 129: 195–215.
Baumeister, R.F., Campbell, J.D., Krueger, J.I. and Vohs, K.D. (2003) Does high self-esteem
cause better performance, interpersonal success, happiness or healthier lifestyles?
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4(1): 1–44.
Baumrind, D. (1966) Effects of authoritative parental control on child behaviour, Child
Development, 37(4): 887–907.
Belsky, J. (1984) The determinants of parenting: a process model, Child Development, 5:
83–96.
Belsky, J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. and Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. (2007) For better and
for worse: differential susceptibility to environmental influences, Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 16: 300–4.
Bergen, D. and Mauer, D. (2000) Symbolic play, phonological awareness and literacy skills
at three age levels, in K.A. Roskos and J.F. Christie (eds) Play and Literacy in Early
Childhood: Research from Multiple Perspectives. New York: Erlbaum.
REFERENCES 153
Berkman, L.F. and Glass, T. (2000) Social integration, social networks, social support, and
health, in L.F. Berkman and I. Kawachi (eds) Social Epidemiology. New York: Oxford
University Press.
Bierman, K.L., Nix, R.L., Greenberg M.T. et al. (2008) Executive functions and school readi-
ness intervention: impact, moderation, and mediation in the Head Start REDI pro-
gram, Development and Psychopathology, 20: 821–43.
Blakemore, S.-J. and Frith, U. (2005) The Learning Brain: Lessons for Education. Oxford:
Blackwell Publishing.
Bodrova, E. and Leong, D.J. (2007) Tools of the Mind. The Vygotskian Approach to Early
Childhood Education, 2nd edn, Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Bowlby, J. (1966) Maternal Care and Mental Health. A Report Prepared on Behalf of the
World Health Organization as a Contribution to the United Nations Programme for
the Welfare of Homeless Children. New York: Schocken.
Bowlby, J. (2005) The Making and Breaking of Affectional Bonds. London: Routledge
Classics.
Bradbury, B.M., Corak, J., Waldfogel, J. and Washbrook, E. (2012) Inequality in early
child outcomes, in J. Ermisch, M. Jäntti and T. Smeeding (eds) From Parents to
Children: The Intergenerational Transmission of Advantage. New York: Russell Sage
Foundation.
Bretherton, I. (1992) The origins of attachment theory: John Bowlby and Mary Ainsworth,
Developmental Psychology, 28: 759–75.
Bretherton, I. (2010) Fathers in attachment theory and research: a review, Early Child
Development and Care, 180(1–2): 9–23.
Bronson, P. and Merryman, A. (2010) Nurture Shock. Reading: Ebury Press.
Brown, J.R., Donelan-McCall, N. and Dunn, J. (1996) Why talk about mental states? The sig-
nificance of children’s conversations with friends, siblings and mothers, Child Devel-
opment, 67(3): 836–49.
Chorpita, B.F. and Barlow, D.H. (1998) The development of anxiety: the role of control in
the early environment, Psychological Bulletin, 124(1): 3–21.
Corpus, H.J. and Lepper, M.R. (2007) The effect of person versus performance praise on
children’s motivation: gender and age as moderating factors, Educational Psychol-
ogy, 27(4): 487–508.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975) Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. Experiencing Flow in Work
and Play. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Cutting, A.L. and Dunn, J. (2006) Conversations with siblings and with friends: links
between relationship quality and social understanding, British Journal of Develop-
mental Psychology, 24: 73–87.
David, T., Goouch, K., Powell, S. and Abbott, L. (2003) Birth to Three Matters: A Review of
the Literature. London: Department for Education and Skills.
DCLG (Department for Communities and Local Government) (2012) Helping Troubled
Families Turn Their Lives Around. Available at: https://www.gov.uk/government/
policies/helping-troubled-families-turn-their-lives-around (accessed 31 October 2014).
Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2000) The ‘what’ and ‘why’ of goal pursuits: human needs and
the self-determination of behaviour, Psychological Inquiry, 11(4): 227–68.
Deci, E.L. and Ryan, R.M. (2002) The Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Roches-
ter, NY: University of Rochester Press.
Dettling, A., Gunnar, M. and Donzella, B. (1999) Cortisol levels of young children in full-day
childcare settings, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 24: 519–36.
