Journal of Environmental Psychology: Nathaniel Geiger, Janet K. Swim
Journal of Environmental Psychology: Nathaniel Geiger, Janet K. Swim
a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t
Article history: Despite the importance of interpersonal public communication about climate change, most citizens
Received 21 August 2015 rarely discuss the topic. In two studies, we find that inaccurate perceptions of others' opinions (i.e.
Received in revised form pluralistic ignorance) contribute to self-silencing among those concerned about climate change. Study 1
29 April 2016
illustrates that those who are aware of others' concern about climate change report greater willingness to
Accepted 6 May 2016
discuss the issue than those with inaccurate perceptions of others' opinions. Study 2 demonstrates that
Available online 8 May 2016
correcting pluralistic ignorance increases concerned participants' willingness to discuss climate change.
In both studies, pluralistic ignorance leads to self-silencing because perceptions that others do not share
Keywords:
Climate change
one's opinion are associated with expecting to be perceived as less competent in a conversation about
Pluralistic ignorance climate change. In contrast to previous research on confronting prejudice, in the present research ex-
Discussion pectations about being disliked did not explain self-silencing. We discuss the implications for self-
Impression management silencing and promoting interpersonal communication about climate change.
Stereotype content model © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
* Corresponding author. 511 Moore Building, University Park, PA 16802, USA. It is perhaps unsurprising that the public has demonstrated
E-mail address: [email protected] (J.K. Swim). pluralistic ignorance about climate change (Leviston et al., 2013).
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.05.002
0272-4944/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
80 N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90
Pluralistic ignorance has been demonstrated across many topics: climate change (Markowitz, 2012). Another point of view,
support for racial segregation in the 1970s (most white Americans expressed by a vocal minority who question the scientific
supported desegregation but believed that most others supported consensus, is that climate change is a conspiracy that is immorally
segregation; O'Gorman & Garry, 1976), norms of alcohol con- being promoted as scientific fact by those who wish to promote a
sumption (university students believed that norms of alcohol specific political agenda (Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Gignac, 2013).
consumption were excessive but perceived that most others sup- In sum, it appears that climate change has culturally acquired a
ported them; Prentice & Miller, 1993), opinions on foreign policy controversial, moral connotation in modern society, and thus we
(most Americans support multilateral foreign policy but perceive propose that the processes described in pluralistic ignorance and
that most other Americans support unilateral foreign policy; spiral of silence will also apply to climate change. Thus, we make
Todorov & Mandisodza, 2004) and comfort with “hooking up” the following prediction:
(students estimated that others felt more comfortable engaging in
Hypothesis 1. Participants will be less willing to talk about climate
uncommitted sexual activity than they did; Lambert, Kahn, &
change when they perceive that their opinions are in the minority (vs.
Apple, 2003). Pluralistic ignorance could in part be due to the
the majority).
lack of regular conversations about climate change (Leiserowitz
et al., 2015), which could lead to individuals having little insight
into others' internal beliefs. Interestingly, pluralistic ignorance on 3. Impression management and self-silencing
climate change has even been found among climate scientists who
underestimate concern among other scientific experts Self-silencing may be a form of impression management. In-
(Lewandowsky, Oreskes, Risbey, Newell, & Smithson, 2015). dividuals desire to be viewed in a positive light and sharing an
Pluralistic ignorance can have significant consequences for unpopular opinion could result in others perceiving them nega-
effectively addressing social issues. Pluralistic ignorance is associ- tively. Researchers have proposed that people self-silence because
ated with attitude change shifting toward the perceived norm of fear of isolation (Noelle-Neumann, 1993), rejection (Bergsieker,
(Leviston et al., 2013; Prentice & Miller, 1993); behavioral confor- Shelton, & Richeson, 2010), social retributions for violating cul-
mity to the perceived norm (Prentice & Miller, 1993; Schroeder & tural norms prescribing silence (Norgaard, 2011), embarrassment
Prentice, 1998), and relevant to the present study, reduced will- (Miller & McFarland, 1987), being dismissed as a “complainer”
ingness to share one's opinion on a topic (Miller & McFarland, 1987; (Kaiser & Miller, 2001; Swim & Hyers, 1999), and being seen as
Rios & Chen, 2014; Taylor, 1982). Conversely, correcting pluralistic ignorant (Salmon & Neuwirth, 1990).
