Chan Ko Wai
Chan Ko Wai
Chan Ko Wai
Master's thesis
University of Lapland
Spring 2020
University of Lapland, Faculty of Education
The title of the pro gradu thesis: Phenomenological study about enhancing university
student's psychosocial wellbeing through YouTube videos: Autonomous Sensory Meridian
Response (ASMR) in Finland
Author: Chan Ko Wai
Degree programme: Media Education
The type of work: Pro gradu thesis_X_Laudatur thesis__Licenciate thesis__
Number of pages: 87 + 7 Appendices
Year: 2020
Abstract. The present qualitative study aimed to explore how the attitudes, experiences, and
feelings of Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) videos could be used to
enhance psychosocial wellbeing similar to mindfulness-based treatment programs. ASMR
is an atypical and multisensory phenomenon in which a tingling sensation is often elicited
following specific audio-visual stimulations. Although ASMR experiences have been
announced to enhance wellbeing and phenomenologically superimposed with mindfulness,
there is a lack of research exploring how ASMR experiences relate to wellbeing. In this study,
the theoretical underpinnings of the Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (DSMM)
by Patti M. Valkenburg and Jochen Peter (2013) are merged with Positive psychology's
wellbeing theory (PERMA) by Seligman (2011) for the first time with the objective to explore
and link the distinguishing characteristics of ASMR video with wellbeing outcomes.
The study entailed two in-depth semi-structured interviews, which were conducted with three
university students (one female and two males) in Finland. Interpretative phenomenological
analysis with coding schemes was employed and guided by DSMM and Seligman's five
domains of well-being. Three main themes and fourteen subthemes emerged from the
analyses of the interview transcripts corresponding to the research question. The main
themes include ASMR intentional use, ASMR media contents, and ASMR multisensory
integration. The subthemes encompass prior ASMR-like experiences, social pressure from
family members and curiosity, different types of sound, voices, and movement; nostalgic
thoughts, pro-change bias, and positive responses. Exploration of interviewees' narratives
further brought to light issues concerning ASMR's emotional effects on wellbeing. Analyses
reveal different subthemes clustered into five main themes: Positive Emotion, Engagement,
Relationships (Connectedness), Meaning and Making a Connection, and Accomplishment.
The findings showed how ASMR videos enhance wellbeing through mindfulness-like
experiences. These research findings are significant due to the ASMR videos' potential to
increase wellbeing and happiness. Furthermore, their influence can extend to elevating the
ability to concentrate on tasks at hand, improving the quality of sleep by stopping rumination,
increasing self-confidence, and motivating altruistic behavior. The beneficial potential of
ASMR videos to promote psychosocial wellbeing is remarkable.
Keywords: ASMR, DSMM, Interpretative phenomenological analysis (IPA), Mindfulness,
PERMA, Psychosocial wellbeing
Further information: I give permission the pro gradu thesis to be read in the Library _X_
I give permission the pro gradu thesis to be read in the Provincial Library of Lapland _X_
List of Figures and Tables in the thesis
Figures
Figure 1. Total number of Views Per Month for Finnish ASMRtist Sita Sofia on
YouTube 3
Figure 5. Mind map of interview narrative subthemes and themes of the PERMA
model in relation to ASMR experience 41
Figure 6. Relative strength of the five PERMA themes and interconnections between
themes 66
Tables
Table 4. Themes and Subthemes for ASMR intentional use and responses 45
Table 5. Themes and Subthemes for ASMR media effects with wellbeing 51
i
List of Abbreviations
Augmented Reality AR
Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response ASMR
ASMR Artist ASMRtist
beats per minute bpm
Big Five Inventory Personality Test BFI
Combined Stimuli CS
Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model DSMM
dorsal Anterior Cingulate Gyrus dACG
Electroencephalography EEG
functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging fMRI
Heart Rate HR
Inferior Frontal Gyrus IFG
Interpretive Phenomenological Analysis IPA
medial prefrontal cortex mPFC
Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale MAAS
Non-Rapid Eye Movement NREM
Nucleus Accumbens NAcc
Positive emotion, Engagement, Relationship, Meaning and PERMA
Accomplishment
Region-Of-Interest ROI
Skin Conductance Level SCL
Supplementary Motor Area SMA
Toronto Mindfulness Scale TMS
ii
Table of Contents
1 – Introduction 1
1.1 ASMR: Definition, History and Online communities 1
1.2 ASMR in Finland 2
1.3 Research Problem and context 3
1.4 Research Questions and Objectives 4
2 – Background 4
2.1 ASMR characteristics and content 4
2.2 ASMR and individual differences 5
2.3 ASMR and other multisensory phenomena 7
3 – Conceptual Framework 9
3.1 Media Use & Wellbeing 9
3.2 In the direction of positive media psychology 10
3.3 Media Effect: Differential Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (DSMM) 11
3.4 PERMA model (Wellbeing Theory) 16
3.4.1 Positive emotion and previous research on ASMR 17
3.4.2 Engagement and previous research on ASMR Flow-like Mental State 19
3.4.3 Relationship and previous research on ASMR Social Connectedness 20
3.4.4 Meaning and Sense of Purpose 20
3.4.5 Accomplishment and Sense of Achievement 21
3.5 PERMA: Exploring media effects 21
4 – Literature Review 22
4.1 Psychophysiological study about ASMR and affective responses 22
4.2 Neurophysiological imaging studies about ASMR and Social Connectedness 24
4.2.1 ASMR related to empathy and social engagement 24
4.2.2 ASMR related to affiliative behaviors 25
4.3 Other studies on the benefits of ASMR engagement 25
4.3.1 Sensory Channel effects of ASMR on Short-term memory 25
4.3.2 ASMR combined with Binaural Beat Inducing for Sleep 26
iii
4.3.3 The linkage between ASMR & Mindfulness Meditation 28
4.4 Conclusion 29
5 – Research Methodology 30
5.1 Phenomenological Approach to Inquiry 30
5.2 Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis (IPA) 31
5.3 Theoretical Descriptions of IPA 32
5.4 Participant Selection 34
5.5 Material and Method 36
5.6 Interview schedule and protocol 37
5.7 Data Analysis 38
5.8 Coding Strategy 43
5.9 Validity and Reliability 43
6 – Findings 44
6.1 Students’ ASMR experiences 45
6.2 The effect of ASMR experiences on the psychosocial wellbeing
of students in Finland 51
References 76
Appendices
Appendix A Student Demographics Questionnaire 88
Appendix B Informed Consent Form 89
Appendix C Interview One: Focused ASMR Lived Experiences 91
Appendix D Interview Two: The Details of Experience 92
Appendix E ASMR digital flyer 93
Appendix F Coding scheme on ASMR & Wellbeing 94
iv
1 Introduction
ASMR phenomenon has become popular in Finland over the last years, and numerous
new ASMR content YouTube channels have been created. The first Finnish ASMR artists
were Matilda Koivisto and started in the 2014 WhiteWinterWhispers channel (Turtiainen,
2019, 27-28). Then, a famous Finnish YouTube video blogger Sita Salminen (Spa-
Roleplay-video), appeared, and her published ASMR channel was viewed over 20 million
times in September 2019, shown in Figure 1.
2
Kaamos’s period Kaamos’s period
Figure 1: Total Views Per Month for Finnish ASMRist Sita Sofia on YouTube. Adapted
from:https://socialblade.com/youtube/channel/UCjNyeUaGpD0o7z5VbruS6-A/monthly
Figure 1 shows that a sudden increase in the total views of Finnish ASMRtists channel in
the period from October to March in 2018-2019 and 2019-2020. This trend has raised the
question whether Finnish people could likely seek ASMR videos to relieve their kaamos
period problems and why the number of viewing increased in that period as the previous
research has shown that ASMR experiencers had reported feelings of relaxation and well-
being (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 1).
The polar night, also called kaamos, refers to the dark period that lasts for 51 days from
October to the beginning of March in the most northern part of Finland. Almost 40 percent
of Finnish people experience specific changes in mood and behavior, and about 9 percent
have symptoms of winter depression during the Kaamos period (Grimaldi, 2009, 9).
Besides, 30 percent of Finnish university students suffered from psychological problems,
including continuous overstrain (43 percent), feeling unhappy and depressed (27 percent),
difficulties concentrating on tasks at hand (32 percent), loss of sleep caused by worrying
(23 percent), and loss of self-confidence (23 percent). One-third of students reported
experiencing considerable stress, and 28 percent of female and 19 percent of male
students reported they worry a lot over study-related matters even in their free time (Kunttu
et al., 2017, 34-37, 68-72). It is also reported that ASMR can bring temporary relief for
those suffering from depression and phenomenologically overlap with mindfulness (Barratt
3
& Davis, 2015 1; Fredborg, Clark & Smith., 2018, 2). The research problem is justified by
previous literature, which mentions that mindfulness is associated with increased ASMR
intensity (Fredborg et al., 2018, 11). Therefore, the current study explores and examines
how ASMR videos can be used to enhance well-being similar to mindfulness-based
treatment programs.
RQ2. What kind of emotional effects from the ASMR experiences have on the
psychosocial wellbeing of students in Finland?
The RQ1 aims to explore the predictors of ASMR media use (e.g., motivation), the effects
of ASMR exposure on a variety of media responses (e.g., positive feelings, and thoughts),
and (RQ2) how these responses enhance media effects (psychosocial wellbeing) of the
university students in Finland.
2 Background
ASMR can be triggered not only by different types of external stimuli, but also by some
internal stimuli, such as focusing attention, recalling the memory of a previous ASMR,
meditating, or state of mind (Tihanyi, Ferentzi, Beissner, & Köteles., 2018, 7). For external
stimuli, ASMR is usually not elicited spontaneously but rather in response to particular
types of stimuli.
ASMR videos usually display an ASMRtist performing and producing sounds with their
voices. Sometimes they used objects to generate sounds. This general format is
transformed into different scenarios. Some of the videos are narrative and involving the
role-play of characters in particular situations. The most popular form of ASMR videos is
that the ASMRtist is performing extremely close to the camera and providing the viewer
with personal attention. The most popular form of ASMR videos is that they usually involve
the ASMRtist performing extremely close to the camera and providing the viewer with
personal attention. Some examples of this include haircut, doctor's appointment, and ear-
cleaning role-play. Other types of videos might involve only whispering or only object
4
sounds. A survey study by Barratt and Davis (2015) was found that whispering, low-
pitched repetitive noises such as tapping sounds, close-up attention, and videos depicting
socially intimate situations to show slow movements (e.g., hair-brushing) elicited tingling
sensation during ASMR engagement. The authors further found that other common ASMR
triggers include listening to and watching an individual tap on different objects, watching
someone open a package, and watching someone complete a mundane task, such as
drawing, painting the nails, or putting on make-up (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 6; Barratt,
Spence & Davis., 2017, 6; Fredborg, Clark, & Smith., 2017, 6). Most individuals like to
watch ASMR before bed, in quiet and relaxed environments, with binaural headphones
(Kovacevich & Huron, 2019, 40). Numerous videos reflect the subjective nature of ASMR
as different people prefer different triggers. As such, there are two critical aspects of
ASMR that need to be focused on and are elaborated in the following literature reviews:
(1) Personalities and psychological differences; and (2) Comparison with other
multisensory phenomena.
Fredborg, Clark & Smith (2017) utilized the Big Five Personality Inventory to examine the
personality traits that are associated with people who experience ASMR and those who do
not (Cash, Heisick, & Papesh., 2018, 2-3). The findings indicated that ASMR experiencers
got higher scores on Neuroticism and Openness-to-Experience but lower on
Conscientiousness, Extraversion, and Agreeableness (Kovacevich & Huron, 2019, 40).
However, McErlean and Banissy correspondingly investigated personality traits associated
with ASMR experiences. They used both Big Five personality inventory and Inter-personal
Reactivity Index, together with viewer preferences and motivation, to undergo a survey
(McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 5). Similar to Fredborg et al. (2017), their results indicated that
ASMR experiencers also got a higher score in Openness-to-Experience and lower on
5
Conscientiousness, but did not differ with Neuroticism, Agreeableness, and Extraversion.
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index was shown that ASMR viewers got a higher score in
both fantasizing and empathic concern (McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 9). They concluded
that ASMR could be a distinct but heterogeneous and highly personalized experience
(Barratt & Davis, 2015, 7; Fredborg, Clark, & Smith., 2017, 7). In the same way, the
perspective of both first-person and third-person from which the stimulus is presented may
also differentially affect one's ASMR experience (Fredborg et al., 2017, 7). Even though
Barratt & Davis (2015) researched participants who reported regularly watching ASMR
videos, their self-reports differed in common triggers, physiological experiences, and
psychological outcomes. These differences were suggested that the existence of individual
differences in ASMR susceptibility and consequences (Kovacevich & Huron, 2019, 40).
Keizer et al., 2020 further extended the research to investigate whether ASMR
experiencers have different self-representation in their brain. It is because bodily illusions
are usually utilized as tools for directing constructs such as self-awareness, interoception,
and plasticity of body representation (Keizer, Chang, O'Mahony, Schaap, & Stone., 2020,
3) and these constructs were measured by using the Sensory Suggestibility Scale (SSS).
Sensory suggestibility is usually illustrated as a personality trait related to how a person
responds to sensory information that is secretly being affected by someone else (Keizer et
al. 2020, 2). The findings showed that participants who experience ASMR are also more
inclined to have illusory sensory experience than controls (Keizer et al., 2020, 2). The
results were further suggested that ASMR is not only related to cognitive traits such as
imagery ability but also to how people physically experience sensory events (Keizer et al.,
2020, 1).
Besides, McErlean & Osborne-Ford (2020) was to illustrate further whether ASMR is
related to traits absorption (McErlean & Osborne-Ford, 2020, 7). Absorption is a stable trait
and capability of getting lost in the task at hand, whether it is watching a movie, reading a
book, or experiencing ASMR (Baños et al., 1999, 144). McErlean and Osborne-Ford
(2020) utilized the Tellegen Absorption Scale (TAS) to measure the degree of absorption
(deep involvement) and to perform an ASMR viewer survey (McErlean & Osborne-Ford.,
2020, 4). The findings demonstrated that ASMR experiencers showed an increase in
absorption compared to age and gender control groups (McErlean & Osborne-Ford. 2020,
5). This increase in the absorption phenomenon suggests that ASMR experiencers
increased the inclination for experiential involvement and strengthened the ability to fully
6
engage the current experience (Tellegen, 1981, 222; Jamieson, 2005, 122). As a result, it
is impressive to consider that the immersion in the virtual reality seems to be a crucial
component of technologically mediated ASMR and that elevated absorption has been
correlated to an extra virtual reality immersion experience (Baños et al., 1999, 147).
Moreover, increased absorption has been concerning the elevated openness-to-
experience (Weilbel, Wissmath & Mast., 2010, 254; Glisky et al., 1991, 263), which is also
enhanced among ASMR experiencers (McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 12; Fredborg et al.,
2017). ASMR has already been linked to another trait relevant to technologically mediated
ASMR, e.g., increased fantasizing (McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 12), which infer to increase
the imaginative proneness involvement in a fictional reality. As such, the combination of
increased absorption, openness-to-experience, and fantasizing may contribute to the
individual likelihood of experiencing ASMR (McErlean & Osborne-Ford, 2020, 8).
ASMR has been studied in conjunction with other sensory phenomena, including
synesthesia, misophonia, and aesthetic chills or frisson. In order to investigate a new
phenomenon such as ASMR, it is necessary to determine and differentiate the
characteristics of ASMR experience from other examples of atypical sensory associations
such as synaesthesia, frisson, and misophonia (Smith, Fredborg, & Kornelsen., 2019b, 3).
Synesthesia is defined as union or blending of the senses that one stimulus, such as a
number or letter, automatically initiated a secondary sensation, such as seeing a color
(photism) (Smith et al., 2019b, 3). The same cognitive or perceptual stimuli can
automatically trigger the same atypical sensory response (e.g., synesthetic photisms or
ASMR tingles) in both ASMR and synesthesia. Barratt and Davis (2015) found that 5.9
percent of ASMR experiencers also have synesthetic experiences, and the two
phenomena were suggested to overlap. (Fredborg et al., 2017, 3) Frisson, on the other
hand, is reported as the emotional chills that happen during an emotional response to
music (Del Campo & Kehle., 2016, 3). ASMR and frisson both involve an affective
constituent, and both experiences are related to substantial individual differences in stimuli
(Del Campo & Kehle, 2016, 4; Fredborg et al., 2019b, 3).