154 REFERENCES
DfE (Department for Education) (2012) Statutory Framework for the Early Years Foundation
Stage 2012. Available at: http://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20130401151715/
https://www.education.gov.uk/publications/standard/allpublications/page1/
dfe-00023-2012 (accessed 20 October 2014).
DfE (Department for Education) (2013) Working Together to Safeguard Children. Avail-
able at: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/working-together-to-safeguard-
children (accessed 25 October 2014).
DfE (Department for Education) (2014a) Permanent and Fixed-Period Exclusions in
England, 2012–2013. Available at: www.gov.uk/government/statistics/permanent-
and-fixed-period-exclusions-in-england-2012-to-2013 (accessed 25 August 2014).
DfE (Department for Education) (2014b) Statutory Framework for the Early Years
Foundation Stage: Setting the Standards for Learning, Development and Care
for Children from Birth to Five. Available at: http://www.foundationyears.org.uk/
eyfs-statutory-framework/ (accessed 20 October 2014).
DfES (Department for Education and Skills) (2007) The Early Years Foundation Stage:
Setting the Standards for Learning, Development and Care for Children from Birth
to Five. Nottingham: DfES Publications.
Diamond, A. (2013) Executive functions, Annual Review of Psychology, 64: 135–68.
Diamond, A. and Lee, K. (2011) Interventions shown to aid executive function development
in children 4–12 years old, Science, 333: 956–64.
Diener, C.I. and Dweck, C.S. (1978) An analysis of learned helplessness: continuous
changes in performance, strategy and achievement cognitions following failure,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36: 451–62.
Dix, T., Stewart, A.D., Gershoff, E.T. and Day, W.H. (2007) Autonomy and children’s reac-
tions to being controlled: evidence that both compliance and defiance may be positive
markers in early development, Child Development, 78(4): 1204– 21.
Dreikurs, R., Grunwald, B.B. and Pepper, F.C. (1971) Maintaining Sanity in the Class-
room: Illustrated Teaching Techniques. London: Harper and Row.
Dunn, J. (1988) The Beginnings of Social Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
Dunn, J. (2004) Children’s Friendships: The Beginnings of Intimacy. Oxford: Blackwell.
Dunn, J., Brown, J. and Beardsall, L. (1991) Family talk about feeling states and children’s
later understanding of others’ emotions, Developmental Psychology, 27(3): 448–55.
Dweck, C.S. (2000) Self-Theories. Their Role in Motivation, Personality and Develop-
ment. Hove: Psychology Press.
Dweck, C.S. (2008) Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. New York: Ballantine Books.
Dweck, C.S. (2013) Social development, in P.D. Zelazo (ed.) The Oxford Handbook of Devel-
opmental Psychology, vol. 2: Self and Other. Available at: http://www.oxfordhand-
books.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958474.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199958474-
e-008?rskey=bRvHVS&result=2 (accessed 30 July 2014).
Dweck, C.S., Davidson, W., Nelson, S. and Enna, B. (1978) Sex differences in learned help-
lessness: I Differential debilitation with peer and adult evaluators, Developmental
Psychology, 14: 258–78.
Early Education supported by the Department for Education (2012) Development Matters
in the Early Years Foundation Stage (EYFS). London: Early Education.
Ensor, R. and Hughes, C. (2005) More than talk: relations between emotion understanding
and positive behaviour in toddlers, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23:
343–63.
REFERENCES 155
Evangelou, M., Sylva, K., Kyriacou, M. et al. (2009) Early Years Learning and Develop-
ment. Literature Review. Research Report DCSF-RR176. London: Department for
Children, Schools and Families.
Galyer, K.T. and Evans, I.M. (2001) Pretend play and the development of emotion regula-
tion in preschool children, Early Child Development and Care, 166(1): 93–108.
Gerhardt, S. (2004) Why Love Matters: How Affection Shapes a Baby’s Brain. Hove:
Brunner-Routledge.
Goldschmied, E. and Jackson, S. (2004) People Under Three: Young Children in Day Care,
2nd edn. London: Routledge.
Goleman, D. (1996) Emotional Intelligence. Why It Can Matter More Than IQ. London:
Bloomsbury.
Gopnik, A, Meltzoff, A and Kuhl, P (2001) How Babies Think. London: Orion.
Goswami, U. and Bryant, P. (2007) Children’s Cognitive Development and Learning (Pri-
mary Review Research Survey 2/1a), Cambridge: University of Cambridge Faculty
of Education.