ignorance by providing information about the true beliefs of others The varied explanations for self-silencing listed in the above
can reverse these effects (Schroeder & Prentice, 1998). paragraph can be organized along two core dimensions of im-
The spiral of silence theory specifically addresses the impact of pressions: fears about being a) disliked or b) losing respect. Inter-
pluralistic ignorance on public discourse (Noelle-Neumann, 1993; personal evaluation research suggests that up to 90% of initial
Taylor, 1982). This theory proposes that individuals scan their so- impressions of others can be organized along these two core di-
cial environment for information about others' opinions and that mensions, which directly reflect the core dimensions of social
people are less willing to share their opinion when informational cognition: warmth (those perceived as low in warmth are disliked)
cues lead them to believe that they hold a minority view (vs. ma- and competence (those perceived as low in competence are not
jority view), especially when the topic is perceived as controversial respected) (Abele & Wojciszke, 2007; Fiske et al., 1999). These two
or morally charged (Noelle-Neumann, 1993). Silencing is proposed dimensions have been consistently described across various
to be self-reinforcing: if many who hold a particular view believe literature (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; Heider, 1958; Rosenberg,
that they are in the minority and remain silent, the silence leads Nelson, & Vivekanathan, 1968; Singh, Ho, Tan, & Bell, 2007) and
others who share this view to believe that their opinion is un- align with two basic impression management goals: the desire to
common and encourages them to also remain silent. Motivation to have an audience think favorably about oneself and the desire to
self-silence is also proposed to increase when individuals believe present one's ideal self to others (Bergsieker et al., 2010; Schlenker,
that their opinion is declining in public popularity (Taylor, 1982). 1975). Thus, warmth reflects being perceived as friendly and
The premises derived from pluralistic ignorance and spiral of cooperative, while competence corresponds with being respected
silence theory may explain why people are hesitant to discuss and achieving high social status (Fiske et al., 1999, 2002). Being
climate change. The principles outlined in spiral of silence theory perceived as either cold (i.e., confrontational and unlikeable) or
are purported to apply primarily to morally controversial or value- incompetent (i.e., not respected and low status) are distinct
laden topics, such as abortion, support for addressing racial grounds for anticipated social rejection, and thus people may alter
inequality, and political party preference in national elections (Moy, their behavior in attempts to manage others' impressions of them
Domke, & Stamm, 2001; Noelle-Neumann, 1993). Climate change on one or both of these dimensions (Holoien & Fiske, 2013).