7
intimacy behaviors (e.g., watching someone perform make-up activities), whispering, and
repetitive sounds (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 6; Fredborg et al., 2017, 5). Nevertheless,
synesthesia is usually involved with the additional sensory responses to non-social stimuli
such as graphemes and somatosensory mirror-touch synesthesia experiences, such as
the sight of someone being touched, causing a tactile sensation on the corresponding part
of the body (Ward, 2013, 1). Second, the tingling sensations in ASMR are usually reported
as wave-like and dynamic, whereas the responses of grapheme-color synesthesia are
often the same across exposure times (Baron-Cohen, Wyke, & Binnie., 1987, 761; Del
Campo & Kehle, 2016, 1). Besides, the secondary sensory experiences with synesthesia
are uncontrollable, whereas ASMR experiences are autonomous but can intentionally be
stopped. (Fredborg et al., 2017, 2). Third, ASMR experiencers can predict the types of
stimuli to elicit tingling sensations (Fredborg et al., 2017, 6). On the contrary, the
responses of aesthetic chills are not predictable, whereas synesthetic responses are both
automatic and predictable (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2003, 51; Ward, 2013, 1). The
tingling sensations associated with frisson have inclined to propagate quickly throughout
the body.
On the other hand, ASMR tingles may last up to several minutes and the intensity of the
tingles can be changed throughout ASMR experience and could propagate from the
regions of head and neck to the peripheral regions of the whole body (Barratt & Davis,
2015, 7; Del Campo & Kehle, 2016, 1). Finally, the tingling sensations with ASMR are
often together with relaxation and joy (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 11), whereas frisson
experiences may be involved an exciting and arousing experience (Del Campo & Kehle,
2016, 5). All these differences have demonstrated that ASMR is a unique emotional and
perceptual phenomenon and has different functional neural activities from those recruited
during other atypical multisensory experiences (Smith et al., 2019b, 4).
8
ASMR and misophonia may be two ends of the same spectrum of synaesthesia-like
emotional responses (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 13). In research about the relationship
between ASMR and aesthetic chills, the authors concluded that the two phenomena share
several similarities, as mentioned above, and suggested that mindfulness is an essential
component in both. They both could promote various facets of happiness or subjective
well-being (Del Campo & Kehle, 2016, 5).
3 Conceptual Framework
Two recent criticisms of media effects research shape the current study, and this study is
an attempt to fill the research gap of the connection between ASMR media effects
research and psychosocial wellbeing. The first criticism shaping this study is by De Leeuw
and Buijzen (2016) that requested researchers to shift from adverse effects of media use
to more work on positive outcomes (De Leeuw & Buijzen, 2016, 41). Second, Valkenburg
and Peter (2013) argued that researchers are over-focused on finding "across-the-board"
(Valkenburg & Peter, 2013 a, 202) media effects. Hence, this generalization tends to
ignore small group variation both in the survey and experimental research because
individual difference variables are often used as covariates, and the randomization is
supposed to get rid of subgroup differences (Bonus, Peebles, Mares, & Sarmiento., 2018,
1). In the current study, the popularity of ASMR is exploited to answer the criticisms
mentioned above. Positive psychology's wellbeing theory or PERMA (Positive emotion,
Engagement, Relationship, Meaning, and Accomplishment) model (Seligman, 2011) is
used to explore how short-term ASMR experiences might contribute to psychosocial well-
being. Additionally, the framework of Valkenburg and Peter's (2013) Differential
Susceptibility to Media Effects Model (DSMM) is utilized to investigate and trace direct and
indirect media effects on individual differences in uses and responses (Bonus et al., 2018,
2). Thus, the PERMA model is embedded in DSMM and transformed into a new
conceptual framework shown in Figure 2.
9
RQ2
(A) Conditional Effects:
B3
Predictors
B2
C: Transactional Effects
Figure 2. Conceptual diagram of research questions and objectives. Adapted from P.M.
Valkenburg and J. Peter, 2013.
In Figure 2, the top two circles (RQ1 and RQ2) correspond to the research questions and aims of
the current study. The research question one is, "How do students experience ASMR?". The
objective is to explore the differential susceptibility variables of ASMR media use. Research question
two is, "What kind of emotional effects from the ASMR experiences have on the psychosocial
wellbeing of students in Finland?". The objective is to explore and trace the effects of ASMR
exposure on a variety of media responses. These responses on how to enhance psychosocial
wellbeing for university students in Finland. The conditional, three types of indirect and transactional
effects are represented by A, B1, B2, B3, and C.
10
investigated predictors of well-being for decades (Ryan & Deci. 2001, 142),
communication researchers have only recently focused attention on the potential of media
contributing to well-being (Rieger, Reinecke, Frischlich, & Bente., 2014, 17). For example,
Wellbeing (PERMA) theory was implemented to identify the strengths in existing games
that generate positive affect and social functioning, contributing to and supporting well-
being written by Jones, Scholes, Johnson, Katsikitis, & Carras (2014). On the other hand,
some researchers started to utilize DSMM in the positive media effects, such as Look on
the bright side (of media effects): Pokémon Go as a catalyst for positive life experiences
that was published by Bonus, Peebles, Mares, & Sarmiento (2018). Therefore, the current
project extends this developing line of research, focusing on wellbeing as an outcome of
media consumption of ASMR video (rather than video games). In the following sections,
the conceptual framework will be illuminated this potential: Valkenburg and Peter's (2013)
DSMM and PERMA with wellbeing theory.
The types of media effects addressed by DSMM include (A) conditional effects, (B) indirect
effects, and (C) transactional effect. There are four components in the DSMM: media use,
individual/differential susceptibility variables, response states, and media effects
(Landrum, Olshansky, & Richards., 2019, 3). According to the framework of the DSMM,
11
media use is referred to the intended use of media types, content (e.g., entertainment,
advertising), and technologies (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013, 222) and also includes a variety
of factors such as media content exposure, media channels selection, and frequency and
length of media use. Differential susceptibility variables predict media use (Landrum et al.,
2019, 4). In the current study, however, self-selected ASMR video clips are used and
viewed by participants and then try to find the interaction between the ASMR video clips
and the differential susceptibility variables on the response states of participants and
subsequent media effects.
The media effects are referred to as the deliberative and non-deliberative short- and long-
term within-person changes in cognitions, emotions, attitudes, beliefs, physiology, and
behavior that result from media use (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013, 222). In the context of
positive media effects, the DSMM suggests three essential tasks: 1) examine the factors
that predict exposure to positive media content; 2) examine how emotional, cognitive,
physiological, and social responses relate to well-being, and 3) examine how these media
response states are influenced by differential susceptibility factors (Bonus et al., 2018, 3).
Part of the newness of the current study lies in exploring all three tasks simultaneously and
the corresponding research objectives: the factors that predict ASMR, positive responses
to ASMR, the factors that moderate responses, and the associations between response
states and well-being. The other newness lies in focus on how ASMR, which affords a
variety of ways to positive outcomes not available in other media.
Conditional effects involve individual or social variables that moderate the direction or
strength of a media effect so that these variables determine who is susceptible to effects
related to media use (Houston, Spialek, & First., 2018, 7). The individual or social
variables are the characteristics of the individual, environments, or situations that influence
the nature of media effects. In addition to acting as moderators, these individual and social
variables can also function as predictors of media use (Houston et al., 2018, 7).
Valkenburg and Peter (2013b) have extended the differential susceptibility prototype to
focus specifically on susceptibility to media effects. They suggest that individual
differences can be grouped into three types of susceptibility: dispositional (P1),
developmental (P2), and social susceptibility (P3) (Piotrowski & Valkenburg, 2015, 1779).
12
Dispositional susceptibility (P1) is defined as all person-based characteristics that may
enhance their susceptibility to media effect; including genetics, gender, temperament,
personality, cognition, values, attitudes, beliefs, motivations, and moods (Beyens,
Valkenburg, & Piotrowski, 2018, 3; Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 226). Most dispositional
variables identified in the DSMM, including gender, personality/temperament, cognition
(e.g., scripts; and schemata), attitudes, motivations, identity, and moods (e.g., sadness,
happiness) have been shown to predispose media use (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 232).
The personality/temperament includes neuroticism, trait aggression, need for affect, need
for cognition, sensation seeking, and cognition encompasses scripts (i.e., a pre-existing
knowledge structure involving event sequences) and schemata (i.e., a pre-existing
knowledge structure in memory.).
Social susceptibility (P3) is defined as all social-context factors that may enhance or
reduce media effects, such as parenting style, media-specific parenting, or peer pressure
(Beyens et al., 2018, 4). These social contexts can act on a micro (e.g., family, friends,
peers), meso (e.g., school, church, work), and macro-level (e.g., cultural norms and
values) (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 227). Media effects are enhanced if the messages
assemble with the values, norms, and opinions in the social environment/context of the
media user. On the other hand, media users can either change their cognitions about the
media message or their cognitions about their social environment (Valkenburg & Peter,
2013b, 234-235). Social contexts can also moderate media response states during shared
media use because media users are very sensitive to others' attitudes, moods, and
emotional reactions. Their own cognitive, emotional, and excitative response states can be
intensified or dampened during shared media use (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 234).
Importantly, the DSMM interprets predictors of media use not just as motivations, but also
as moderators of the relationship between media use and responses. In other words,
different attitudes and experiences that motivate people to use particular types of media
might also modify the experiences that result from that use (Bonus et al., 2018, 8).
13
Therefore, these individual and social characteristics are likely to be media effects
moderators and media use predictors (Houston et al., 2018, 7).
The second form of media effects includes indirect effects addressed by three types of
media effect mediators (B1, B2, & B3). The first group of mediators involves situations
where media use (B1) functions as the mediators between an individual or social variable
and outcomes of media use (Houston et al., 2018, 7). For example, teenagers high in
sensation seeking are predisposed to use violent media, which in turn will reinforce their
aggressive behavior (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 223). The second form of potential
mediators in DSMM includes media response states. For example, exposure to an
arousing news item may stimulate viewers' attention and physiological arousal, which in
turn stimulates their recall of memory or attitudes toward the news issue (Valkenburg &
Peter, 2013b, 223-224). Media response states include that occur during or just after
exposure to media: cognitive (i.e., the attention to and processing of particular media
content), emotional (i.e., affective reactions, such as fear and joy while or after watching or
playing), and excitative (i.e., physiological arousal while or just after watching or playing)
response states that may, in turn, mediate other effects (Beyens et al., 2018, 2; Houston et
al., 2018, 7). The DSMM posits that media effects are indirectly mediated by media
response states of the media user that is originated from media use (Piotrowski &
Valkenburg, 2015, 1780-1781).
The cognitive response state (R1) refers to media users selectively attend to and invest
cognitive effort to understand media content, which is operationalized here as self-reported
concentration as well as physiological heart rate. Concepts like cognitive absorption,
reality perception, the cognitive dimensions of empathy, and counterarguing also represent
cognitive response states (Fikkers & Piotrowski, 2019, 3; Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b,
228).
Emotional response states (R2) are conceptualized as all affective reactions to media
content (i.e., the message, the storyline, and the vicarious affective reactions to
characters). The emotional dimension of state empathy (i.e., the experience of emotions
that are similar to those experienced by media characters) and sympathy are also seen as
emotional response states; and operationalized as positive and negative self-reported
emotions (Fikkers et al., 2019, 3; Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 228).
14
Excitative response states (R3) reflect the degree of physiological arousal in response to
media content, which operationalizes as self-reported and physiological arousal (Fikkers et
al., 2019, 3; Piotrowski & Valkenburg, 2015, 1780-1781). For example, ASMR is an
arousing (but not sexual) experience, and ASMR videos have consorted with boosted
exciting sensation and skin conductance levels (an indicator of physiological arousal)
(Poerio, Blakey, Hostler, & Veltri., 2018, 14). Each of these response states is expected to
change in response to different media.
The final type of indirect effect involves first-order media effects (B3) as a mediator of
other second-order media effects (Houston et al., 2018, 7). For example, adolescents' use
of social media can enhance their self-disclosure to friends, which in turn influences their
perceived quality of these friendships (Valkenburg & Peter, 2009, 79). Likewise,
informational media use enhances interpersonal discussion, which in turn, reinforces
participatory behavior (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 224).
15
(c) Transactional effects
The final form of media effect includes transactional media effects (C), or media effects
that influence media use (Houston et al., 2018, 8). The DSMM extends these earlier
models in two ways. First, it proposes that media outcomes influence media response
states. Second, it states that media outcomes affect the differential-susceptibility variables,
and thus media effects have a reciprocal causal effect on media processing, media use,
and the differential susceptibility variables (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 235). The media
effects drive media use, which may continue so that more media use continues to
strengthen effects, and this transactional interaction can continue in a reinforcing and
spiraling process as indicated by the bottom lines in Figure 2 (Houston et al., 2018, 8). As
an example, exposure to disaster media might increase levels of anxiety, and anxiety
might, in turn, motivate individuals to seek out more information from additional media
coverage to soothe that anxiety (Houston et al., 2018, 8).
Guided by DSMM, the current study is conducted by several in-depth interviews together
with interpretative phenomenological analysis to explore and summarize existing
knowledge in terms of ASMR media (a) use predictors, (b) effects moderators, (c) effects
mediators (response states, media use, media effects) (d) transactional effects, and (e)
direct effects.
16
The current focus of medical psychology on pathology has limitations as a psychological
treatment approach because the "fix-what' s-wrong" approach gives an incomplete picture
of the human potential to improve themselves (Umucu et al., 2019, 2). In modern
psychology, the examination on well-being and its predictors has been seen as a revival
since the positive psychology movement has begun to attempt shifting the traditional focus
of (clinical) psychological research away from pathology and strived for a stronger
emphasis on factors such as happiness and positive experiences (Seligman &
Csikszentmihalyi, 2000, 6). Traditionally, media effects research has been dominated by a
hedonic perspective on wellbeing and been treated media as a root of mood enhancement
(Zillmann, 1988, 331). Some research has recently begun to focus on the media use and
its effects on eudaimonic wellbeing, for example, by bringing up psychological growth via
"meaningful entertainment" (Oliver & Bartsch, 2011, 30). As such, Seligman's Wellbeing
Theory integrates the hedonic and eudaimonic views of wellbeing and proposes that
optimal wellbeing occurs when these two components of wellbeing are present within an
individual at the same time (Umucu et al., 2019, 2). Moreover, it allows for the
measurement of each element using both objective and subjective approaches (Forgeard,
Jayawickreme, Kern, & Seligman., 2011, 97) and the PERMA-Profiler is now proved as a
multidimensional scale with excellent reliability and acceptable levels of convergent,
divergent, and criterion-related validity (Umucu et al., 2019, 1). Thus, the PERMA model is
chosen to utilize in the current study.
Positive emotion is the feeling of happiness, joy, cheer, and the many other descriptors of
good feelings (Jones et al., 2014, 2). A good level-appropriated emotion could include
feelings of deep pleasure (Hidayat et al., 2018, 149-150). It can enhance attention, help to
generate creative and flexible ideas, and broaden the self-concept to include others more
17
readily (Kok, Catalino, & Fredrickson., 2008, 5). Moreover, it also was associated with
greater self-regulation (Tice, Baumeister, Shmueli, & Muraven., 2007, 383), task
persistence, and goal adoption (Fishbach & Labroo., 2007, 169). In other words, college
students' feelings of strong positive emotions are more likely to absorb deeper in learning,
and the students will result in better academic achievement (Hidayat et al., 2018, 150).
Rising the public acknowledgment of ASMR media suggests that people are upsurge
using ASMR videos as therapeutic tools, including sleep and mood disorders (Poerio et
al., 2018, 3). ASMR could be used for providing temporary relief to individuals with
depression, stress, and chronic pain (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 1). The previous findings had
shown that fifty percent of participants said their mood improved even in sessions when no
tingling sensation was produced, and 30 percent said that achieving this sensation was
vital to mood improvement (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 8). The outcomes of this research
suggest that ASMR can give temporary alleviation in the mood for those suffering from
depression, in company with many people consciously utilizing it for this intention (Barratt
& Davis, 2015, 11). Besides, sixty-nine percent of those who scored moderate to severe
on the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) claimed using ASMR to relieve their depressive
symptoms (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 8) and suggested more elevating effect of using ASMR
than those without depression (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 11).
In 2018, a large-scale online experiment was further conducted where participants viewed
a set of three videos (ASMR and control) and then reported their tingles and affective
response (Poerio et al., 2018, 3). The participants watched both spoken and sound-only
videos. For example, a spoken-only video was a haircut play-acting and included triggers
of whispering, nuanced movements of the hand, and close-up personal attention. A sound-
only video, for example, was a snapshot of hands folded a piece of paper, which included
triggers of the repetitive and slow hand movements and scratching sounds (Poerio et al.,
2018, 4). After watching both types of ASMR videos, ASMR participants compared to non-
ASMR participants felt significantly more excited, calmer, and less stressed (Poerio et al.,
2018, 8) and were consistent with the results in previous Barratt & Davis (2015) research.
To sum up, listening to an ASMR video with inaudible or unintelligible whispering likewise
forces the listener to focus and concentrate on the sound and voice itself, reducing
extraneous impressions from her surroundings and achieving for relaxation purposes
(Klausen, 2019, 94).