Gottman, J., with Declaire, J. (1997) The Heart of Parenting. How to Raise an Emotionally
Intelligent Child. London: Bloomsbury.
Gottman, J.M., Katz, L.F. and Hooven, C. (1996) Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the
emotional life of families: theoretical models and preliminary data, Journal of Family
Psychology, 10(3): 243–68.
Gralinski, J.H. and Kopp, C.B. (1993) Everyday rules for behavior: mothers’ requests to
young children, Developmental Psychology, 29(3): 573–84.
Gray, C. (2001) My Social Stories Book. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Greenberg, M.T., Kusche, C.A., Cook, E.T. and Quamma, J.P. (1995) Promoting emotional
competence in school-aged children: the effects of the PATHS curriculum, Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 7: 117–36.
Gresham, F.M., Gansle, K.A., Noell, G.H. et al. (1993) Treatment integrity of school-based
behavioral intervention studies: 1980–1990, School Psychology Review, 22: 254–72.
Grieshaber, S.J. and McArdle, F. (2010) The Trouble with Play. Maidenhead: Open Univer-
sity Press.
Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K.E., Kindler, H. and Zimmermann, P. (2008) A wider view of
attachment and exploration: the influence of mothers and fathers on the development
of psychological security from infancy to young adulthood, in J. Cassidy and P.R.
Shaver (eds) Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research and Clinical Applications,
2nd edn. New York: Guilford Press.
Gunderson, E.A., Gripshover, S.J., Romero, C. et al. (2013) Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds
predicts children’s motivational frameworks 5 years later, Child Development, 84(5):
1526–41.
Gunnar, M.R. (1978) Changing a frightening toy into a pleasant toy by allowing the infant
to control its actions, Developmental Psychology, 14(2): 157–62.
Gunnar, M.R. (1980) Control, warning signals and distress in infancy, Developmental Psy-
chology, 16(4): 281–89.
Gunnar, M.R., Larson, M.C., Hertsgaard, L. et al. (1992) The stressfulness of separation
among nine-month-old infants: effects of social context variables and infant tempera-
ment, Child Development, 63(2): 290–303.
Hammond, C. (2011) Mind changers: Walter Mischel’s marshmallow study, BBC Radio 4.
Available at: http://www.bbc.co.uk/programmes/b00ymjpr (accessed 15 September
2014).
156 REFERENCES
Harris, P.L. (2006) Use your words, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24:
253–61.
Harris, P.L., de Rosnay, M. and Pons, F. (2005) Language and children’s understanding of
mental states, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14: 69–73.
Hart, B. and Risley, T.R. (1995) Meaningful Differences. London: Brooks Publishing.
Hauser-Cram,P., Sirin, S.R. and Stipek, D. (2003) When teachers’ and parents’ values dif-
fer: teachers’ ratings of academic competence in children from low-income families,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(4): 813–20.
Hooper, J. (2012) What Children Need to Be Happy, Confident and Successful: Step by Step
Positive Psychology to Help Children Flourish. London: Jessica Kingsley.
Hughes, C. and Dunn, J. (2000) Hedonism or empathy? Hard-to-manage children’s moral
awareness, and links with cognitive and maternal characteristics, British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 18: 227–45.
Hughes, C. and Dunn, J. (2002) When I say a naughty word: a longitudinal study of young
children’s accounts of anger in themselves and close others, British Journal of Devel-
opmental Psychology, 20(4): 515–35.
Humphrey, N. (1976) The social function of intellect, in P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde
(eds) Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jack, R., Garrod, O. and Schyns, P. (2014) Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit
an evolving hierarchy of signals over time, Current Biology, 24(2): 187–92.
Jogulekar, A. (2012) Chocolate consumption and Nobel Prizes: a bizarre juxtaposition if
ever there was one. Available at: http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/the-curious-
wavefunction/2012/11/20/chocolate-consumption-and-nobel-prizes-a-bizarre-juxta-
position-if-there-ever-was-one/ (accessed 5 September 2014).
Kochanska, G. (2002) Committed compliance, moral self and internalization: a mediational
model, Developmental Psychology, 38(3): 339–51.
Kochanska, G., Barry, R.A., Stellern, S.A. and O’Bleness, J.J. (2009) Early attachment
organisation moderates the parent–child mutually coercive pathway to children’s
antisocial conduct, Child Development, 80(4): 1288–300.