might appear to differ from these more commonly studied topics
because climate change is a scientific topic supported by a solid 3.1. Avoiding being disliked
body of evidence and an overwhelming consensus of scientific
experts whom agree that human-caused climate change is occur- The desire to avoid being disliked has been well established as a
ring and presents a significant threat to global civilization (Cook motive for self-silencing when one is a target of discrimination and
et al., 2013, 2016; Oreskes, 2004). Yet, expression of opinions prejudice. (Sechrist, Swim, & Stangor, 2004; Shelton & Stewart,
about climate change has taken on a cultural significance distinct 2004; Stangor et al., 2003; Swim & Hyers, 1999). This desire leads
from scientific understanding of the topic due to its politicization. individuals to refrain from confronting discrimination despite their
About half of U.S. senators recently voted to publicly deny that wishes to do so (e.g., Swim, Eyssell, Murdoch, & Ferguson, 2010) or
“human activity significantly contributes to climate change” despite what they expect they would do (Shelton & Stewart, 2004;
(Goldenberg, 2015), despite scientific consensus and only approx- Swim & Hyers, 1999; Woodzicka & LaFrance, 2001), particularly in
imately 10% of Americans similarly dismissing the scientific evi- the presence of others expected to not share one's own point of
dence behind anthropogenic climate change (Leiserowitz et al., view (Swim & Hyers, 1999). Individuals faced with discrimination
2015). Further, many perceive the topic principally as a moral often perceive the possibility of confronting as impolite (Swim &
topic because of the potential negative impacts of unchecked Hyers, 1999) and those who do confront are devalued as difficult
N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90 81
to interact with and “complainers” (Kaiser & Miller, 2001). shows that most nonscientists have limited understanding of the
Consistent with the argument that individuals self-silence to avoid scientific mechanisms of climate change (Leiserowitz, Smith, &
being disliked, women were less likely to assertively respond to Marlon, 2010; Swim et al., 2014), and thus may be concerned
sexist comments during a job interview when the desire to be liked about appearing ignorant or incompetent when discussing this
was emphasized than when the desire to be respected was valued topic. Further, the expectation of appearing incompetent may be
more highly (Mallett & Melchiori, 2014). amplified when considering a discussion with an audience not
Fear of being disliked may also motivate suppression of opinions expected to share one's views, partly because a dissenting audience
about climate change. Individuals may be concerned about being may challenge the speaker or question their assumptions. Based
perceived as an “alarmist” or environmental activist if they were to upon the above analyses we make the following hypothesis:
express their concern about the topic, labels which carry poten-
Hypothesis 3. Individuals’ hesitation to discuss climate change in
tially negative connotations of being disliked by others. Similar to
situations when they perceive their opinions are in the minority (vs.
those who confront discrimination, those who are “alarmed” about
the majority) will be partly explained by expectations of appearing less
climate change are perceived as “whiny”, “nagging”, and “com-
competent in the conversation.
plainers” (Swim & Geiger, 2016b) and prototypic environmental
activists are commonly stereotyped as “eccentric”, “self-righteous,”
and “reactive” (Bashir, Lockwood, Chasteen, Nadolny, & Noyes,
2013), all traits associated with being seen as cold and disliked by 4. Present research
others. These negative impressions are associated with reduced
willingness to engage in climate change activism and to affiliate In two studies, we examine the effects of pluralistic ignorance
with environmental activists. Expectations about being disliked for on willingness to discuss climate change. We first conducted two
speaking one's opinions may be accentuated in particular contexts; pilot studies to verify that the pattern of pluralistic ignorance about
namely, when individuals anticipate that others do not share their climate change observed in Leviston et al. (2013) work would
views and thus believe that expressing their opinion would be replicate in our target population. Next, in Study 1 we examine
confrontational within a given context (Noelle-Neumann, 1993). whether participants who do not themselves doubt the scientific
Based upon the above, we make the following prediction. view on climate change and hold inaccurate perceptions of others'
opinions are less willing to discuss the topic than those who
Hypothesis 2. Individuals’ hesitation to discuss climate change in
endorse similar views about climate change but hold accurate
situations when they perceive their opinions are in the minority (vs.
perceptions of others' opinions. In Study 2, we experimentally
the majority) will be partly explained by expectations of appearing less
manipulate perceptions of others' opinions and examine the effects
warm in the conversation.
of correcting pluralistic ignorance on facilitating discussion relative
to emphasizing the false perceptions of others' opinions. Study 2
3.2. Avoiding losing respect participants include a full range of personal opinions about climate
change ranging from those who are very concerned about climate
Another motive for self-silencing is the concern that one would change to those who consider themselves nonbelievers. In both
lose others' respect following a conversation about a topic. studies, we examine whether expectations of being perceived as a)
Expressing an unpopular opinion could result in appearing igno- warm and/or b) competent explain the psychological process un-
rant to others (Salmon & Neuwirth, 1990), and people may remain derlying these effects.