18
3.4.2 Engagement and previous research on ASMR Flow-like Mental State
Engagement refers to the connection one feels to the activities one is doing as well as
feelings of being absorbed in and focuses on those activities (Seligman, 2011, 11). An
activity with a high level of engagement in an activity is also referred to as "flow"
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), or the overall feeling of being "in the zone" (Tansey et al., 2018,
132).
Barratt & Davis (2015) proposed that ASMR is a "flow-like" phenomenon obtained by
viewing the similar state of intense focus performed by others and diminished awareness
of the passage of time (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 12). Anecdotal reports of ASMR illustrate a
state of focus, of more substantial "presence" and relaxation, which is similar to the non-
active aspects of flow (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 3). This passive feature is notably resonant
with ASMR, along with both states encompassing the sensation of deep relaxation and
well-being, even if the passive feature of flow is task-directed, whereas ASMR seems to
involve entire passivity from the person (Barratt, Spence, & Davis., 2017, 2). The scholar
utilized a modified version of the Flow State Scale (Jackson & Marsh, 1996, 1), which taps
into the passive aspects of flow. The findings showed that participants with greater
susceptibility to flow had been found to report a higher number of ASMR triggers,
highlighting a link between the two phenomena (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 12). It suggests
that flow may be necessary to achieve sensations associated with ASMR (Barratt & Davis,
2015, 1). Intriguingly, some of the most popular ASMR triggers, such as some specialists
hand movements, in immensely focused states (e.g., carrying out medical exams) or
engaged in repetitive tasks (e.g., folding towels), are typical examples of being in a state of
flow (McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 610). ASMR is induced by obtaining a flow-like state,
which is facilitated by observing others partly in such a state. The transference of state
from performers to the audience has been examined in studies pursuing the role of mirror
neurons (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 12). Besides, participants prefer content that is happy,
inviting, relaxed, and lacks danger, suggests that popular ASMR videos centering around
the manipulation of objects may induce an effortless, flow-like feel to content that is
conducive to ASMR induction (Barratt et al., 2017, 9, 11).
19
3.4.3 Relationship and previous research on ASMR Social Connectedness
A positive relationship is understood as the feeling of being cared about by others, socially
integrated, and supported and involves a sense of connectedness, loving,
and sharing emotions with others (Tansey et al., 2018, 132). The previous research has
been found that good relationships with friends are positively related to self-esteem, and
that increase in friendship quality perception is associated with the elevation of wellbeing
(Bagwell, Bender, Andreassi, Kinoshita, Montarello, & Muller., 2005, 252). Furthermore,
college students socialize more frequently and have more solid romantic and social
relationships, and they have a tendency to be happier than students without these
relationships (Diener & Seligman, 2002, 82).
Poerio et al. (2018) researched to investigate whether ASMR videos produced feelings of
connectedness. The results were shown that the spoken-only ASMR videos also made
ASMR participants feel more socially connected compared to non-ASMR participants, a
result that did not occur for the sound-only ASMR videos (Poerio et al., 2018, 8). This
result suggests that increase connectedness may be an additional benefit of ASMR
because of the social and interpersonal context triggered by ASMR. One possible
explanation is that ASMR simulates a form of social grooming, for instance, being calmed
and comforted by another through the tactile-like tingling sensations induced by ASMR
triggers. This grooming simulation enhances well-being and interpersonal bonding through
diminutions in heart rate and circulation of endorphins (Poerio et al., 2018, 14). ASMR
videos create a form of embodied and technologically mediated presence, tactile
sensations, and distant intimacy or closeness through sonically binaural qualities as well
as through narratives supported by sound in the form of ASMR role-play videos, vibrations
of sound and image (Klausen, 2019, 99). Thus, ASMR can be characterized as a form of
'social audio-grooming,' as the ASMR videos provide first-person-like social attention and
care in the form of technologically mediated grooming (Klausen, 2019, 94).
20
meaning in life and frequently involves a pro-social or altruistic intent such as a
commitment to helping others or improving the world (Noble & McGrath, 2015, 11).
Additionally, recent work has suggested that media can indirectly impact wellbeing via
short-term responses to exposure. For example, playing an (Augmented Reality) AR video
game can lead to an array of positive, short-term outcomes that also relate to shifts in well-
being (Bonus et al., 2018, 18). Thus, the current study can extend this idea to explore how
and in what ways the indirect effects of viewing ASMR on "PERMA" wellbeing via ASMR
users' media response states.
21
4 Literature Review
ASMR has upsurge in popularity, and the research about ASMR has simultaneously
increased since 2015 up to now continued. However, whether it is a real phenomenon still
under debate. In an investigation of how expectancy effects alter ASMR, participants with
earlier unfamiliarity of ASMR were more likely to get the experience of ASMR after their
expectations were manipulated (Cash, Heisick, & Papesh., 2018, 11). ASMR participants,
however, were not influenced by the same manipulations. The researchers construed that
ASMR experiencers might be the consequence of expectancy effects; that merely
accepting a specific type of stimulus to bring on a sensory experience will give rise to that
sensory experience. (Cash, Heisick, & Papesh., 2018, 12). Thus, ASMR experience, or
part of it, could be suggested as the product of a placebo effect (Valtakari, Hooge,
Benjamins, & Keizer., 2019, 2). On account of the controversial essence of ASMR,
researchers have been vastly interested in an attempt to examine whether the subjective
experience of ASMR can be utilized to more objective physiological measures (Valtakari et
al., 2019, 2). Besides, the neurophysiological underpinnings may give other insights into
the possible mechanism of the ASMR phenomenon (Smith, Fredborg, & Kornelsen., 2019,
3). Therefore, the objective approaches enable scholars to investigate how ASMR
influences somatosensation and emotion rather than describing those experiences (Smith
et al., 2019, 3). Thus, this chapter will review the available literature in various objective
scientific methods relevant to this study, namely psychophysiological and
neurophysiological, and the literature concerning DSMM and PERMA model has already
been included in the previous chapter 3. Finally, this chapter will explore other ASMR
benefits and how it could be used within the online community.
The findings showed that ASMR videos promoted both calmness and excitement feelings
while diminishing heart rate and elevating skin conductance levels (Valtakari et al., 2019,
2). These results are curious in that increased SCLs are typically associated with
physiological arousal, whereas a slowed heart rate has consorted with the opposite side of
arousal (Johnston & Anastasiades, 1990, 25; Shapiro et al., 2001, 12). The researchers
concluded that these apparently contradictory results are possibly related to the
complicated situations of the ASMR experience. Self-report studies have consistently
shown that ASMR is associated with a feeling of calm (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 11;
Fredborg, Clark & Smith., 2017, 2). However, the psychophysiological data gives a clue
that ASMR is an experience of physiological arousal as well (Smith et al., 2019a, 2). The
same pattern of somatovisceral activity has been associated with surprisingly different
emotions, and the same emotion has been associated with different patterns of
somatovisceral activity (Norman, Berntson, & Cacioppo., 2014, 117). Recent findings also
pointed out that physiological responses in various somatic systems (e.g., heart, skin) are
likely underlying different patterns of neural interactions (Poerio et al., 2018, 14) and both
the sympathetic and parasympathetic interactions and compartments in HR and SCL are
in different involvement (Eisenbarth, Chang, & Wager., 2016, 11995).
Emotions are sometimes more complex than how we feel, and the mixed emotions have
been defined as affective experiences characterized by the co-activation of two emotions,
usually opposite in valence (Berrios, Totterdell, & Kellett., 2015, 1). As such, ASMR is a
complex emotional blend comprising of activating and deactivating positive affect (Poerio
et al., 2018, 14) and that affect system is flexible enough to spontaneously permit multiple
activation patterns, ranging from bipolar affect reactions to different blends of mixed
emotions (Berrios et al., 2015, 12). The presence of positive and negative emotions
enhances physical health; people find a way to feel good when feeling bad; allowing for
positive affect to be experienced concurrently with negative affect prompts individuals to
face adverse events in life and gain insight into them (Hershfield, Scheibe, Sims, &
Carstensen., 2013, 54). ASMR may offer an opportunity to understand the emotional
23
complexity of individual differences capabilities, and the potential positive influences of
mixed emotional experiences on health and wellbeing (Poerio et al., 2018, 14).
There was a significant brain activation in the mPFC regions that were associated with
self-awareness, social cognition, and social grooming during ASMR, and the result may be
indicated that ASMR videos could activate the brain regions similar to that of actual social
engagement (Lochte et al., 2018, 300). In the meantime, oxytocin has been shown to bind
to receptors in the mPFC and mediate relaxation responses (Sabihi, Dong, Durosko, &
Leuner., 2014, 1) and mPFC was activated during ASMR to be possibly suggested that a
potential contribution of oxytocin to the relaxing sensations during tingles (Lochte et al.,
2018, 300). On the other hand, the network of the dACC, SMA, and insula was also
considered to be involved in empathy (Fan, Duncan, De Greck, & Northoff., 2011, 903)
and the activation of these regions during ASMR tingles further was given the evidence for
the correlation of ASMR with social cognition and caring feelings towards others (Lochte et
al., 2018, 301). This finding was in line with a prior study reported that ASMR experiencers
scored higher for empathic concern than control participants (McErlean & Banissy, 2017,
613). Also, the association of ASMR and increased empathy may be suggested that
ASMR could also be associated with personality traits and mental health. The previous
24
study utilized the Big Five Inventory to assess five personality traits in ASMR experiences
and control, and the findings showed that ASMR experiencers had significantly higher
scores for the trait of 'openness-to-experience' than control (Fredborg et al., 2017, 1).
Affiliative behaviors are the caring behaviors that occur between parents and children,
romantic partners, and friends, and these behaviors involve close interpersonal bonds and
can include grooming behaviors along with a broader array of caregiving and care-
receiving behaviors (Feldman, 2012, 381). The attention-receiving or observation granting
behaviors in many ASMR videos have similarities to being cared for or to looking after
someone (Lochte et al., 2018, 302). Previous fMRI studies have shown that some affiliate
behaviors include potent activation of the mPFC, NAcc, insula, and IFG that were strongly
activated during ASMR (Feldman, 2012, 384). Moreover, the neurohormones widely
reported to be responsible for most affiliative behaviors including dopamine, oxytocin, and
endorphins (Feldman, 2012, 382-384; Colonnello, Petrocchi, Farinelli, & Ottaviani., 2017,
545), and these hormones were known for inducing feelings of comfort, relaxation, and
sleepiness that were self-reported with ASMR. Thus, ASMR may be activating the brain
regions and releasing neurochemicals typically associated with affiliative behaviors
(Lochte et al., 2018, 302). This neurobiochemical pathway may give reasons why ASMR
video makes individuals relax, cope with stress, diminish their anxiety, and quickly fall
asleep.
Previous studies were never focused on investigating sensory channels for delivering
ASMR stimuli but tried to examine the overall mental states and neural involvement
associated with ASMR (Kim et al., 2019, 226). There was enough evidence for the
relationship between emotion and memory; such as recent findings indicated that
emotional stimuli engaged specific cognitive and neural mechanisms that enhanced
explicit memory (Hamann, 2001, 396); and the amygdala and hippocampal complex acted
in concert when emotional situations happened (Phelps, 2004, 201). Hence, Kim et al.
(2019) underwent research to examine the sensory channel effects of ASMR on short-term
memory. ASMR and negative affect contents were prepared to compare their differences
of short-term memory performance according to three types of sensory channels; such as
25
'visual,’ 'auditory,' and 'visual + auditory' channels (Kim et al., 2019, 226). Forty-five
participants who were randomly assigned to each of three sensory channel conditions took
part in the experiments, which consisted of three sessions: training session, negative
affect session, and ASMR session (Kim et al., 2019, 227). Through three sessions, each of
the participants perceived negative affect and ASMR contents using the assigned sensory
channel and then conducted short-term memory tests, respectively. The experimental
results showed that the difference in short-term memory test scores between negative
affect and ASMR contents is statistically significant only for the 'auditory' channel (Kim et
al., 2019, 229). It means that the positive effects of ASMR contents on short-term memory
are significant only through the 'auditory' channel, so it would be recommended to use
ASMR sounds for increasing short-term memory performance (Kim et al., 2019, 230).
Sleep is essential to maintain physical and cognitive functions in everyday life, but sleep
disorders are on the rise (Song & Lee, 2019, 1). This problem could be better solved by
inducing sleep employing an auditory stimulus to improve the quality of sleep than other
methods; such as transcranial direct current stimulation, transcranial magnetic stimulation,
and pharmacological approaches (Song & Lee, 2019, 2).
Two almost equivalent pure tones of acoustic beats could have listened in each ear
simultaneously, and a binaural beat is generated, which induces brain signals at a specific
desired frequency (Song & Lee, 2019, 2). The binaural beat cannot be measured directly
but can be perceived by us because the brain is the origin of the beat, and it is observed
as an auditory illusion (Jirakittayakorn & Wongsawat, 2018, 1; Perez, Dumas, & Lehmann.,
2019, 4). Nevertheless, the unnatural and repetitive sound of the binaural beat could make
people feel uncomfortable (Crespo, Recuero, Galvez, & Begoña., 2013, 527), and thus,
this auditory stimulus is discomfort for users to induce sleep. To overcome the
disadvantage of the binaural beat, the perceptual phenomenon ASMR can be exploited to
trigger the calmness and relaxation feelings that are utilized by many people to relax their
negative moods to result in sleep (Barratt et al., 2017, 1) and the ASMR triggers were
presented from natural sound as the sensory stimuli (Song & Lee, 2019, 3).
The researcher proposed a novel auditory stimulus for inducing sleep that the binaural
beat was combined with randomized natural sounds (e.g., rain, sea waves, waterfall,
forest, and river) to entrain brainwaves with ASMR because some ASMR triggers (e.g.,
26
whispering, tapping, and crisp sounds) can induce a tingling sensation (Barratt & Davis,
2015, 6; Song & Lee, 2019, 3). Specifically, a 6 Hz binaural beat was utilized because this
frequency at which brain activity is entrained during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) in
sleep stage 1 (Song & Lee, 2019, 2-3). The best brainwave frequencies are studied as five
bands: (1) delta (0.5-4 Hz) are associated with deep sleep; (2) theta (4-8 Hz) are
associated with light sleep, creativity, and insight; (3) alpha (8-13 Hz) reflect a calm and
peaceful yet alert state; (4) beta (13-21 Hz) are associated with thinking, focusing state; (5)
high beta (20-32 Hz) are associated with intensity or anxiety (Huang & Charyton, 2008,
39). Prior studies suggested that the transition from wakefulness to sleep is noted by the
elevation of theta wave activity in the midline region of the brain (Wright, Badia, &
Wauquier., 1995, 888; Marzano, Moroni, Gorgoni, Nobili, Ferrara, & De Gennaro., 2013,
1). Thus, the research suggested that the change in the midline region of the brain should
be noted for sleep induction (Song & Lee, 2019, 3).
There were two sessions for this experiment. In session 1, two auditory stimuli (the 6 Hz
binaural beat and ASMR triggers) were combined at three-decibel ratios to identify the
optimal ratio and the combined stimuli (CS) ratios were shown as follows: (i) CS1- binaural
beats: ASMR triggers = 45:60; (ii) CS2 - binaural beats: ASMR triggers = 30:60; and (iii)
CS3 - binaural beats: ASMR triggers = 20:60 (Song & Lee, 2019, 3). The aim of the
session 1 was focused on whether theta power happens in each of the three CS
conditions with different decibel rates. In session 2, the outcomes of these CS were
compared with an only ASMR trigger, only binaural beat, or a sham condition (i.e.,
monaural beats: a silent stimulus was used with earphones in each ear). The objective of
this session investigated sleep induction and the continuous effects of auditory stimuli
(Song & Lee, 2019, 4). In both sessions, the changes of auditory stimuli in five frequency
bands were analyzed as the spectral elements of the electroencephalography (EEG)
signals (Jirakittayakorn & Wongsawat, 2018, 2). A paired t-test was then utilized to analyze
further which frequency was induced after listening to each auditory stimulus (Song & Lee,
2019, 5).
The results were shown as below: In session 1, the findings showed that the ratio of
binaural beat and ASMR trigger at 30:60 dB (CS2) was the most effective combinations to
induce both the theta power and psychological stability (Song & Lee, 2019, 5). In session
2, the combined stimulus still preserved the benefits of the binaural beat and settled its
weakness with the ASMR triggers, including psychological self-reports (Song & Lee, 2019,
27
6). The power of the theta power increased sharply in the midline brain region associated
with transition into sleep, especially after listening to CS2. The researchers supposed that
the escalation of theta power after CS2 in the midline region could induce sleep (Song &
Lee, 2019, 11).