Kochanska, G. and Sanghag ,K. (2013) Difficult temperament moderates links between
maternal responsiveness and children’s compliance and behaviour problems in low-
income families, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3): 323–32.
Koestner, R. and Losier, G.F. (2002) Distinguishing three ways of being highly motivated:
a closer look at introjection, identification and intrinsic motivation, in E.L. Deci
and R.M. Ryan (eds) The Handbook of Self-determination Research. Rochester, NY:
University of Rochester Press.
Krevans, J. and Gibbs, J.C. (1996) Parents’ use of inductive discipline: relations to
children’s empathy and prosocial behaviour, Child Development, 67: 3263–77.
Kuczynski, L. and Kochanska, G. (1990) Development of children’s noncompliance strate-
gies, Developmental Psychology, 26(3): 398–408.
Lagattuta, K.H. and Wellman, H.M. (2001) Thinking about the past: early knowledge about
links between prior experience, thinking and emotion, Child Development, 72: 82–102.
Lamb, M.E. (1978) Qualitative aspects of mother–infant and father–infant attachments in
the second year of life, Infant Behaviour and Development, 1: 265–75.
Lamb, M.E. (2010) The Role of the Father in Child Development, 5th edn. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Lepper, M., Greene, D. and Nisbett, R. (1973) Undermining children’s intrinsic interest with
extrinsic rewards: a test of the ‘overjustification’ hypothesis, Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 28(1): 129–37.
REFERENCES 157
Leslie, A.M. (1987) Pretense and representation: the origins of ‘theory of mind’, Psychologi-
cal Review, 4: 412–26.
Licht, B., Simoni, H. and Perrig-Chiello, P. (2008) Conflict between peers in infancy and
toddler age: what do they fight about? Early Years: An International Research Jour-
nal, 28(3): 235–49.
Lyons-Ruth, K. (1996) Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behav-
iour problems: the role of disorganized early attachment patterns, Journal of Con-
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 64: 64–73.
Maccoby, E.M. (1980) Social Development: Psychological Growth and the Parent–Child
Relationship. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
MacLure, M., Jones, L., Holmes, R. and MacRae, C. (2012) Becoming a problem: behaviour
and reputation in the early years classroom, British Educational Research Journal,
38: 447–71.
Main, M. (1996) Introduction to the special section on attachment and psychopathology:
2. overview of the field of attachment, Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychia-
try, 64(2): 237–43.
Main, M. and Hesse, E. (1990) Parents’ unresolved traumatic experiences are related to
infant disorganized attachment status: is frightened and/or frightening parental
behavior the linking mechanism? in M.T. Greenberg, D. Cichetti and E.M. Cummings
(eds) Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research and Intervention. Chi-
cago: University of Chicago Press.
Main, M. and Solomon, J. (1990) Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized-disori-
ented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation, in M. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti and E.M.
Cummings (eds) Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research and Interven-
tion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Martins, C. and Gaffan, E.A. (2000) Effects of maternal depression on patterns of infant–
mother attachment: a meta-analytic investigation, Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 41(6): 737–46.
Maselko, J., Kubzansky, L., Lipsitt, L. and Buka, S.L. (2011) Mother’s affection at 8 months
predicts emotional distress in adulthood, Journal of Epidemiology and Community
Health, 65(7): 621–5.
Maslow, A.H. (1943) A theory of human motivation, Psychological Review, 50(4): 370–93.
Mauss, I. (2005) Control your anger, Scientific American Mind, 16(4): 64–71.
Meins, E. (2014) Attachment, in A. Wenzel (ed.) The Oxford Handbook of Perina-
tal Psychology, Oxford Handbooks. Available at: www.oxfordhandbooks.com/
view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199778072.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199778072-e-
005?rskey=TtNcDv&result=5 (accessed 20 September 2014).
Meins, E., Ferneyhough, C., de Rosnay, M. et al. (2012) Mind-mindedness as a multidi-
mensional construct: appropriate and nonattuned mind-related comments indepen-
dently predict infant–mother attachment in a socially diverse sample, Infancy, 17(4):
393–415.
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. and Rodriguez, M.L. (1989) Delay of gratification in children, Sci-
ence, 244(4907): 933–8.
Moffitt, T., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D. et al. (2011) A gradient of childhood self-control pre-
dicts health, wealth, and public safety, PNAS, Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(7): 2693–8.