silent out of fear of embarrassing themselves when they believe
that they are less knowledgeable about a topic than others (Miller &
McFarland, 1987). Yet, research suggests that confronting discrim- 5. Pilot studies
ination does not lead to the confronter being perceived as incom-
petent (i.e. losing respect; Stangor et al., 2003; Swim, Gervais, Pilot testing with undergraduate students in introductory psy-
Pearson, & Stangor, 2009), and women more interested in being chology courses (reflecting a range of students across the campus)
respected than liked were more likely to confront sexism during a were consistent with previous findings about pluralistic ignorance
job interview than other women (Mallett & Melchiori, 2014). This on climate change (Leviston et al., 2013). One pilot sample
could suggest that concerns about losing respect are less central completed a screening instrument that has been used to categorize
than concerns about being disliked when individuals consider the public into different levels of concern about climate change (i.e.,
whether to self-silence unpopular opinions. the Six Americas' questionnaire, Maibach, Leiserowitz, Roser-
However, in contrast to confronting discrimination, the degree Renouf, & Mertz, 2011) and self-categorized into different levels
to which an individual expects to be perceived as competent may of concern about climate change as assessed by the same screening
affect willingness to engage in discussions about climate change. instrument. Both methods indicated that a majority of students
Since climate change is a scientific topic, expectations of appearing were on concerned side of the opinion spectrum: survey instru-
competent may be more salient than expectations of appearing ment (N ¼ 365): 7% Alarmed, 40% Concerned, 40% Cautious, 3%
warm since understanding of scientific topics maps onto the Disengaged, 8% Doubtful, 3% Dismissive; self-categorization
competence dimension, but not the warmth dimension, of social (N ¼ 368): 8% Alarmed, 28% Concerned, 39% Cautious, 19% Disen-
cognition (Fiske, Cuddy, & Glick, 2007). This proposition is sup- gaged, 3% Doubtful, 3% Dismissive. This pattern is similar to this age
ported by work examining informal scientific educators' concerns group in the American public (Leiserowitz et al., 2015) and in
about incorporating climate change into their education curriculum subsequent tests in the same participant pool (Geiger & Swim,
(Swim & Fraser, 2013, 2014). The more concerned educators were 2014). Yet, despite this majority concern, in a second pilot test
about being able to competently communicate about climate (N ¼ 89), only 30% of respondents accurately perceived that a
change the more likely they were to avoid extensively communi- majority of other students were concerned about climate change.
cating with visitors about this topic. The most common misperceptions were that: 1) most un-
Given that even trained scientific educators express concern dergraduates were disengaged with the topic, 2) most un-
about being capable of communicating climate change, non- dergraduates were doubtful about climate change, or 3)
scientists may be even more likely to hold these concerns. Research undergraduates' opinions were polarized.
82 N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90
6.1.2. Procedure
6.1.4. Survey measures
Approximately six weeks after completing pre-screening mea-
The survey measures were assessed six weeks after the pre-
sures, participants were directed to an online survey. Participants
screening measures detailed above.
were instructed to imagine the following scenario:
You are assigned a group class project in your Intro to English 6.1.4.1. Anticipated evaluations by others (proposed mediators).
class at Penn State. You are in a group with 4 other people, and Participants rated their expectations of being perceived as warm
you decide to meet together in the library to work on this (friendly, fun, good-natured, likeable, nice; a ¼ 0.89) and compe-
project. After a few productive hours of work, your group begins tent (competent, responsible, intelligent, level-headed, successful;
to get distracted. The topic of the weather comes up. You are a ¼ 0.76) (each on a 2 “Very Unlikely” to 2 “Very Likely” scale) if
N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90 83
Fig. 1. Opinion distribution answer choices in Study 1. Those who chose Option A were not recruited to participate in the study.