Sleep and wakefulness are controlled by ascending arousal sleep-promoting and the
wake-promoting systems that are inhibited by each other, and these changes of different
neural oscillations are observed through EEG signals (Jirakittayakorn & Wongsawat, 2018,
2). When the binaural beats get into the primary auditory cortex, the nerve impulses are
transmitted directly to the relevant auditory areas and other associated areas. The arrival
impulse then induces the brain to resonate at a rate of the expected frequency of binaural
beats which can be measured by EEG rhythms and the phenomenon is called entrainment
of neural oscillation (Jirakittayakorn & Wongsawat, 2018, 2; Wahbeh, Calabrese, Zwickey,
& Zajdel., 2007, 200). Accurately, these signals enter the thalamus, where audio sensory
information is processed through the sensory neural pathway (Tang, Vitiello, Perlis, Mao,
& Riegel., 2014, 3). Eventually, the auditory signals in the thalamus may affect the sleep-
promoting system. As such, binaural beats can regulate the sleep cycle similar to their
usage for the modulation of behavioral states by entrainment effects (Jirakittayakorn &
Wongsawat, 2018, 3; Perez et al., 2019, 13). Regarding this mechanism, CS could
potentially induce sleep while keeping 6 Hz theta waves, which is the feature of NREM in
sleep stage 1 (Song & Lee, 2019, 12).
Finally, the findings indicated that the proposed auditory stimulus could induce sleep while
keeping the user in a psychologically relaxed state at the same time. This technology
provides a significant opportunity to develop a novel method for increasing the quality of
sleep (Song & Lee, 2019, 13).
Many people self-reported that ASMR is triggered when focusing on the external triggers,
which resembles mindfulness practice (Fredborg, Clark & Smith., 2018, 2). The
researchers underwent survey research to investigate the link between ASMR and
mindfulness. Mindfulness can be conceptualized as a trait and as a state. State
mindfulness refers to an ephemeral, from one moment to the next conscious experience
(Tanay & Bernstein, 2013, 1286), whereas trait mindfulness is a more stable and long-
28
lasting inclination to experience the world in a mindful manner (Brown & Ryan, 2003, 824).
Two hundred eighty-four people with ASMR completed the Toronto Mindfulness Scale
(TMS), the Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale (MAAS), and a questionnaire
examining ASMR experiences (Fredborg et al., 2018, 2). On the other hand, the controls
were requested to watch two ASMR videos to make sure that they did not experience
tingling sensations consorted with ASMR; and then they answered the TMS and MAAS
questionnaires (Fredborg et al., 2018, 5). The results were shown that ASMR experiencers
have a higher MAAS questionnaire score in terms of trait mindfulness than controls, and
this score differences further implicated one’s general predisposing to attention to and the
awareness of the present moment (Brown & Ryan, 2003, 824). Besides, they also scored
higher on the Curiosity subscale in terms of state mindfulness measure in TMS. The
findings further implicated the interest in one’s own inner experiences were elevated
among the ASMR group (Lau et al., 2006, 1447). ASMR female experiencers have a
higher Curiosity score than males one and further suggest that the cognitive foundations of
ASMR may be different between the sexes, and ASMR is a cognitively active process
(Fredborg et al., 2018, 7). The findings also indicated that perceptual curiosity is a factor of
ASMR experiences according to the pattern of ASMR triggers. The “Touching” and
“Repetitive Sounds” triggers significantly correlated with curiosity, and both types of
triggers may involve more top-down cognitive interpretation than triggers such as
whispering (Fredborg et al., 2018, 10). Listening to repetitive sounds and watching people
touch things may induce the observer to create a mental structure of the perceived stimuli.
Individuals have the motivation or curiosity to do so, and they would be more likely to
experience tingles (Fredborg et al., 2018, 10 -11).
Conclusion
This chapter has reviewed the literature that relates to this study and discusses previous
research and understanding of the subject area. It has discussed the current scientific
research approach from psychophysiological to neuroimaging techniques. Moreover, the
final part of the review mentioned the linkage between ASMR and mindfulness. The
authors further asked for additional research to examine whether ASMR has a connection
to well-being like mindfulness-based treatment programs (Fredborg et al., 2018, 11).
Hence, the current study attempts to explore and fill the research gap.
29
5. Research Methodology
The previous chapters reviewed the literature related to the ASMR phenomenon and
PERMA model concerning both subjective self-report and more objective scientific (e.g.,
psychophysiological and neurophysiological) ways of examination. The images on the
fMRI are not only a representation of reality, but they are necessary subjectively
interpreted by someone (first-person) who knows how to read rather than objective in the
phenomenological view (Stadlander, 2011, 43). Therefore, phenomenology could give
neuroscience an insight to pinpoint what structures need to be explained in human
experience and what to seek for (Stadlande, 2011, 44). Now, ASMR was identified as the
multisensory atypical phenomenon under the empirical study. This chapter outlines the
methodology that was employed including the phenomenological approach to inquiry, the
use of Interpretive Phenomenological Analysis (IPA) as a methodology for exploring
ASMR experiences of YouTube video, by the selection of participants and ASMR video
types, (informed consent) ethical considerations, how data were collected and analyzed
and the quality and validity of the study.
IPA was developed by Johnathan Smith and his colleagues and has become a popular
methodology of phenomenological analysis. It focuses on the individual human
experiences and insights to understand the subjective world of people under consideration
of the sociocultural context (Larkin, Watts, & Clifton., 2006, 107). In this study, IPA was
utilized as the optimal way to examine and capture a complicated and challenging
phenomenon (Priyadarshini, Dubey, Kumar, & Jha., 2020, 184) and enabled the
researcher to unveil the individuals’ lived experiences (Dowling & Cooney, 2012, 14). Now,
there is still no phenomenological studies in the dimension of ASMR concerning
psychosocial wellbeing. The current study strives to gain insight into participants’ lived
experiences, and the lived experience is usually under the subconscious mental state. The
scarcity of the conceptual framework in the ASMR-related literature, especially in the
aspects of wellbeing, indicates the necessity of qualitative study. Of all the qualitative
methodologies, IPA is chosen because it is the best way to construe and deduce the lived
experiences of the participants (e.g., ASMR experiences). As has been shown in the
literature review, research into ASMR has mostly utilized quantitative studies but very few
qualitative studies. Thus, the present study explored the beliefs, lived experiences of
embodied and encultured, and feelings of ASMR about wellbeing and discovered more
meaningful experiences via qualitative phenomenological inquiries that made use of the e-
communication method. Besides, IPA was considered a suitable methodology to
implement in response to the research questions, with data collected directly from each of
several participants.
31
5.3 Theoretical Descriptions of IPA
32
Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis
Perceived
Reflective Thinking
Emotional
(In-depth interview questions) P ositive Emotion
Experience
E ngagement
RQ2: What kind of emotional effects from the ASMR experiences have on the psychosocial
wellbeing of students in Finland?
(D) Idiography:
The diagram (Figure 3) shows that the theoretical underpinnings of IPA have four stages:
Phenomenology (Back to experience), hermeneutics (From experience to meaning), holism
(Meaningful experience); and idiography. Adapted from: Smith et al., 2009 and Daher et al., 2017.
33
5.4 Participants
A total of 3 participants had a mean age of 26.3 years, ranging from Bachelor to master's
degree. Reflecting on the sampling area, the participants were Finnish. Each interviewee is
anonymous and labeled as A, C, and D. See Table 1 for the characteristics of individual
study participants and their ASMR usage profiles.
sensation sometimes arms Ears and arms The neck is the most
Both sides sensitive
Both ears are the More on the right
same on triggering side
the sensation Both ears are the
same on triggering
the sensation
34
As seen in Table 1, participant A is a Finnish male student and studying a master’s degree
in Finland. He got high scores in Agreeableness and Openness-to-Experience personality
in the BFI test. He usually consumes ASMR videos once a week and only listening through
the headphones before sleep. He mentioned the environment to be peaceful, quiet, and
ample space in order to get the maximum effects. He does not always get the tingling
sensation, but in case it comes, the sensation can last for several minutes. Typically, the
tingling sensation originates in head, shoulders, and sometimes arms. Participant C is a
Finnish female and has graduated with a bachelor’s degree in Finland. She got high
scores in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness of the BFI test. Her optimal environment
for engagement of ASMR is a quiet and dark place.
Moreover, the reason for ASMR consumption is mainly improving the quality of sleep. She
usually watches one to two ASMR videos per session and four to five times per month.
She always got the tingling sensation, but the period is not sure. Also, she describes the
tingling sensation starting from the right side of the head and then spread both sides of
ears and arms. Participant D is a Finnish male student and still studying a master’s degree
in Finland. His BFI test score is mainly focused on Agreeableness and Openness-to-
Experience. He needs a quiet and darkroom in his ASMR engagement and consumes
videos three times a day and three to five ASMR videos per session. He is a pop music
performer and usually gets the tingling sensations along with ASMR videos. The tingling
sensation can last for ten seconds and usually occurred on the right side, and the neck is
the most intense region.
The samples are selected purposively rather than the probability method because they can
offer a research project insight into a particular experience. (Smith et al., 2009, 48). The
sample sizes should be small because IPA is an idiographic approach and tries to
understand particular phenomena in particular contexts. Moreover, each case can be
conducted in a detailed analysis to develop three separate case studies, and this allows
for a micro-analysis of similarities and differences across cases. (Smith et al., 2009, 52).
Moreover, a homogeneous sample should be used because the research question will
become more meaningful (Smith et al., 2009, 49). The inclusion criteria in this research
were that the participants with significant experience in ASMR (Barratt, Spence & Davis.,
2017, 4). On the one hand, exclusion criteria were that individuals reported without any
35
tingling sensation, in response to music, or without any external stimulation before or after
watching selected ASMR videos were excluded from the study. (Lochte et al., 2018, 296).
Digital flyer (Appendix E) was posted on the Facebook page for Media education of the
University of Lapland, and four participants with ASMR experience were recruited by
purposive sampling in the current study. They were either known to the researcher or
recruited through snowball sampling. Interested potential participants then contacted the
researcher through phone or email, and their questions were clarified. One of the potential
participants withdrew from the study of their own accord so that ultimately the study got
three participants. Potential participants were sent an email giving brief details of the
study. Attached to the email was a consent form should they wish to participate.
Participants then were briefed, and informed consent obtained, predicated on using
pseudonyms to protect privacy. The design of informed consent followed the ethical
principles of research with human participants guided by Finnish National Broad on
Research integrity (TENK 2019), and the University of Lapland commits to this guideline.
As such, Informed Consent (Appendix B) of the current study includes that participants are
well informed about the research, the potential benefits, and risks of their participation. The
individual has the right to confidentiality and the right to withdraw from the study at any
time without any consequences (Kohonen, Kuula-Luumi, & Spoof., 2019, 51-52).
Self-selected and standardized stimuli approach (Poerio et al., 2018, 8) were utilized for
ASMR video selection because this approach can maximize the chances of triggering
ASMR. Hence, participants were asked for self-selecting ASMR video clips and also
suggested to them the inclusion video clips from previous research that produced the most
reliable ASMR response (Poerio et al., 2018, 9). Inclusion: The content of the selected
ASMR videos included the interaction of ASMRtist with an object, the viewer, or another
actor. Moreover, the video contained the characteristic ASMR triggers such as whispering,
crinkle sounds, tapping, caring inclinations, task manifestation, personal attention, ear-to-
ear vocals, hair touching, and role-playing (Lochte et al., 2018, 296). Exclusion: Videos
with music, atypical ASMR triggers, or unusual (e.g., black and white film, video "cuts," still
picture with audio) visual elements (Lochte et al., 2018, 296).
36
The interviewees were given informed consent before the interview and then completed
the demographic questionnaire with Big Five Personality Test (BFI; John et al., 1991;
Appendix A). Semi-structured interviews were conducted for four months (from December
2019 to March 2020). The interviews were mainly carried out via email with Instant
Messenger and Skype. Participates over the Internet might be willing to disclose more
intimate details about the research topic than they would share in face-to-face interviews.
Email interviews also provide the participants time to reflect on their answers to the
questions without any pressure (Beck, 2005, 412). The interviews began with a few
structured questions regarding demographics, then asked semi-structured questions about
interviewees’ feelings and experiences concerning their day-to-day activities and behavior.
The open-ended questions of the interviews allowed participants to develop their narrative
by exploring their experiences of ASMR. The participants were sent reminders one week
before the deadline in case of no reply. Follow-up questions were sent to them after each
email transcript returned, especially when clarifications, explanations, or elaborations are
needed. The interview protocol was revised and refined at different stages of the research
process. Therefore, questions were modified, recorded, added, or eliminated as
subsequent interviews were completed. The interview schedule and its procedures are
shown in the following section.
Two primary email interviews will be arranged and spaced approximately 20-30 days apart
(See Table 2). In between, the interviewees were asked for further discussion details
through Instant Messenger (e.g., WhatsApp and Skype). The design and content of the
interview protocol were based on the previous ASMR literature review and relevant
wellbeing questions in the articles (Kern et al., 2015).
2019 2020
Dec Jan Feb Mar
Interview One
Interview Two
37
Referring to Table 2, interview One (Appendix C) is focused on ASMR’s lived history
experience and asked, “How did you experience ASMR from the beginning up to now?”.
Interview Two (Appendix D:, including the detailed instructions) is focused on the details of
ASMR experience and the meaning related to wellbeing. The interview involved an in-
depth discussion of the details of the participant’s experience in Psychosocial Wellbeing
after ASMR exposures and take part in school function activities, such as group projects,
assignments, general examination, winter sports activities, Christmas party organization,
etc. The Interview One was mainly corresponded to Research Question One and its
objectives to explore the predictors and mediators (moderators) of ASMR. In contrast,
interview two was corresponded to the Research Question Two to explore how ASMR
media responses (e.g., positive feelings and thoughts) enhanced the meaning-making of
psychosocial wellbeing and lived experiences for university students in Finland. Besides,
ASMR participants were asked to abstain from watching ASMR videos for the three days
before the study to elevate the chance of eliciting ASMR. (Poerio et al., 2018,10).
The data collected do not require transcription and are thus already in written format and
readily available for analysis. Themes and subthemes were developed by following
analysis of the three transcripts; 22 pages and 529 lines. The supporting extracts are
provided, and all names have been changed to the anonymity of participants, and the
format of extract quotation is (Participant name: Page number, Line number), such as (A.
P.1, line: 101–111). Data analysis followed standard interpretative phenomenological
methods (Smith, Flowers, & Larkin., 2009, 72–93) involving several readings and re-
readings the initial transcript to search significant quotes and interest for ASMR-related
responses and PERMA five domains. Then, concise phrases representing units of
meaning were coded and repeating for the other transcripts to identify emergent themes
from PERMA to get the ideas of media effects. Then, each coded transcript is made
conceptual connections between subthemes and condensed into clusters. Each cluster of
themes was given a descriptive subtheme label shown in Figures 4 and 5.
38
Topic Area Emotional Reaction Immersion of ASMR Description of Relationship Experience Personal Meaning of ASMR experience
experience
Mindfulness
Emergent Subthemes
Figure. 4. Flow process of the emergent subthemes and subsequent application to the themes of the PERMA model, identified
through IPA. Adapted & modified from Yerbury, R. M., & Boyd, W. E. (2019).
39
All transcripts were first individually analyzed before the identified themes were considered
together as a whole to form a group analysis, and organized into interconnected
hierarchies (i.e., themes, subthemes, and categories). In Figure 4, the subthemes are
emerged from the narratives of the corresponding interview questions and giving rise to
four topic areas, emotional reaction, the immersion of ASMR experience, description of
relationships experience, and personal meaning of the experience. The subthemes are
subsequently applied to the similar themes of the PERMA model. Moreover, the flow
process and information of topic areas and the emergent subthemes expressed as a mind
map in Figure 5.
40
Figure. 5. Mind map of interview narrative subthemes and themes of the PERMA model concerning ASMR experience.
41
As seen in Figure 5, the interview narrative themes and subthemes of ASMR experience
and wellbeing are organized and corresponding to the research question two. The
remaining transcripts are coded by using the master list of themes that was regarded as a
guideline for subsequent transcripts. Finally, a summary of the interviews by incorporating
cross-case analysis among that themes extracted from the data (Yerbury & Boyd, 2019,
203; Priyadarshini, Dubey, Kumar, & Jha., 2020, 185; Chappell, Eatough, Davies, &
Griffiths., 2006, 207; Ilias, Liaw, Cornish, Park, & Golden., 2017, 78). The relevant
components of the IPA and related stages are outlined in Table 3.
Reading & Regarding “immersion” into the data, several readings are accompanied
rereading by a feeling of being overwhelmed by ideas and possible connections
(Smith, Flowers et al. 2009, 82).
Initial noting Exploratory comments include (1) descriptive comments, (2) linguistic
comments, and (3) conceptual comments. These comments are also
useful in the subsequent step of identifying themes.
Developing Themes are likely to be identified as much from the researcher’s
emergent exploratory comments as the transcript itself, and thus emergent themes
themes might emerge as a natural next step from prior notes.
Connecting Abstraction (i.e., identifying patterns and super-ordinate theme),
emergent polarization (i.e., looking for differences rather than similarities),
themes contextualization, and function attempt to connect themes for write-up
summaries.