Montessori, M. (2005) Dr. Montessori’s Own Handbook. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications.
(Original work published in 1914).
158 REFERENCES
Moullin, S., Waldfogel, J. and Washbrook, E. (2014) Baby bonds, parenting bonds:
parenting, attachment and a secure base for children. Available at: http://www.
suttontrust.com/wp-content/uploads/2014/03/baby-bonds-final.pdf (accessed 5 June
2014).
Mueller, C.M. and Dweck, C.S. (1998) Intelligence praise can undermine motivation and
performance, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75: 33–52.
Newland, L.A. and Coyl, D.D. (2010) Fathers’ role as attachment figures: an interview with
Sir Richard Bowlby, Early Child Development and Care, 180(1 & 2): 25–32.
Noble, T. and McGrath, H. (2013) Well-being and resilience in education, in L. Boni-
well, S.A. David, and A.C. Ayers (eds) The Oxford Book of Happiness, Oxford
Handbooks. Available at: http://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/
9780199557257.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199557257-e-043 (accessed 21 April 2014).
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D. and Gushkin, K. (1995) Helplessness in
children of depressed and nondepressed mothers, Developmental Psychology, 31:
377–87.
Overmeier, J.B. and Seligman, M.E.P. (1967) Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent
escape and avoidance responding, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy-
chology, 63: 28–33.
Parish-Morris, J., Golinkoff, R.M. and Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2013) From coo to code: a brief
story of language development, in P.D. Zelazo (ed.) The Oxford Handbook of Devel-
opmental Psychology, vol. 1: Body and Mind, Oxford Handbooks. Available at:
http://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199958450.001.0001/
oxfordhb-9780199958450-e-30?rskey=Abg7Rw&result=1 (accessed 31 October 2014).
Parker, G. (1983) Parental Overprotection: A Risk Factor in Psychosocial Development.
New York: Grune and Stratton.
Paulssen-Hoogeboom, M.C., Stams, G.J., Hermanns, J.M., et al. (2008) Parenting style as
a mediator between children’s negative emotionality and problematic behaviour in
early childhood, Journal of Genetic Psychology, 169(3): 209–26.
Porter, L. (2013) Applied behaviour analysis: its applications and limitations, in T. Cole, H.
Daniels, and J. Visser (eds) The Routledge International Companion to Emotional
and Behavioural Difficulties. London: Routledge.
Power, T.J., Blom-Hoffman, J., Clarke, A. et al .(2005) Reconceptualizing intervention
integrity: a partnership-based framework for linking research with practice, Psychol-
ogy in the Schools, 42(5): 495–507.
Reddy, V. (2008) How Infants Know Minds. London: Harvard University Press.
Ripley, K. and Yuill, N. (2005) Patterns of language impairment and behaviour in boys
excluded from school, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(1): 37–50.
Roberts, J. and Donkin, A. with Pillas, P. (2014) Measuring What Matters: A Guide for
Children’s Centres. London: UCL Institute of Health Equity.
Rogers, C. (1961) On Becoming a Person. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Rose, J., Gilbert, L. and Smith, H. (2012) Emotion coaching, available at: http://www.
attachmentawareschools.com/resources/documents/Emotion_Coaching_Pilot_Find-
ings.pdf (accessed 7 September 2014).
Rosenthal, R. (2003) Covert communication in laboratories, classrooms and the truly real
world, Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(5): 151–4.
Rosenthal, R. and Jacobson, L. (1968) Pygmalion in the Classroom: Teacher Expectation
and Pupils’ Intellectual Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
Royer, E. (2013) Training and professional development for educators working with
children and young people with EBD, in T. Coles, H. Daniels and J. Visser (eds) The
REFERENCES 159
Volling, B.L., MacKinnon Lewis, C., Rabiner, D., and Baradaran, L.P. (1993) Children’s
social competence and sociometric status: further exploration of aggression, social
withdrawal, and peer rejection, Development and Psychopathology, 5: 459–83.
Vygotsky, L. (1978) Mind in Society. Cambridge MA: MIT Press. (Original work published
in 1930.)