they were to talk about climate change. We also assessed expec- 6.2.2. Pluralistic ignorance and discussions
tations of appearing as a complainer, an alarmist, and an environ- Perceptions about undergraduates' opinions were associated
mentalist to analyze since expectations about being disliked (i.e., with willingness to discuss climate change, F(3, 301) ¼ 2.96,
perceived as cold) might be grounded in expectations about being p ¼ 0.03, h2 ¼ .03.1 Contrast tests compared those who accurately
categorized in one or more of these groups. Similar to expectations perceived that most undergraduates were concerned about climate
of being perceived as warm (as presented below), results show null change versus each of the other three possible inaccurate percep-
results for relationships between these variables and perceptions of tions. As hypothesized, those who inaccurately perceived that most
others' opinions. Thus, to simplify presentation of results, we do not undergraduates were doubtful about climate change were signifi-
present analyses related to these variables. cantly less willing to discuss climate change (M ¼ 0.85, S.d. ¼ 1.00)
than those who accurately perceived that most undergraduates
were concerned (M ¼ 0.23, S.d. ¼ 1.15), p ¼ 0.004, h2p ¼ 0.03. In
6.1.4.2. Willingness to discuss climate change (proposed outcome). contrast, those who inaccurately perceived that most un-
Following other research measuring willingness to discuss dergraduates were disengaged (M ¼ 0.40, S.d ¼ 1.11), or polarized
controversial topics (e.g. Noelle-Neumann, 1993; Scheufele, in their opinions (M ¼ 0.44, S.d ¼ 1.24), were not statistically less
Shanahan, & Lee, 2001), respondents answered the question, willing to discuss the topic than those with accurate opinions
“How likely would you be to discuss climate change in the above (ps > 0.25), although the difference was in the expected direction.
situation?” (2 “Very Unlikely” to 2 “Very Likely”). Responses were We conducted a separate ANOVA to examine whether percep-
normally distributed (skewness ¼ 0.23, kurtosis ¼ 1.00), sug- tions of the American public's opinions were related to willingness
gesting that ordinary least squares (OLS) regression techniques to discuss climate change. In contrast to the above findings with
would be appropriate for data analysis. perceptions of undergraduates' opinions, perceptions of the
American public's opinions were not related to willingness to talk
about climate change, F(3, 294) ¼ 0.37, p ¼ 0.77, h2 ¼ 0.004.
6.2. Results
6.2.1. Overview
6.2.3. Impression management
We used one-way ANOVAs to test whether perceptions of a)
Perceptions of other undergraduates' opinions were associated
other undergraduates' and b) the American public's opinions are
with expectations about appearing competent, F(3, 300) ¼ 4.31,
related to i) willingness to discuss climate change and ii) antici-
p ¼ 0.005, h2 ¼ 0.04, but not with expectations about appearing
pated evaluations by others. The independent variable corre-
sponded to the four opinion distributions the respondent chose for
the relevant target group. We next conducted mediation analyses 1
For this and all other analyses presented below, we examined whether results
to test whether anticipated evaluations mediated the relation be- were moderated by participants' personal opinion about climate change. We found
tween opinion perceptions and willingness to discuss climate no significant interaction effects in any analyses, ps > 0.17, perhaps partly due to the
change. restricted range of opinions among participants recruited to be in this sample.
84 N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90
warm, F(3, 300) ¼ 1.65, p ¼ 0.18, h2 ¼ 0.02. Contrast tests again omitted the variable corresponding to perceptions of the American
compared those who accurately perceived that most un- public's opinions from these analyses because the prior analyses
dergraduates were concerned about climate change versus each of showed that this variable was unrelated to the other variables in
the other three possible inaccurate perceptions. As hypothesized, the mediation analysis.