Moving to the Each case is taken as an individual unit before broader comparisons,
next case and a cross-case review is conducted. Therefore, care should be made
to bracket any assumptions or crossover knowledge from one interview,
when analyzing the next.
Patterns In the current study, the patterns across cases are represented
across cases graphically and through table structures, pulling cross-case themes into
focus while referring to the supporting cases for each.
Note: Adapted and summarized by Smith et al., 2009.
As seen in Table 3, the detailed examination of one transcript recollects the ideas of the
interview. Then, the meaning of relevant topics is tried to capture and develop the
emergent themes through the exploratory commenting. The emergent themes were
connected by identifying common links between them. The remaining transcripts were
then analyzed by using previous stages, and each case was moving in its right direction,
and finally, the main and subthemes were relabelled and drawn together, and this resulted
in several main themes for the group each with some related subthemes.
42
5.8 Coding Strategy
Yardley’s (2000) principles were chosen to guide the quality of the current study because
Smith et al. (2009, 153–157) particularly recommended this guideline that has four
principles: sensitivity to context, commitment and rigor, transparency, and coherence; and
impact and importance.
Sensitivity to context relates to the degree of awareness of the relevant literature and
previous empirical work related to lived experience (Yardley, 2000, 220). Hence, the
researcher familiarised with the literature review on ASMR and wellbeing and immersing in
the literature relating to the theoretical underpinnings of IPA. Moreover, sensitivity also
related to the socio-cultural context of the sample, ethics, and data generation (Yardley,
2000, 220-221). Thus, numerous verbatim extracts and quotations are required to meet
this quality standard and give participants a voice in this ASMR research and let the reader
check the interpretations (Smith et al., 2009,154). Rigour and Commitment refer to
43
thorough data collection and the depth and breadth of analysis (Yardley, 2000, 222). In
current research would be fulfilled the requirements: the homogenous and purposive
sampling (i.e., Finnish university students with significant ASMR experience);
appropriateness of the interview schedule and conducted a thorough and systematic data
analysis while maintaining IPA’s idiographic commitment. Transparency and coherence
relate to the clarity of write-up and presentation (Yardley, 2000, 222). The methodology
chapter lets the reader see the coherence and an appropriate fit between the research
question and the methodology selected according to the objectives and conceptual
framework of research. Transparency would be heightened by describing the details of
participant selection, the formation of the interview schedule, interview process, and
analysis steps of the IPA and coding strategy shown in the Appendix and this chapter.
Impact and Importance are about both theoretical and practical impact when applied to
practical issues by other researchers (Yardley, 2000, 223). As such, the current research
will be expected other researchers to reflect on possible ASMR and Wellbeing implications
for their practice and inspire them to continue with investigations in this area.
6 Findings
The purposes of this chapter are provided a phenomenological and interpretative narrative
of the research findings. The findings included themes in two main overarching areas
regarding research questions identified through IPA data analysis and coding strategy.
Each theme has several related sub-themes shown in Tables 4 and 5, and then further
interpreted after the presentations of tables. Each subtheme will be discussed in turn of
each theme, with a thorough account of how themes are relevant within individual
participant transcripts. Transcript extracts and quotations will be included proportionally
across participants so that individual voices can be heard, and individual experiences can
be illuminated.
44
6.1 Students’ ASMR experiences
Participants presented a picture of the ASMR effects on their lives. The identified themes
and subthemes included ASMR intentional use and corresponding to subthemes prior to
ASMR-like experiences, social pressure from family members, and curiosity. The second
main theme, called ASMR multisensory integration, including nostalgic thoughts, pro-
change bias, positive activating, and deactivating affects. The final main theme is called
ASMR media use, and corresponding subthemes included different types of sound and
movements. The themes and subthemes are shown in Table 4.
Table 4. Themes and Subthemes for ASMR Intentional use and responses
Theme Sub-Theme
ASMR Intentional use Prior ASMR-like experience
Social Pressure from family members
Curiosity
The subthemes of Table 4 will be interpreted and analyzed in the following section.
45
ASMR Intentional Use
Previous ASMR-like experience was explicitly related to later use of new ASMR media
sources, and the following typical quotes showed their unique past experiences of ASMR
sensation:
Extract 1: “My mother used to read most of the Harry Potter books
to my brother and me when we were little, so I remember listening
to her pleasant voice for hours on end. Nevertheless, I have not
thought about ASMR experiences before engaging with the videos
(C: P.2, line: 55-57).”
Extract 2: “I’ve felt ASMR experiences as a child in elementary
school. The previous experiences came from someone’s hand
movements, teachers writing, paper noise, teacher, is licking the
finger while turning the page of a book. I remember feeling ASMR
when we were doing a group task in 3rd grade. One of the groups
was doing something with her hands (drawing or something), and I
was just starting the process. In a way, I linked in her activity (D: P.6,
line: 163-169).”
These quotes show that both participants’ past experiences (related to both family
members and the social activities) store the ASMR-like memories and strengthen their
later ASMR videos engagement. Besides, people who were previously familiar with the
use of old brand products would be more likely to engage with the products as adults
(Bonus et al., 2018, 6). One participant described what he felt as a young boy watching
ASMR-like TV show:
Extract 3: “As a kid, I use to relax with one of the Finnish comedic
show Kummeli’s sketches, “Virsikirjan lisälehdillä.” The main
characters speak softly and whisper a lot, which made me relaxed.
When I got to know to ASMR, I’ve been hooked (D: P.1, line: 10-
14).”
46
The participant watched ASMR-like TV show in the childhood stage, and then he got this
pre-ASMR experience to develop ASMR YouTube video engagement later. Moreover, the
DSMM proposed predictors of media use not just as motivations, but also as moderators
of the relationship between media use and responses (Bonus et al., 2018, 8). For
participants C & D, individuals with previous ASMR experience and prior exposure to the
ASMR-like videos might not only be more likely to engage ASMR videos later, but they
may also be more likely to experience nostalgic thoughts.
The DSMM suggests that social factors, like preferences of friends and family members,
impact the types of media people choose, either because these social forces actively
enable or restrict access to media, or because they encourage exposure to certain types
of media (Valkenburg & Peter, 2013b, 227). Participant A was influenced by his sister's
experience about relieving her sleeping problem of using ASMR video, and finally, he
engaged ASMR videos:
Extract 4: “About half a year ago my sister told that she uses ASMR
videos to relax and fall asleep, and she recommended ASMR for
me, so I decided to try the videos for insomnia (A: P.1, line 7, 10)”
Curiosity
Curiosity is operationalized as the perceived ability and desire to close an information gap
in one’s knowledge. Generally speaking, it is interrelated with other human
connectedness, such as social interaction and general experience-seeking
(Schaekermann et al., 2017, 144). In terms of curiosity, one participant reported:
This quote shows how the news media (one article) arouse her interest and novelty-
seeking sensation and later decided to engage ASMR videos.
47
ASMR Multisensory Integration
Nostalgic thoughts are represented by memories of highly social activities that are
preserved in person and often associated with an exaggerated experience (Bonus et al.,
2018, 7). For instance:
These quotes highlighted that ASMR video recalls the childhood-teacher relationships and
linked the past affect and feelings with present events through ASMR experiences;
whereas, some other ASMR video can trigger him to recreate and understand the self-
identity (student identity).
For some participants, they claimed that they have pre-conception or bias to ASMR, such
as seeing it as a joke and thought it as noises and showed in the following quotes:
Extract 8: “When I first got to know the videos about two years ago,
I was amused, and I thought that it was a joke (A: P.1, line 15-16).”
Extract 9: “First, I thought it was just irritating noises of banging and
stroking and looking at people making those noises. How boring!
However, as I realized that ASMR has such a wide variety of videos,
I got hooked. I was under the assumption that ASMR was only about
making noises with objects. When I came across a video from a
Finnish vlogger who whispered instead of just making noises with
objects, I was hooked as I noticed the tingles (C: P.1, line: 3-4; 19-
21).”
48
To protect themselves from the risk of change, the participants’ bias motivates them to
select evidence based on the status quo or reject evidence supporting change. However,
they reset their rigidity of mind after they engaged in ASMR, and they noticed they got the
tingling sensations from ASMR triggers.
Emotional & Excitative response state: Excited, Happy, Content, Relax & Calm
Participants reported that ASMR videos made them excited, happy, relaxed, calm, and
contented as activating and deactivating positive affect, respectively. For instance:
Tingling Sensation
Many of the accounts claimed that they got the tingling sensation after watching ASMR
videos. In the following extract, some participants describe the feelings of tingling
sensation in details:
49
The quotes above show that the tingling sensation can provide mixed physiological
responses: arousal, relaxed, and calm. Besides, tingling sensation seems to be through
the channels and vessels to connect with his body and mind. For the other participant,
tingling sensation also made her relax, and she tends to seek this sensation but not
addiction.
Sound: Whispering, Tapping sound, Non-spoken sound, & Soft and kindness words
Extract 18: “At least whisper as well as soft and relaxing talk (A: P.1,
line 14).”
Extract 19: “I always ended up listening to only a small selection of
people and videos who were all women whispering in Finnish, who
made a minimal amount of noises with objects (C: P.1, line: 7-10).”
Extract 20: “Listening to no-talk ASMR videos that had consistent
sounds helped me to lose track. (C: P.4, line: 120-121).”
Extract 21: “The affirmations and kind words ASMR videos (C: P.5,
line: 153).”
Nature landscape
50
Movement: Repetitive hand movements & Slow movement
Five main themes emerged from the analyses: Positive Emotion, Engagement,
Relationships, Meaning and Making Connection, and Accomplishment. Both individual and
collective themes are discussed, including overlapping (shown in Table 5 & Figure 6).
Table 5. Themes and Sub-themes for ASMR media effects with wellbeing
Theme Sub-Theme
Positive Emotion Relaxation
Calmness
Excitement
Joy
Engagement Mindfulness
Self-awareness
Telepresence
Stop rumination
Absorption
Flow
Relationship (Connectedness) Self-in-relationship & love
Prosocial behavior, Empathy
Digital intimacy
Meaning and Making Connection Being a part of something bigger
A sense of curiosity: Finding a meaningful life
Accomplishment Self-identity & Innovativeness
Memory & decision-making
Self-confidence & goal achievement
51
The subthemes of Table 5 will be interpreted and analyzed in the following section.
The first subtheme related to the PERMA framework is positive emotions with various
range of emotions described: relaxation, calmness, excitement, and joy. Seligman (2011,
p. 25) posited that positive emotion is a subjective term that defined by what you think and
feel. Feelings of relaxation and calmness (B3; E1) when engaging with ASMR were the
most common. Participant C tends to seek both relaxed and calm (B2; R2) feelings and to
relieve the “worry” & “busy” mind and showed in the following extracts:
For participant D, ASMR video makes him relaxed and calm (B2; R2) feelings so that his
“headspace” can be expanded and open the door to relieve his stress and busy mind and
then induce sleeping (B3). Meaning was also highly linked with Emotion. He commented,
for example, that
Extract 28: “I try to get more ‘headspace’ in my brain with these via
calming, soothing, and relaxing sensations. I have a busy schedule
with three active bands, school, work, ‘Speksi,’ and in organizing live
music events. Maybe this is my gateway to sleep and turning off
stress. So, I try to keep my wellbeing holistic and meaningful in many
ways (D: P.8, line: 216-219).”
On the other hand, the repetitive natural sound (horse walking) (B1) gives the feeling of
calm (B2) and then induce the sleeping (B3) in the following quotes:
52
Extract 29: “Before sleeping, I have listened to horse walking ASMR.
I think that it is very calming because there is a smooth and stable
rhythm in the horse's walk. At times I have also watched the ASMR
horse walking video because it has a beautiful landscape (A: P.2,
line 56-58).”
In summary, ASMR videos released the body and mind tension through relaxation, leading
to a psychophysiological state of decreased arousal that opposes that of the stress
response and is experienced as a calming state and improved quality of sleep.
The slow tapping finger movement (B1) triggers his happy feeling (B2; R2) and enhance
his joy (B3; E1) emotion and reading interest.
Extract 31: “In ASMR video, she is calmly pottering about the books
and so on. She is introducing books that are interesting to me, and
she even reads some of them out loud. That brings me joy, knowing
that there are good books to read, and good perceptions to dive in
to (D: P.8, line: 213-215).”
53
The engagement was linked with Emotion: ASMR video also makes him more self-
awareness (E2) his positive feelings (E1) and again “headspace” is just like a bridge to link
between Emotion and engagement. The repetitive hand movement (B1) can trigger him
more joy and excitement of feelings (B2; R2), even in both ASMR video and real music
performance.
In the current study, engagement was manifested in both subthemes, (a) as mindfulness
meditation: self-awareness, telepresence and stop rumination, and (b) as full immersion in
the ASMR videos and activities.
54
Mindfulness: Self-awareness, Telepresence, Stop Rumination
Extract 33: “ASMR video can help me to focus on breath deeper and
feel a presence. When I am listening to ASMR Horse walking, at
best, I feel how my heart rate drops, and I feel a strong presence. At
that moment, I forget all the stress and worries (A: P.2, line 33-34,
58-60).”
Participant D also reported that ASMR triggered the tingling sensation and “headspace” to
facilitate and empower the insight and self-awareness that enabled him to create a pause
that allowed them to stop his repetitive spiral rumination.
Engagement overlapped with Relationships. ASMR video makes him more aware of the
present-moment experience (E2) and enhances his love with a girlfriend (E3).
55
Extract 36: “When I watch the ASMR video with my girlfriend next to
me, I’m watching the video and get the body feels more present. (D:
P.8, line: 232-233).”
Extract 37: “When I feel excited and interested in things, for example,
my schoolwork, I feel very focused on what I do, and I have a flow
mode. If I am restless before my schoolwork, listening ASMR videos
might help me to relax and concentrate better to do my schoolwork
(A: P.3, line 64-66).”
Participant C listened to non-talking sound ASMR videos during the noisy working
environment. It helped her to absorbed in the task and experienced time loss and worked
longer than intended.
Extract 38: “I listened to long trigger sound videos while working, and
those helped me to concentrate. I was much more restless while
working when I was not listening to ASMR videos. I felt better about
myself after having been able to concentrate on my work better. My
work is quite susceptible to being ‘interrupted’ by something all the
time because I do communication work. Listening to no-talk ASMR
videos that had consistent sounds helped me to lose track of time.
The videos were putting me in flow when I had to do some longer
writing work or thinking work (C: P.4, line: 104-107, 119-122).”
56
Participant D felt that time went by quickly, and he mentioned that ASMR engagement
time is different from real-world time and can lead to not noticing real-world time and
experienced time loss. He felt just like the connection of mind, body, and sound music
altogether.
Extract 39: “This happens in music and playing it, especially when
I’m doing a demo of some new song or inspirational piece of music.
I feel a connection with the music. It feels in my body and fills my
mind. In a way, the mind and body become one. This sometimes
happens during ASMR videos. So, in a way, the video I’ve been
watching has done its work (D: P.4, line: 107-112).”
The above in-depth immersion reflected a state of flow that Seligman (2011) regarded as
one domain of wellbeing in the PERMA framework; Indeed, in describing flow,
Csikszentmihalyi (1990) posits that this state of mind/body necessitates an optimal
balance between challenge and skill, results in happiness (Mirehie, & Gibson, 2019, 9).
The third sub-theme associated with PERMA is relationships that are emerged when
participants mentioned extensively about the importance of ASMR’s companions in
shaping their digital-intimacy experiences. Numerous statements described the presence
of others in the interaction of ASMR videos as generating a sense of prosocial, self-
compassion, and digital intimacy relationships.
Participant C can be more sensitive to the love of others after watching the ASMR
affirmation and kindness videos. The soft and kind words of ASMR videos change her
prior self-negative thought and enhance her self-compassion and accept that everybody
has challenging life experiences and makes mistakes (common humanity) and take
actions with kindness and understanding for herself (self-kindness).
57
Extract 40: “I feel being loved by my friends and family very often. I
still feel like the person talking is someone who does not know me.
On the other hand, the affirmations and kind words do make me think
about positive and gentle thoughts about myself, and they help me
not be so hard on myself (C: P.5, line: 141-144).”
ASMR videos with slow painting movement enhance his ability to experience and
communicate to feel a sense of others’ (friends and his girlfriend) inner experiences; be
more present to others’ feelings; empower other’s their body sensations and feelings.
58
Extract 43: “When I watch the ASMR video with my girlfriend next to
me, I’m watching the video and get the body feels more present. I’ve
sometimes been watching this Ross the painter ASMR video with
some friends at the same time. In a way, sometimes, when I am
sharing that sensation about ASMR more powerful (D: P.8, line: 232-
236).”
The specific discussion topics of ASMR videos enhance him to have present-focused
awareness and get more ideas to understand others' perspectives and notice how to help
them. Besides, these types of ASMR videos also increase his sensitivity to getting help
from others (by changing his thoughts) in the school.