Webster-Stratton, C. and Taylor, T. (2001) Nipping early risk factors in the bud: prevent-
ing substance abuse, delinquency, and violence in adolescence through interventions
targeted at young children (0–8 years), Prevention Science, 2(3): 165–92.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reinke, W.M., Herman, K.C. and Newcomer, L.L. (2011) The Incredible
Years Teacher classroom management training: the methods and principles that sup-
port fidelity of training delivery, School Psychology Review, 40(4): 509–29.
Welsh, J.A., Nix, R.L., Blair, C. et al. (2010) The development of cognitive skills and gains in
academic school readiness for children from low-income families, Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 102(1): 43–53.
Winnicott, D.W. (1965) The Maturational Process and the Facilitative Environment. New
York: International Universities Press.
Winsler, A., Ducenne, L. and Koury, A. (2011) Singing one’s way to self-regulation: the role
of early music and movement curricula and private speech. Early Education and
Development, 22: 274–304.
Index
AAI see Adult Attachment Inventory attunement 26, 36–8, 44–5, 128
ability see competence authenticity 79
abuse 4, 78 authoritarian parenting 55
‘acting out’ behaviours 41, 55 authoritative parenting 57–60
active engagement 117 autism 6, 138
adaptation to environment 32–3 auto education 122
Adichie, Chimamanda 128 autonomy 9–10, 47–84, 130–31, 137–8
Adult Attachment Inventory 18, 43, 45 being true to yourself 48–52
adversity 48 boundary setting 53–61
affection 18–19 emotions 62–9
affectionless control 59, 61 parents’ viewpoints on 130–31
aggression 41, 51, 57–8, 67, 71–3, 80–81, 83 practitioners’ viewpoints 137–8
Ainsworth, Mary 24–6, 30, 35–8, 44–5, 74 uniqueness 70–83
alcohol dependency 43, 128 autonomy-supporting environments 49,
ambivalent attachment 38–40, 45 70–71
anger 57–8, 65, 77–9 avoidant attachment 36–8, 45
anti-social behaviour 2 awareness of emotion 67
see also externalising behaviour
anxiety disorders 59, 61 baby massage 30, 44
anxious attachment 38–40, 45 bargaining 76
appropriate balance of behaviours 24 basic emotions 64–8
appropriate boundaries 56 basic needs 9–10
arbitrary rules 49, 57, 59–60 Baumrind, Diana 55–8
Asperger’s syndrome 142–3 behaviour patterns 41–2
aspirations 9 behavioural learning theory 24
assertion 76 being yourself see autonomy
‘attachment lens’ 36, 44–5 belonging see relatedness; uniqueness
‘attachment style’ 12 Belsky, J. 128
attachment theory 20–30 Bodrova, E. 117
key concepts of 22–30 body language 104, 107–9
attention-seeking 45 boredom 91
162 INDEX
Julia Manning-Morton
9780335246847 (Paperback)
October 2013
Key features:
www.openup.co.uk
Downloaded by [ Faculty of Nursing, Chiangmai University 5.62.156.86] at [07/18/16]. Copyright © McGraw-Hill Global Education Holdings, LLC. Not to be redistributed or modified in any way without permission.
PROMOTING POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR
IN THE EARLY YEARS
“Karen is a highly respected professional who draws on a wealth of
experience working with young children to produce an accessible and
PROMOTING
POSITIVE BEHAVIOUR
practical book.”
Behaviour remains a key concern for many working or aspiring to work with
children and families. Written in a clear and readable style, this book considers
theory and recent research on children’s social and emotional development,
KAREN MORRIS
highlighting the implications for effective practice in promoting positive behaviour.
The book encourages you to gain a broad picture of factors underpinning more
challenging behaviour and to work positively and sensitively with children and
families. By carefully considering the factors which support or undermine the
fulfilment of children’s basic psychological needs, fresh light is shed on some
children’s behaviour. The approach also generates constructive ways of addressing
unmet needs and considers the child’s voice throughout.
With an emphasis firmly on finding ways to promote positive behaviour
and to equip you with effective strategies to help the child, the book
includes:
> A strong evidence base
> A rich range of helpful strategies
> Consideration of a range of viewpoints, including those of parents and
practitioners
> Links with the Early Years Foundation Stage
Morris
If you are working or training to work with young children this book has a highly
practical emphasis that will help you use theory to devise effective strategies to
support children.
Karen Morris is Senior Lecturer and Programme
Leader for the foundation degree and
undergraduate degree in Childhood Studies at
the University of Winchester, UK.