participants who believed that most undergraduates were doubtful As shown in Fig. 2, those who believed most other students
about climate change expected to appear less competent (M ¼ 0.46, doubted climate change were less willing to discuss the topic than
S.d. ¼ 0.65) than undergraduates who held more accurate percep- those who believed most other students were concerned because
tions (M ¼ 0.81, S.d. ¼ 0.52), p ¼ 0.001, h2p ¼ 0.04. Undergraduates the former expected to be perceived as less competent in a dis-
who held the more accurate perception also perceived that others cussion than did the latter, indirect effect ¼ 0.09, SE ¼ 0.06, 95% CI
would perceive them as more competent than those who believed [0.01, 0.25], while expectations of appearing warm did not play a
others were disengaged (M ¼ 0.60, S.d ¼ 0.54), p ¼ 0.01, h2p ¼ 0.02, role in this process, indirect effect ¼ 0.01, SE ¼ 0.03, 95% CI [0.02,
but not more than undergraduates who believed others were 0.11].
mostly polarized, (M ¼ 0.69, S.d. ¼ 0.53), p ¼ 0.16.
We conducted separate ANOVAs to examine whether percep- 6.3. Discussion
tions of the American public's opinions were related to expecta-
tions of appearing warm or competent. In contrast to perceptions of Among students who do not doubt climate change, those who
other undergraduates' opinions, perceptions of the American endorse the misperception that most others doubt climate change
public's opinions were not related to expectations of appearing were less likely to engage in discussions about climate change than
competent, F(3, 294) ¼ 2.02, p ¼ 0.11, h2 ¼ 0.02, or expectations of those who held accurate perceptions of others' opinions. In
appearing warm, F(3, 294) ¼ 0.09, p ¼ 0.97, h2 ¼ .001. contrast, those who held other types of inaccurate perceptionsd-
that others are disengaged or polarizedewere not significantly less
6.2.4. Mediation analyses willing to discuss climate change than those who held the accurate
We used PROCESS with 5000 bootstraps to conduct a perception. The lack of statistical significance in differences be-
regression-based parallel mediation analysis (Hayes, 2013, Model tween these two groups and those who believed others were
4) testing whether the observed difference between those who concerned prevented us from making any conclusions about dif-
believed other students were concerned and those who believed ferences among these three groups. However, examining the mean
other students were doubtful was explained by perceptions of values for each of the four groups leads to the speculation that the
appearing warm or competent. We created the following dummy effect on perceptions of others' opinions on self-silencing is a
code based on participants' perceptions of other students to test matter of degree: the more an individual's perception diverges
this relationship: concerned ¼ 0, disinterested ¼ 0, polarized ¼ 0, from the reality that others share their concern about climate
doubtful ¼ 1 (entered as the independent variable) and included change, the more hesitant they may be to speak up. Future research
the following two other dummy coded variables as covariates in the could consider assessing perceptions of others' opinions as a
mediation model to control for other possibilities examined in the continuous variable to examine whether the relationship between
ANOVA: a) concerned ¼ 0, disinterested ¼ 1, polarized ¼ 0, perceptions of others' opinions and self-silencing best fits a linear
doubtful ¼ 0 and b) concerned ¼ 0, disinterested ¼ 0, pattern or if there is a certain tipping point upon which individuals
polarized ¼ 1, doubtful ¼ 0 (see Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, become more likely to self-silence.
2003). We entered a) perceptions of appearing warm and b) per- Results also show that the reason that those who believe that
ceptions of appearing competent as parallel mediators and will- most others doubt climate change are less willing to discuss the
ingness to discuss climate change as the dependent variable. We topic than those who accurately perceive others' opinions is
Fig. 2. Expectations of appearing competent and warm as mediators of the effects between the perceived opinions of others and willingness to discuss climate change in Study 1.
Higher values on Perceived Opinions of Others indicate more accurate perceptions of others' opinions. See text for a full description of how these variables were entered into the
model. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05; þ p ¼ 0.056.
N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90 85
because the former expect to lose respect (appear less competent) In addition to these added dimensions, we also altered a key
in a discussion, and not because they expect to be disliked aspect of the design from Study 1 to increase the external validity of
(perceived as less warm). This suggests that many individuals are our study: in contrast to the hypothetical situation used in Study 1,
uncomfortable engaging in discussions about climate change with a participants learned that they would actually speak about climate
dissenting audience because they believe that they do not have the change in a classroom setting. This has been shown to enhance the
ability to do so, rather than being concerned about appearing so- validity of findings related to willingness to discuss a topic
cially deviant or hostile. (Scheufele et al., 2001).