Digital intimacy
The slowing and repetitive movement ASMR videos elevate his sensitivity to being loved
and also giving love to others.
Extract 46: “ASMR videos are usually very slow, and there isn’t much
happening. So, in a way, I find the sensation of loving in those little
things. I think love is the clue of our existence toward others, and it’s
in life’s every thingness, and it is seen in short forms (D: P.5, line:
138-139, 141-142).”
These types of ASMR also induce his awareness to make a connection with the others
and the world. This aspect was further exemplified by the relationships that participant D
formed while engaging in (ASMR) virtual worlds.
59
Extract 47: “After the ASMR video, I feel like I’m more connected to
the person and engaging after watching ASMR videos. Some of the
very simple searching’s like slow movements or some kind of visual
triggers ASMR videos gives me more headspace, so they make me
more aware of the world, of the room in the situation of the
surrounding and make me more aware of the world, so they make
me in a way more aware of the persons I have relations with
roommate or girlfriend (D: P.8, line: 221-227).”
The intimacy enhanced through the ASMR videos was affected in real life, revealing the
way these virtual stimulating triggers with simple and slow movements created new forms
of closeness (loves and the world) to extend and continue.
The fourth sub-theme associated with PERMA, meaning, was mainly depicted in the
narratives of the Participant D. Almost all interviewees referred to Meaning, with some
variation: ‘‘Meaningful? I don’t know how to answer that question with ASMR”. The
participant showed a more in-depth understanding and stronger emotional attachment,
engagement, and relationship to ASMR videos and to his both environment and activities,
all of which created meaning for them. Seligman (2011) suggests that a central part of the
meaning domain is a feeling that you are doing something meaningful and part of
something bigger (Mirehie & Gibson, 2019, 11). Meaning and Making the connection of
experiences was also the central theme. Subthemes included being a part of something
bigger and personal curiosity relevance of the experience.
60
Being a part of something bigger
Participant D emphasized ASMR YouTube videos to act as a platform to let him connect
with others and the world meaningfully. The platform can also make the networks link to
understanding the others. All these connections can give him a sense that he is part of
something bigger than himself. Meaning is essential not just to him, friends and family, but
rather to a much larger group, such as the whole world human race. Also, the slow and
repetitive hand movements in the videos can trigger him to connect the ASMR platform
and others.
Participant D’s narrative shows the links between the three themes of Meaning,
Relationships, and Emotion as he describes the encounter: He has the unwillingness
subjective experience, such as shyness feelings, taking action to avoid and escape to
make a friendship with others in his childhood development. However, the ASMR-like
experiences promote him exposure to previously avoided experiences and giving him
good feelings (B2, R2; E1) by focusing full attention (E2) and curiosity to explore others’
inner meaningful thinking and also help himself connect with others and more understand
the meaning of his own life. Later, he actively engaged ASMR videos instead of ASMR-like
real experiences to extend his curiosity and exploration of other meaningful life. Again, the
repetitive mundane movements are the primary trigger of this effect.
61
Extract 50: “As a child, I was very shy and didn’t meet new people
very much. However, later years, I’ve learned to get to know people
in a meaningful way. I see humans fundamentally a social animal,
so I try to explore that. I think I have a great social life, on which I’m
truly grateful. I’ve heard people saying that I have a large amount of
social capital. I've always wanted to know people's true meanings in
life, asking about what they want and try to connect to every people
in a meaningful and deep way. I would like to see the doctor, the
librarian, and the people in their specific works, which give me ASMR
experiences. The offices, sound of paper, typing, and intense focus
gives me shivers and good feelings. I don’t have that much in my
life, so I’m trying to compensate it with the ASMR videos. (D: P.5,
line: 122-135).”
As noted above, this theme was more prominent in the experiences of high-level ASMR
participants. For him, ASMR videos and experiences become a central part of life, and
associated activities provide them with meaning, which seems to be consistent with
Seligman’s (2011) conception of the meaning domain.
Accomplishment
The fifth and final subtheme related to PERMA, accomplishment, emerged through the
participant’s descriptions about his sense of achievement to ASMR. Narratives about
enhancing self-identity, memory & decision-making, and self-confidence were associated
with a sense of achievement. The participant mentioned the term of headspace that is
mainly involved in relation to both memory processing and self-identity.
The accomplishment was linked with Relationship and Meaning. ASMR videos make him
recall the memory of the student identity through the headspace and time. The student
identity can be implicated to recall further and give more ideas about the coping skills to
deal with different tasks in the university, such as examinations, playing music, and other
social activities (E4). In creating his narrative of student identity, he and objects are tightly
62
interconnected (E3). Everything is instrumental in setting up the world and moved memory
into his mind and finally are implicated in creating future ideas.
Extract 51: “I feel that some videos help me creating student identity
by giving ideas or just giving me time to think about something else.
ASMR helps me aware of the daily tasks in school or giving me more
ideas about social life. I think the answer lies again in the solid
student identity. But also, I need to have a feeling of great social life
so that I feel appreciated and loved. Everything is connected (D: P.6,
line: 152-158).”
Extract 52: “When I’m watching the ASMR videos, I remember how
to make music meaningful and how to make the world meaningful,
sometimes the video reminds me. Sometimes they remind me of
more awareness. (D: P.9, line: 246-248).”
ASMR videos content enhance him remember something and create student identity and a
psychological link between past himself experience and current himself memory. Finally,
the connection between past and current experiences can further extend the new ideas
into his future meaningful life, music, and the world. He mentioned that ASMR media
effects are the same as mindfulness meditation.
63
Memory & decision-making
The accomplishment was connected with Meaning, Engagement, and Positive Emotion.
ASMR video makes the informant decrease the physiological arousal (i.e., calm and relax)
(B2, R2) and more focus attention (E2) to headspace. To take Participant D as an
example:
Extract 55: “ASMR something calming you down and relaxing and
more headspace again. It helps me to concentrate on my headspace
a nd mo re ro om to m emor ize . ( D: P.10 , lin e: 26 2 -2 63) . ”
Slow hand movement ASMR video triggers the informant's specific memory to remember
how to play the music meaningfully (E4).
Extract 56: “The one about the ASMR card magic tricks, but the way
he does it reminds me of the question in music on how to compose
and how to play music and how to play and remind me about the
question about how to play, not what to play. So, you don’t have to
have a fast-playing all real technical songs you have to have simple
songs which you play meaningfully. (D: P.9, line: 241-245).”
ASMR video also stops his mind wandering and more focus on the present-moment
situation and triggers the informant’s headspace to recall past experiences and help him
decision-making and problem-solving.
64
Self-confidence & Goal Achievement
The repetitive painting movements ASMR video give the informant’s positive affect and
stop rumination and finally elevate self-confidence to re-organize the ideas in connection
with the world and keep the right track and pathway towards the goals (See, for example,
participant D).
The fact that ASMR enhances the Emotion and engagement for him to recall the learning
skills and dare to take part in the activities and generates a sense of accomplishment that
seems to contribute to their overall sense of wellbeing.
65
Different facets of PERMA were combined in the participants’ accounts. Specifically, this
was the case for accomplishment and positive emotion, engagement, positive emotion,
relationships and accomplishment, relationships and positive emotion, accomplishment,
engagement, meaning, and positive emotion. The interconnections between five PERMA
themes are shown in Figure 6.
Figure 6. The relative strength of the five PERMA themes and interconnections between
themes. This diagram indicates the relative strength of both the individual themes and their
associations (number of arrows represented), based on the numbers of references in interview
transcripts.
Figure 6 showed that the relative individual strength of the five themes of PERMA is
ordered according to the number of arrows as follows: 1. Accomplishment, 2. Emotion and
Engagement, 3. Meaning; and 4. Relationships. All these themes with their multiple
subthemes will be discussed in detail in the following chapter. The chapter will discuss
these themes about existing research literature and will address the implications of the
findings of this study.
66
7 Summary of Findings & Discussion
Through the participants’ recollections and expressions of ASMR lived experiences, this
study gives insight into the phenomenon of ASMR engagement and its effect on multiple
dimensions of human well-being. This is the first study to unite the DSMM with the PERMA
framework (Seligman, 2011) and the first to explore and link the distinguishing
characteristics of ASMR video with well-being outcomes. Hence, this study proposed the
elaboration of the ASMR media effects model shown in Figure 7 to represent the ASMR
experiences with psychosocial wellbeing. The current findings point to which conclusions
and several directions for future research.
Positive Emotion
Relaxation
Calmness
Dispositional Cognitive Excited
Sound Joy
Nostalgic
Thoughts Engagement
Whispering
Curiosity Soft and Pro-change Mindfulness meditation
Prior ASMR- kindness bias 1. Self-awareness
words
like 2. Telepresence
experiences 3. Stop rumination
Nature Absorption
Openness- Emotional &
landscape Flow
to- Excitative
Experience Relationship
Roleplay Positive Activating
Affect Self-in-Relationship & love
Painter Prosocial behavior &
Librarian Excited Empathy
Teacher Happy Digital intimacy
Positive Deactivating Meaning
Movement Affect
Social Being a part of something
Content
Repetitive bigger
Social: hand Relax
A sense of curiosity
movements Calm
Pressure Slow
Tingling Sensation Accomplishment
from family movement
members Self-identity &
Innovativeness
Memory & decision-making
Self-confidence & goal
achievement
67
In figure 7, the proposed model is composed of four building blocks. The first one
represents the predictors of ASMR media use, including dispositional and social
subthemes. The subthemes include prior ASMR-like experiences, social pressure from
family members, and curiosity. The second building block represents the subthemes of
media use and contents of ASMR, including sound, nature landscape, roleplay, and
different types of movement. The third one represents the body sensation and reactions
during or just after media exposure. The subthemes include cognitive (e.g., nostalgic
thoughts and pro-change bias), emotional and excitative (e.g., tingling sensation) states.
The last block represents the media effects based on the five themes of PERMA, and
multiple subthemes emerged shown in figure 7. In this study, participants expressed their
personal stories through unique ASMR lived experiences and interpretations with their
common points and suggested that engagements with ASMR can have a significant effect
on human well-being. The details of each theme and subthemes’ meanings are discussed
in the following sections.
The findings of the current study show that the prior ASMR-like experiences and
Openness-to-Experience personality traits were identified as media and non–media use
predictors and moderators to predict and moderate ASMR media use or as its influencing
effects. All participants have completed the BFI test before the first interview, and the
result found that two-thirds of participants scored higher on Openness-to Experience (See
Table 1). The findings are consistent with prior research that ASMR experiencers have
higher scores on the Openness to Experience and lower scores on Conscientiousness
subscales of BFI (Fredborg & Smith, 2017, 1). Openness to Experience refers to an
individual’s curiosity and preference for novel and stimulating experiences (McErlean &
Banissy, 2017, 9). However, one participant has lower scores in Openness-to-Experience
but a high curiosity tendency to the use of ASMR videos (Extract 5). It may be the
inconsistency between the previous studies and the findings to reflect differences in
sampling sizes and methodology. Additionally, participants who had experienced previous
ASMR-like events (Extracts 1 & 2) were more prone to ASMR media use later. Also, the
similar situations of affecting ASMR media use was found to be related to participants who
consumed previous television coverage of ASMR-like programs (Extract 3). These findings
linked with the previous self-report experiencing ASMR since childhood (Poerio et al.,
2018, 2). Future research examining media effects related to ASMR media predictors
68
should further explore how these previous ASMR-like experiences moderate the ASMR
media effects, such as nostalgic thoughts. Social aspects of ASMR media effects are also
explored and identified in the current study. The findings indicate that the preferences of
family members (Extract 4) could influence the ASMR media use. This result is aligned
with previous research about a familial aspect of the ability to ASMR experience (Barratt &
Davis, 2015, 10).
The current findings (Extracts 18-24) in terms of the types of ASMR media contents and
triggers are similar to both Barratt and Davis (2015) & McErlean and Banissy (2017)
studies. They identified whispering, attention, crisp sounds, finger tapping, hair brushing,
various role-playing scenarios, and slow movements as common triggers (Barratt & Davis,
2015, 6; McErlean & Banissy, 2017, 10). Also, the current results show that the reasons
for watching ASMR videos described by participants (See Table 4) were similar to those
found in the Barratt and Davis (2015) study. All participants mentioned that ASMR videos
made them relaxed, enhanced sleeping, and reduced stress. Moreover, some of them
used ASMR videos while working, doing the assignment, and playing music. These results
directly concur with previous results and indicate that ASMR videos are usually utilized for
helping sleeping, relaxation, and for easing stress (Barratt & Davis, 2015, 11; McErlean &
Banissy, 2017, 10).
Regarding media response states, there are several responses are expressed by
participants, including the experience of ASMR regarded as pleasurable, the calming and
relaxation effects of the video, feeling comforted or cared-for of ASMR videos, expressions
of nostalgic thoughts and pro-change bias, and the complicated and mixed feelings of
tingling sensations. Participants mentioned that the tingling sensations were started from
the right side and can spread from the head (from upper back to forehead) to ears, neck,
and arms. These findings reinforce previous study results that the physiological response
of a tingling sensation most often originated at the back of the head and neck (Barratt &
Davis, 2015, 11). Moreover, the result also shows that the tingling sensation could be
acted as a central hub to regulate the physiological responses of arousal, relaxed, and
calm (Extracts: 16 & 17). It seems to be involved in a unique internal system connecting
the body and mind through the channels & vessels (Extract 15), and that special internal
system would be described as a regulatory role in the mixing console analogy in DSMM. In
terms of DSMM, these emotional reactions and mediators are also related to other media
effects, such as empathy (Extracts 43 & 44), confidence (Extracts 58 & 59), and
69
compassion (Extract 40). Besides, the media use of ASMR could act as a moderator to
change the cognitive bias thoughts of resisting ASMR usage (Extract 8 & 9) and recall the
nostalgic reverie (Extract 6).
In terms of both DSMM and Seligman’s first PERMA element, positive emotions are
appeared as joy & excitement (Extracts 30 & 31) and relaxation & calmness (Extracts 26-
29) through the activating (happy & excited) and deactivating (relaxed & calm) positive
affects (Extracts 10-14) of ASMR media response states by watching ASMR videos
(indirect media effects). These findings are consistent with the earlier work on ASMR self-
reports and physiological measurements. First, ASMR experiencers are reported that more
frequent tingling, increased levels of excitement and calmness, and decreased levels of
stress after watching a range of ASMR videos. (Poerio et al., 2018, 8). Second, ASMR is
explicitly associated with the reduced heart rate and increased skin conductance level in
ASMR participants while watching ASMR videos. (Poerio et al., 2018, 13). When taken
together, these studies provide cumulative support that ASMR engagement is mainly
associated with such activating and deactivating self-reported emotions and physiology
coincided. These responses are corresponding to the emotional complexity of the ASMR
phenomenon (Poerio et al., 2018, 14). Besides, the present findings suggested that the
spoken voices, slow and repetitive hand movements (Extracts 29, 30 & 31), maybe the
indicative of these complex emotional responses. These results are consistent with the
previous research that both spoken and sound-only ASMR videos triggered significantly
more excited, calmer, and less stressed (Poerio et al., 2018, 8).
Engagement, the second pillar of PERMA, emerged in the ASMR experiences, which were
in the form of mindfulness, full absorption, and flow-like mental state in the activity. The
present findings showed that the ASMR videos (e.g., non-spoken) caused time loss and
absorbed into their activities. For participants (Extracts 38 & 39), the experience of time
loss causes them to neglect the time flow of real-world tasks such as working in an
unfavorable environment and playing the music. This result aligns with Barratt & Davis’s
(2015) findings in that the passive component of flow was associated with the ASMR
experience and also concurred with the increased absorption when ASMR experiencers
engaged in music or when concentrating on a work-related task which are typical
examples of flow-inducing activities (McErlean & Osborne-Ford, 2020, 9). Moreover, the
70
interviews also yield data to find that ASMR experiencers used ASMR videos to alleviate
negative feelings by enhancing both self-regulation of attention and non-judgmental
awareness of the present moment. For participants (Extracts 33,34, & 35) state that the
ASMR videos made them more aware and accepting of their present- moment experience,
including their own inner emotions and body sensations, and distract their repetitive spiral
thoughts. The findings are consistent with previous results of increased mindfulness in
ASMR (Fredborg et al., 2018, 8). The repetitive sound (Extract 33) of ASMR videos is the
main trigger to enhance the reminiscent of mindfulness meditation, and the previous
literature suggested that the repetitive sounds and watching people touch things may
enhance the viewers to get more tingles and create the mental structures (mindfulness)
(Fredborg et al., 2018, 10). Combining the results as mentioned earlier, people absorbed
in ASMR videos may distract from psychological distress (e.g., ruminations), which is
similar to the other immersive means as effective pain reduction strategies (McErlean &
Osborne-Ford, 2020, 8).
Relationship was the third domain in PERMA among ASMR experiencers in this study.
Strengthening existing intimate relationships (Extracts 43, 46 & 47) and developing the
altruistic behaviors (Extracts 42 & 44) after watching ASMR videos is an essential aspect
of these ASMR’s experiences. The sound-only (e.g., slow, repetitive painting movement)
ASMR videos enhanced participants’ sense of love towards others. On the other hand, the
spoken-only (e.g., affirmation and discussion contents) ones strengthened their awareness
of others and made them more prone to help others. This finding aligns not only with
previous studies on ASMR research that the spoken-only ASMR videos made ASMR
participants feel more socially connected (Poerio et al., 2018, 8) but also noted the
importance of social interactions (empowering the loved ones and friendships) (McErlean
& Banissy, 2017, 613). Lochte et al. (2018) further suggested that the attention-receiving
contents in many ASMR videos were similar to being cared for someone, and this
suggestion is fitted into the current result. Also, some soft and kindness spoken ASMR
videos (Extract 40) seem to enhance participant’s self-compassion to change the negative
thoughts and feel the others’ love. Davis et al. (2011) stated that self-compassion fully
mediated the relationship between empathy and mindfulness. Hence, the results suggest
that spoken-only ASMR videos are most likely to improve self-acceptance and
interpersonal relationships.
71
In PERMA (Seligman, 2011), meaning referred that individual life is valuable and feeling
connected to something greater. The participant spoke of finding meaning and a sense of
purpose through the ASMR YouTube video as a platform to connect with others, and they
linked this personal growth perspective with better wellbeing. Moreover, the
meaningfulness of the experience is associated with the development process of
character, such as developing the curiosity of others and focusing on the present moment.
They move towards the acceptance of self and use reframing situations to find meaning
and promote positive emotion. The findings linked with the previous research suggested
that mindful awareness helps to strive for the purpose in life and enhances mental health
(Crego, Gómez-Martínez, & Karim., 2019, 2). Curiosity can motivate the exploration of the
self and the world by reducing the self-discrepancy gap between actual and ideal self
(Ivtzan, Gardner, & Smailova., 2011, 322). It could be possible to link this ASMR mediated
self-regulatory and positive meaning effect with some exploratory findings: (1) ASMR
videos serve as novel inputs triggering new interpretive information and arousing one’s
curiosity, (2) allow stimulus increases openness to alternative viewpoints and reframing
the adverse events through self-reflection; (3) shifting the attention toward the positive
aspects of self and world and enhance positive emotion; and (4) deepened the sense of
meaning and finally engaged in prosocial behavior (Garland, Farb, Goldin, & Fredrickson.,
2015, 7-12).
In terms of the fifth PERMA element, accomplishments referred to making progress toward
goals and having a sense of achievement (Seligman, 2011). Participants seem to gain a
sense of accomplishment (e.g., skills & goal settings) through ASMR videos utilizing the
mindful awareness and headspace of self-identity, recalling the memories, and broaden
the connections with others. The sense of confidence in their ability further empowers a
sense of meaning and purpose. Grégoire et al. (2012) suggested that individuals being
more attentive and aware tend to be more motivated autonomously and fosters their
wellbeing (Grégoire, Bouffard, & Vezeau., 2012, 244). Moreover, autobiographical memory
has two components; episodic to re-experience past events and semantic self-knowledge
that comprises facts and details about oneself. Haslam et al. (2011) stated that knowledge
of personal semantics mediated the relationship between episodic self-knowledge and
identity and hence one’s sense of self may determine what one remembers (i.e., knowing
who I determine what I remember) and vice versa (Haslam, Jetten, Pugliese, & Tonks.,
2011, 198-199). Also, semantic memory is used to provide the contextual information in
72
order to activate the relevant action rules in the procedural memory (Wang, Teow, & Tan.,
2014, 1476), and listening ASMR positive contents can enhance the short-term memory
(Kim et al., 2019, 229). Therefore, the present study’s findings complement and link the
description of the relevance of ASMR for fostering meaning, skills & goal settings by using
awareness of self and enhancing the retrieval of memories, such as episodic, semantic,
and working memory.
7.3 Limitations
There were several limitations to the current study. The combination of synchronous and
asynchronous data collection may affect the findings. Moreover, lacking the body language
and tone of voice, for instance, is making it difficult for the interviewer to know the
psychological responses of the interviewees. Furthermore, the findings from this study are
not generalizable owing to an exploratory study using a small sample (though perfectly
suitable for IPA). The ASMR videos sampling is also self-selecting, and the ASMR
experiencers that participated are not possible to represent all experiences. The data are
all self-report and thus lead to different biases, such as recall biases. Replication using
larger samples and other methodologies (e.g., quantitative) are therefore needed.
Furthermore, DSMM’s developmental susceptibility factors and transactional effect cannot
be identified in the study; that is, the failure to include younger adolescents and the cross-
sectional work are undoubtedly limitations of explorations in both elements of DSMM.
This study illuminates the variables in ASMR media effects research and moving forward,
ASMR media effects researchers can utilize ASMR media effect variables and ASMR
media effects model to inform the design of their studies. Moreover, they may choose to
examine further the variables included in this model or may work to identify other missing
variables and which could be included in future work to increase understanding. By using
quantitative methods, examining these variables in the psychosocial wellbeing among
larger samples could be a new direction for future research. Second, the social and
cultural aspects of ASMR media effects are not understood in the current study. Thus, how
the social and cultural aspects of consuming, understanding, and applications of ASMR
video affected the cross-cultural phenomenon (e.g., ASMR Mukbang in South Korea) is
needed to research in the future.
73
Furthermore, future research could also expand on the current findings by investigating
teens’ cognitive and emotional reactions to ASMR videos and assessing whether they get
similar benefits as adults. Longitudinal work would facilitate examination of relationships
between ASMR media use and subsequent wellbeing; it could also examine whether
positive media effects and ASMR media use over several months has decreasing returns
or, conversely, has increased the benefits. Therefore, future research should include a
longitudinal approach. Additionally, the result suggested that tingling sensation seems to
be controlled by a specific unknown system, and this system may act as a central hub to
connect body and mind through the complex physiological responses. At the same time,
the meridian is one of the terms in autonomous sensory meridian response, and the
meridians are invisible pathways in which Qi flows through the energy network that
connects all parts of the body, and the mind to the environment according to Traditional
Chinese Medicine (TCM) theory (Jeon & Lee, 2013, 3; Li & Zhao, 2012). ASMR
phenomenon is characterized by pleasant tingling sensations similar to the description of
De-qi sensations (e.g., tingling) in TCM (Zhu et al., 2013, 1); but the physiological
mechanisms that produce De-qi effect are still not well understood (Yang et al., 2013, 1)
and the same situation happened in the tingling sensation of ASMR (Valtakari et al., 2019,
3). Therefore, ASMR media effect researchers can explore this unknown area in
multidisciplinary approaches, such as biochemistry, genetics, neuroscience, and
psychophysiology. Hence, longitudinal studies with multidisciplinary methods are
recommended to illustrate and explore the challenges and the lived experiences of ASMR
in unknown research regions.
Research with a focus on the relationships of wellbeing with ASMR media effects
experiences is the first time using both DSMM and PERMA framework. The findings
suggest that ASMR experiences are shown to be linked to wellbeing, and as such, I
propose an ASMR media effects model to elaborate both DSMM and PERMA framework
that characterizes the five wellbeing domains and DSMM variables. The results show that
ASMR has flow-like and absorption states, and both occur when there is focused on
attention to present-moment stimuli and simultaneous awareness of self. Ultimately, based
on the role of attention, both tingling sensations and awareness appear to be linked to
mindfulness. The mindfulness-like practice in ASMR of which can result in the elevation of
positive emotions and other facets of wellbeing. As such, the findings provide support for
the connection of ASMR and wellbeing through mindfulness, as noted by Fredborg et al.,
74
(2018, 11) and Del Campo & Kehle. (2016, 5) that ASMR linked with intrinsically to
mindfulness to improve a variety of psychological wellbeing similar to mindfulness-based
treatment programs. In conclusion, this study suggests that university student’s
engagement with ASMR may uniquely contribute to enhancing wellbeing, such as feeling
happy, elevating the concentration on tasks at hand, improving the quality of sleep by
stopping rumination, increase self-confidence, and motivating altruistic behavior. This has
societal implications for the pursuit of flourishing and reaching wellbeing goals at-university
or at-home stress and adaptive program.
75
References
Bagwell, C., Bender, S., Andreassi, C., Kinoshita, T., Montarello, S., & Muller, J. (2005).
Friendship quality and perceived relationship changes predict psychosocial adjustment in
early adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 235–254.
Banissy, M. J., & Ward, J. (2013). Mechanisms of self-other representations and vicarious
experiences of touch in mirror-touch synesthesia. Frontiers in human neuroscience,
7(112).
Baños, R., Botella, C., Garcia-Palacios, A., Villa, H., Perpiñá, C., & Gallardo, M. (1999).
Psychological variables and reality judgment in virtual environments: the roles of
absorption and dissociation. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 2(2), 143–148.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wyke, M. A., & Binnie, C. (1987). Hearing words and seeing colors: an
experimental investigation of a case of synaesthesia. Perception, 16(6), 761–767.
Barratt, E. L., & Davis, N. J. (2015). Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR): a
flow-like mental state. PeerJ, 3, e851, 1–17.
Barratt, E. L., Spence, C., & Davis, N. J. (2017). Sensory determinants of the autonomous
sensory meridian response (ASMR): understanding the triggers. PeerJ, 5, e3846, 1–13.
Berrios, R., Totterdell, P., & Kellett, S. (2015). Eliciting mixed emotions: a meta-analysis
comparing models, types, and measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 6(428), 1–15.
76
Beyens, I., Valkenburg, P. M., & Piotrowski, J. T. (2018). Screen media use and ADHD-
related behaviors: Four decades of research. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 115(40), 9875–9881.
Boden, Z., Larkin, M., & Iyer, M. (2019). Picturing ourselves in the world: Drawings,
interpretative phenomenological analysis, and the relational mapping interview. Qualitative
Research in Psychology, 16(2), 218–236.
Bonus, J. A., Peebles, A., Mares, M. L., & Sarmiento, I. G. (2018). Look on the bright side
(of media effects): Pokémon Go as a catalyst for positive life experiences. Media
Psychology, 21(2), 263–287.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its
role in psychological well-being. Journal of personality and social psychology, 84(4), 822.
Cash, D. K., Heisick, L. L., & Papesh, M. H. (2018). Expectancy effects in the autonomous
sensory meridian response. PeerJ, 6. Advance online publication.
doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5229
Chappell, D., Eatough, V., Davies, M. N., & Griffiths, M. (2006). EverQuest—It’s just a
computer game, right? An interpretative phenomenological analysis of online gaming
addiction. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 4(3), 205–216.
Crego, A., Yela, J. R., Gómez-Martínez, M. Á., & Karim, A. A. (2019). The Contribution of
Meaningfulness and Mindfulness to Psychological Well-Being and Mental Health: A
Structural Equation Model. Journal of Happiness Studies, 1–24.
Crespo, A., Recuero, M., Galvez, G., & Begoña, A. (2013). Effect of binaural stimulation on
attention and EEG. Archives of Acoustics, 38(4), 517–528.
77
Colonnello, V., Petrocchi, N., Farinelli, M., & Ottaviani, C. (2017). Positive social
interactions in a lifespan perspective with a focus on opioidergic and oxytocinergic
systems: Implications for neuroprotection. Current Neuropharmacology, 15(4), 543–561.
Daher, M., Carré, D., Jaramillo, A., Olivares, H., & Tomicic, A. (2017, July). Experience
and meaning in qualitative research: A conceptual review and a methodological device
proposal. In Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research,18 (3).
Davis, D. M., & Hayes, J. A. (2011). What are the benefits of mindfulness? A practice
review of psychotherapy-related research. Psychotherapy, 48(2), 198.
De Leeuw, R. N., & Buijzen, M. (2016). Introducing positive media psychology to the field
of children, adolescents, and media. Journal of Children and Media, 10(1), 39–46.
Del Campo, M. A., & Kehle, T. J. (2016). Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR)
and frisson: Mindfully induced sensory phenomena that promote happiness. International
Journal of School & Educational Psychology, 4(2), 1–8.
Diener, E., & Seligman, M. (2002). Very happy people. Psychological Science, 13(1), 81–
84.
Diener, E., Suh, E. M., Lucas, R. E., & Smith, H. L. (1999). Subjective well-being: Three
decades of progress. Psychological Bulletin, 125(2), 276–302.
D’raven, L. L., & Pasha-Zaidi, N. (2016). Using the PERMA model in the United Arab
Emirates. Social indicators research, 125(3), 905–933.
Eisenbarth, H., Chang, L. J., & Wager, T. D. (2016). Multivariate brain prediction of heart
rate and skin conductance responses to social threat. Journal of Neuroscience, 36(47),
11987–11998.
Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., De Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural
network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 903–911.
Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones and Behavior,
61(3), 380–391.
78
Fikkers, K. M., & Piotrowski, J. T. (2019). Content and person effects in media research:
Studying differences in cognitive, emotional, and arousal responses to media content.
Media Psychology, 1–28.
Fishbach, A., & Labroo, A. (2007). Be better or be merry? How mood influence self-
control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(2), 158–173.
Forgeard, M. J., Jayawickreme, E., Kern, M. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2011). Doing the right
thing: Measuring wellbeing for public policy. International journal of wellbeing, 1(1), 79–
106.
Fredborg, B., Clark, J., & Smith, S. D. (2017). An examination of personality traits
associated with autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). Frontiers in psychology,
8, 247.
Fredborg, B. K., Clark, J. M., & Smith, S. D. (2018). Mindfulness and autonomous sensory
meridian response (ASMR). PeerJ, 6. Advance online publication.
doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5414.
Gadgil, A. A., & Desai, V. V. (2018). Modeling Situational Behavior Using Semantic And
Episodic Memory Through Machine Learning. Conference: International Conference on
Electrical, Electronics, Computers, Communication, Mechanical and Computing
(EECCMC).
Garland, E. L., Farb, N. A., R. Goldin, P., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2015). Mindfulness
broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic meaning: A process model of mindful positive
emotion regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 26(4), 1–34.
Glisky, M. L., Tataryn, D. J., Tobias, B. A., Kihlstrom, J. F., & McConkey, K. M. (1991).
Absorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizability. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 60(2), 263.
Grégoire, S., Bouffard, T., & Vezeau, C. (2012). Personal goal setting as a mediator of the
relationship between mindfulness and wellbeing. International Journal of Wellbeing, 2(3).
Grewe O, Kopiez R, Altenmüüller E. 2009. The chill parameter: goosebumps and shivers
as promising measures in emotion research. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary
Journal 27(1), 61–74
79
Grimaldi Toriz, S. (2009). Epidemiological data of seasonal variation in mood and
behavior. (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from
https://www.julkari.fi/bitstream/handle/10024/79896/b757a4bd-9245-438e-a383-
37a4737e9d43.pdf?sequence=1
Gyllensten, A. L., Skär, L., Miller, M., & Gard, G. (2010). Embodied identity - A deeper
understanding of body awareness. Physiotherapy Theory and Practice, 26(7), 439–446.
Haslam, C., Jetten, J., Haslam, S. A., Pugliese, C., & Tonks, J. (2011). ‘I remember
therefore I am, and I am therefore I remember’: Exploring the contributions of episodic and
semantic self‐knowledge to strength of identity. British Journal of Psychology, 102(2), 184–
203.
Hershfield, H. E., Scheibe, S., Sims, T. L., & Carstensen, L. L. (2013). When feeling bad
can be good: Mixed emotions benefit physical health across adulthood. Social
psychological and personality science, 4(1), 54–61.
Hidayat, R., Habibi, A., Saad, M., Rashid, M., Mukminin, A., & Idris, W. (2018). Exploratory
and confirmatory factor analysis of PERMA for Indonesian students in mathematics
education programmes. Pedagogika, 2018,132 (4),147–165.
Houston, J. B., Spialek, M. L., & First, J. (2018). Disaster media effects: A systematic
review and synthesis based on the differential susceptibility to media effects model.
Journal of Communication, 68(4), 734–757.
Huang, T. L., & Charyton, C. (2008). A comprehensive review of the psychological effects
of brainwave entrainment. In Database of Abstracts of Reviews of Effects (DARE): Quality-
assessed Reviews [Internet]. Centre for Reviews and Dissemination (UK).
Huta, V., & Waterman, A. S. (2014). Eudaimonia and its distinction from hedonia:
Developing a classification and terminology for understanding conceptual and operational
definitions. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 1425–1456.
Ilias, K., Liaw, J. H. J., Cornish, K., Park, M. S. A., & Golden, K. J. (2017). Wellbeing of
mothers of children with “AUTISM” in Malaysia: An interpretative phenomenological
analysis study. Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 42(1), 74–89.
80
Ivtzan, I., Gardner, H. E., & Smailova, Z. (2011). Mindfulness meditation and curiosity: The
contributing factors to wellbeing and the process of closing the self-discrepancy gap.
International Journal of Wellbeing, 1(3).
Jackson SA, Marsh HW. 1996. Development and validation of scale to measure optimal
experience: flow state scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18(1), 17–35
Jeon, J., & Lee, S. (2013). The meanings and prospects of primo vascular system from the
viewpoint of historical context. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine,
2013.
Jirakittayakorn, N., & Wongsawat, Y. (2018). A novel insight of effects of a 3-Hz binaural
beat on sleep stages during sleep. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 12(387).
Johnston DW, Anastasiades P. 1990. The relationship between heart rate and mood in
real life. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34(1), 21–27
Jones, C., Scholes, L., Johnson, D., Katsikitis, M., & Carras, M. C. (2014). Gaming well:
links between videogames and flourishing mental health. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(260),
1–8.
Keizer, A., Chang, T. H., O’Mahony, C. J., Schaap, N. S., & Stone, K. D. (2020).
Individuals Who Experience Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response Have Higher Levels
of Sensory Suggestibility. Perception, 49(1), 113–116.
Kim, D., Kim, T., Seo, G., Shin, M. H., Lee, Y. J. & Hwang, W. (2019). Sensory channel
effects of autonomous sensory meridian response on short-term memory. ICIC Express
Letters, 13(3), 225–230.
Kohonen, I., Kuula-Luumi, A., & Spoof, S. K. (2019). The ethical principles of research with
human participants and ethical review in the human sciences in Finland. Helsinki, Finland:
Finnish National Board on Research Integrity TENK.
Kok, B., Catalino, L., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2008). The broadening, building, buffering
effects of positive emotions. In S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Positive psychology: Exploring the best
of people (Vol. 3, pp. 1–19). Westport, CT: Greenwood.
81
Kovacevich, A., & Huron, D. (2019). Two Studies of Autonomous Sensory Meridian
Response (ASMR): The Relationship between ASMR and Music-Induced Frisson.
Empirical Musicology Review, 13(1-2), 39–63.
Larkin, M., Watts, S., & Clifton, E. (2006). Giving voice and making sense in interpretative
phenomenological analysis. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 102–120
Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., ... & Devins,
G. (2006). The Toronto mindfulness scale: Development and validation. Journal of clinical
psychology, 62(12), 1445–1467.
Li, X. T., & Zhao, J. (2012). An approach to the nature of Qi in TCM-Qi and bioenergy.
Recent Advances in Theories and Practice of Chinese Medicine, 79–108.
Lochte, B. C., Guillory, S. A., Richard, C. A., & Kelley, W. M. (2018). An fMRI investigation
of the neural correlates underlying the autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR).
BioImpacts: BI, 8(4), 295.
Marzano, C., Moroni, F., Gorgoni, M., Nobili, L., Ferrara, M., & De Gennaro, L. (2013).
How we fall asleep: regional and temporal differences in electroencephalographic
synchronization at sleep onset. Sleep medicine, 14(11), 1112–1122.
82
McErlean, A. B. J. & Banissy, M. J. (2017). Assessing individual variation in personality
and empathy traits in self-reported autonomous sensory meridian response. Multisensory
Research, 30(6), 601–613.
Mirehie, M., & Gibson, H. J. (2019). Women's participation in snow-sports and sense of
wellbeing: a positive psychology approach. Journal of Leisure Research, 1–19.
Noble, T., & McGrath, H. (2015). PROSPER: A new framework for positive education.
Psychology of Wellbeing, 5(1), 1–17.
Perez, H. O., Dumas, G., & Lehmann, A. (2019). Binaural Beats through the auditory
pathway: from brainstem to connectivity patterns. eNeuro, 7(2). Advance online
publication. DOI: 10.1523/ENEURO.0232-19.2020
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: interactions of the amygdala and
hippocampal complex. Current opinion in neurobiology, 14(2), 198–202.
Poerio, G. L., Blakey, E., Hostler, T. J., & Veltri, T. (2018). More than a feeling:
Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is characterized by reliable changes in
affect and physiology. PloS one, 13(6), 1–18.
83
Priyadarshini, C., Dubey, R. K., Kumar, Y. L. N., & Jha, R. R. (2020). Impact of a Social
Media Addiction on Employees’ Wellbeing and Work Productivity. The Qualitative Report,
25(1), 181–196.
Ratan, R. (2013). Self-presence, explicated: Body, Emotion, and identity extension into the
virtual self. In Handbook of research on technoself: Identity in a technological society (pp.
322-336). IGI Global.
Reid, K., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2005). Exploring lived experience. The Psychologist,
18(1), 20–23.
Rieger, D., Reinecke, L., Frischlich, L., & Bente, G. (2014). Media entertainment and well-
being—Linking hedonic and eudaimonic entertainment experience to media-induced
recovery and vitality. Journal of Communication, 64(3), 1–23.
Rouw, R., & Erfanian, M. (2018). A large‐scale study of misophonia. Journal of clinical
psychology, 74(3), 453–479.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of
research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual review of psychology, 52(1),
141–166.
Sabihi, S., Dong, S. M., Durosko, N. E., & Leuner, B. (2014). Oxytocin in the medial
prefrontal cortex regulates maternal care, maternal aggression and anxiety during the
postpartum period. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 8(258).
Schaekermann, M., Ribeiro, G., Wallner, G., Kriglstein, S., Johnson, D., Drachen, A., ... &
Nacke, L. E. (2017, October). Curiously Motivated: Profiling Curiosity with Self-Reports
and Behaviour Metrics in the Game Destiny. In Proceedings of the Annual Symposium on
Computer-Human Interaction in Play, 143–156.
Scott, Baird (2020). So, Nintendo-Approved Pokémon ASMR Videos Exist. 20.01.2020.
Retrieved from https://www.thegamer.com/nintendo-approved-pokemon-asmr-videos/
84
Seligman, M. E. (2004). Can happiness be taught? Daedalus, 133(2), 80–87.
Setz, Clemens (2015). “High durch sich räuspernde Menschen”. Süddeutsche Zeitung (in
German). 06.04.2015. Retrieved https://www.sueddeutsche.de/kultur/gastbeitrag-das-
namenlose-gefuehl-1.2423469
Shapiro D, Jamner LD, Golstein IB, Delfino RJ. (2001). Striking a chord: moods, blood
pressure, and heart rate in everyday life. Psychophysiology, 38(2), 197– 204.
Smith, J. A., Flowers, P., & Larkin, M. (2009). Interpretative phenomenological analysis:
Theory, method and research. London, England: Sage Publications.
Smith, S. D., Fredborg, B. K., & Kornelsen, J. (2019a). A functional magnetic resonance
imaging investigation of the autonomous sensory meridian response. PeerJ, 7. Advance
online publication. doi.org/10.7717/peerj.7122.
Smith, S. D., Fredborg, B. K., & Kornelsen, J. (2019b). Atypical functional connectivity
associated with autonomous sensory meridian response: an examination of five resting-
state networks. Brain connectivity, 9(6), 1–39.
Song, C. B., Lee, M., & Lee, S. W. (2019). Possible Effect of Binaural Beat Combined with
Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response for Inducing Sleep. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 13(425).
Stefanov, M., Potroz, M., Kim, J., Lim, J., Cha, R., & Nam, M. H. (2013). The primo
vascular system as a new anatomical system. Journal of acupuncture and meridian
studies, 6(6), 331–338.
Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): development and initial
validation. Psychological assessment, 25(4), 1286.
85
Tang, H. Y., Vitiello, M. V., Perlis, M., Mao, J. J., & Riegel, B. (2014). A pilot study of
audio–visual stimulation as a self-care treatment for insomnia in adults with insomnia and
chronic pain. Applied psychophysiology and biofeedback, 39(3-4), 219–225.
Tansey, T. N., Smedema, S., Umucu, E., Iwanaga, K., Wu, J. R., Cardoso, E. D. S., &
Strauser, D. (2018). Assessing college life adjustment of students with disabilities:
application of the PERMA framework. Rehabilitation Counselling Bulletin, 61(3), 131–142.
Tice, D., Baumeister, R., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive
affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 43, 379–384.
Tihanyi, B. T., Ferentzi, E., Beissner, F., & Köteles, F. (2018). The neuropsychophysiology
of tingling. Consciousness and cognition, 58, 97–110.
Umucu, E., Wu, J. R., Sanchez, J., Brooks, J. M., Chiu, C. Y., Tu, W. M., & Chan, F.
(2019). Psychometric validation of the PERMA-profiler as a wellbeing measure for student
veterans. Journal of American college health, 1–7.
Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. (2009). The effects of instant messaging on the quality of
adolescents’ existing friendships: A longitudinal study. Journal of Communication, 59, 79–
97.
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013a). Five challenges for the future of media-effects
research. International Journal of Communication, 7, 197–215.
Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2013b). The differential susceptibility to media effects
model. Journal of Communication, 63, 221–243.
Valtakari, N. V., Hooge, I. T., Benjamins, J. S., & Keizer, A. (2019). An eye-tracking
approach to Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): The physiology and nature
of tingles in relation to the pupil. PloS one, 14(12), 1–13.
86
Wahbeh, H., Calabrese, C., Zwickey, H., & Zajdel, D. (2007). Binaural beat technology in
humans: a pilot study to assess neuropsychologic, physiologic, and
electroencephalographic effects. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine,
13(2), 199–206.
Wang, W., Tan, A. H., Teow, L. N., & Tan, Y. S. (2014). Declarative-procedural memory
interaction in learning agents. In Proceedings of the 2014 international conference on
Autonomous agents and multi-agent systems (pp.1475–1476).
Weibel, D., Wissmath, B., & Mast, F. W. (2010). Immersion in mediated environments: the
role of personality traits. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13(3), 251–
256.
Woolf, V. (2005). “Mrs Dalloway”. Selected Works of Virginia Woolf (p.141). Hertfordshire:
Wordsworth Editions.
Wright Jr, K. P., Badia, P., & Wauquier, A. (1995). Topographical and temporal patterns of
brain activity during the transition from wakefulness to sleep. Sleep, 18(10), 880–889.
Yang, X. Y., Shi, G. X., Li, Q. Q., Zhang, Z. H., Xu, Q., & Liu, C. Z. (2013).
Characterization of deqi sensation and acupuncture effect. Evidence-Based
Complementary and Alternative Medicine. Advance online publication.
doi.org/10.1155/2013/319734
Yerbury, R. M., & Boyd, W. E. (2019). Dolphins and Human Flourishing: A Novel
Application of the PERMA Model. Ecopsychology, 11(4), 201–212.
Zhu, S. P., Luo, L., Zhang, L., Shen, S. X., Ren, X. X., Guo, M. W., ... & Zhu, J. (2013).
Acupuncture de-qi: from characterization to underlying mechanism. Evidence-Based
Complementary and Alternative Medicine. Advance online publication.
doi.org/10.1155/2013/518784
87
Appendix A
Name: Email:
Instructions
In the following table, you mark the statement (1-50) on the left side of the box: on the
scale 1-5, where 1=disagree, 2=slightly disagree, 3=neutral, 4=slightly agree and 5=agree.
Rating I….. Rating I…..
1. Am the life of the party. 26. Have little to say
2. Feel little concern for others. 27. Have a soft heart.
3. Am always prepared. 28. Often forget to put things back in the first place.
4. Get stressed out easily 29. Get upset easily.
5. Have a rich vocabulary. 30. Do not have a good imagination.
6. Don't talk a lot. 31. Talk to different people at different parties.
7. Am interested in people. 32. Am not really interested in others.
8. Leave my belongings around. 33. Like order.
9. Am relaxed most of the time. 34. Change my mood a lot.
10. Have difficulty understanding abstract ideas. 35. Am quick to understand things.
11. Feel comfortable around people. 36. Don't like to draw attention to myself.
12. Insult people. 37. Take time out for others.
13. Pay attention to details. 38. Shirk my duties.
14. Worry about things. 39. Have frequent mood swings.
15. Have a vivid imagination. 40. Use difficult words.
16. Keep in the background 41. Don't mind being the center of attention.
17. Sympathize with others' feelings. 42. Feel others' emotions.
18. Make a mess of things. 43. Follow a schedule.
19. Seldom feel blue. 44. Get irritated easily.
20. Am not interested in abstract ideas. 45. Spend time reflecting on things.
21. Start conversations. 46. Am quiet around strangers.
22. Am not interested in other people's problems. 47. Make people feel at ease.
23. Get chores done right away. 48. Am exacting in my work.
24. Am easily disturbed. 49. Often feel blue.
25. Have excellent ideas. 50. Am full of ideas.
88
Appendix B
You are invited to participate in a master’s research study being conducted by Chan Ko
Wai, who is studying the master's degree in media education at the University of Lapland.
This consent form is designed to inform individuals of their research involvement entirely.
Please review the document carefully and feel free to ask for additional clarification.
You will be asked to participate in two in-depth interview series regarding how ASMR
experience through YouTube video enhances Psychosocial Wellbeing at the University of
Lapland. You will be provided with Personality Test before the study and interview
questions during the study and ask to check for accuracy and share any additional
commentary within seven days of receiving the transcription through email.
89
What are the possible disadvantages of participating in this study, and how should these
risks be minimized?
As in all research, there may be unforeseen risks to you as a participant. This study
presents only minimal risk because data is collected and communicated through the
anonymity of a pseudonym.
Your participation in this study may help me shape the way to understand the relationship
between ASMR experiences and Psychosocial Wellbeing. You may benefit from knowing
you are participating in sharing precious experiences of ASMR and Welling at the
University of Lapland. All the information collected from you is confidential. Your name will
not appear on any documents where information is recorded. Names will be changed with
a pseudonym of your choosing and will be kept in a separate master list. Once the
interview transcripts are collected and analyzed, the master list will be destroyed.
You can choose to stop participating in the study at any time for any reason.
If you have any inquiries before or during the study, you can contact the Researcher, Mr.
Chan Ko Wai, at [email protected] if questions arise during the study.
------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
I understand my participation in this informed consent document. The risks and benefits
have been explained to me. I consent and voluntarily agree to participate in this study.
______________________________ ____________________________
Date: _________________________
90
Appendix C
(A review of the participant's life history related to the topic of inquiry, up to the time s/he ASMR experiences. Objective:
to find out as much as possible about the context of the participant has lived ASMR experiences leading up to now: early
experiences with ASMR experiences? what were the perceived emotional and mediated experiences of the ASMR
phenomenon?)
Sub-Questions/Explorations:
The participants do NOT need to answer ALL sub-questions. These questions help them to think deeper.
1. What are the earliest age you can recall having an ASMR experience and describe what you feel and happenings?
2. How long does the tingling sensation typically last?
3. What do you feel a tingling sensation when watching ASMR videos?
a. Where can your tingles originate? (Possible list: Head, Shoulders, Chest, Back, Arms, Stomach/lower abdomen,
Genitals, Hips, Legs, and Feet, etc.)
b. Does this tingling sensation always originate in one area of your body?
c. Do you feel this tingling sensation more on one side of your body than the other? If yes, which side, or both
sides?
d. Which body areas that experience tingles when the sensation is most intense?
e. Is the tingling sensation triggered more easily by hearing in one ear than the other? (left ear, right ear, or both
ears are the same)
1. What do you feel that watching ASMR videos helps with your sleeping issues? How often do you use ASMR videos
or audio files to help you go to sleep?
2. What do you feel that watching ASMR videos affects your mood? How often do you use ASMR videos or audio files
to help you relax (but not sleep)?
91
Appendix D
Questions/Explorations:
2. How and what do you feel excited and interested in things (e.g., schoolwork and tasks)?
3. How do you lose track of time while doing something you enjoy and become absorbed in what
you are doing?
4. How and what do you get help and support from others when you need it?
5. How fulfilled and satisfied are you with your relationships (Prompt: Parent, Peer, and Teacher-
student)?
6. Have you been feeling loved? How?
7. How do you lead a purposeful and meaningful school life (e.g., learning task)?
8. How do you feel you have a sense of direction in your school life?
9. Can you tell me about any times, if any, you feel that ASMR experience has influenced or been
a part of your schoolwork or activities in any way? Prompts: within the university, working with
peers.
10. How do you feel you are making progress towards accomplishing your goals in the university?
92
Appendix E
93
Appendix F
states invest cognitive effort to understand media content, which is operationalized here
Indirect Effects (mediators)
Relationship E3 The feeling of being cared about by others, socially integrated, and supported and
involves a sense of connectedness, loving, and sharing emotions with others related to
self-esteem, and that increase in friendship quality perception
Meaning E4 A feeling of doing something meaningful and having connected to something
bigger
A sense of purpose that is closely related to a sense of meaning in life and
frequently involves a pro-social or altruistic intent such as a commitment to helping
others or improving the world
Accomplishment E5 An individual’s perception of making progress toward goals and having a sense of
achievement in one’s life
Desire to achieve something (e.g., having a persevering attitude) rather than one’s
actual accomplishments
94