Table 1
Effects of others' alleged opinions, changes in others alleged opinions over time, and participants' own opinions on dependent measures (Study 2).
simplify the presentation. them as more competent in the discussion when they believed
others shared their opinions (vs. did not share their opinions)
7.1.3.3. Willingness to discuss climate change. Similar to Study 1, (Fig. 3). In contrast, being led to believe that concern was increasing
Study 2 participants answered the question: “Please indicate your (vs. decreasing) did not influence expectations of appearing
willingness to participate in the group discussion on climate competent and this effect was not moderated by participants'
change,” (2 “Very Hesitant” to 2 “Very Willing”). Again, this var- concern about the topic, ps > 0.10.
iable was normally distributed (skewness ¼ 0.34, The effect of others' alleged opinions about climate change on
kurtosis ¼ 0.23), suggesting that OLS regression techniques were expectations of appearing warm was marginally moderated by
appropriate for data analysis. participants' own concern about the topic, b ¼ .18, SE ¼ 0.10,
p ¼ 0.08.2 As before, students being led to believe that concern was
7.2. Results increasing (vs. decreasing) did not predict expectations of appear-
ing warm and this null relationship was not moderated by partic-
7.2.1. Overview ipants' concern about the topic, ps > 0.10.
We first tested the hypotheses that the effects of both experi-
mental manipulations would be moderated by personal opinions 7.2.4. Mediation analyses
about climate change. We conducted a three-step hierarchical We conducted a conditional mediation analysis (Hayes, 2013,
regression procedure (Cohen et al., 2003) to examine main effects model 8 with 5000 bootstraps) to explore whether the interaction
and interactions, entering a) students' willingness to discuss between others' alleged opinions and personal opinions on will-
climate change and b) anticipated evaluations by others were ingness to discuss climate change was mediated by expectations of
regressed on i) others' current alleged opinions, ii) whether par- appearing competent or warm. As shown in Fig. 4, expectations
ticipants were informed that concern was increasing or decreasing, about appearing competent, but not expectations of appearing
and iii) personal opinions about climate change in Step 1, all two- warm, mediated the relation between the interaction between
way interactions in Step 2, and the three-way interaction (for undergraduates' opinions and of others' alleged opinions and
exploratory purposes) in Step 3 (see Table 1). We next conducted a willingness to discuss climate change. Spotlight analyses revealed
conditional mediation analysis to examine whether the mediation that those who were concerned about climate change were more
findings in Study 1 would replicate for those upon both ends of the willing to discuss climate change when they were led to believe
opinion spectrum. that others were concerned (vs. not concerned) because they
believed they would be perceived as more competent when others
shared their views, indirect effect b ¼ 0.06, SE ¼ 0.05, 95% CI [0.004,
7.2.2. Pluralistic ignorance and discussions
0.156]. In contrast, those who were doubtful about climate change
The effect of others' alleged opinions about climate change on
were less willing to discuss climate change when they believed
willingness to discuss climate change was moderated by partici-
others were concerned (vs. not concerned) because they believed
pants' own concern about the topic, b ¼ 0.41, SE ¼ 0.16, p ¼ 0.01
they would be perceived as less competent when others disagreed
(Table 1). As predicted, undergraduates were more willing to
with their views, indirect effect b ¼ 0.15, SE ¼ 0.10, 95% CI
discuss the topic when they were led to believe others would share
[0.36, .02].
their opinion than when they were led to believe they would not
(see Fig. 3). The effect of the manipulation was of similar magnitude
7.3. Discussion
(but of opposite direction) for participants of opposing opinions.
In contrast, students who were led to believe that concern was
Results replicate and extend Study 1 findings that concerned
increasing (vs. decreasing) were no more willing to discuss climate
participants are less willing to discuss climate change when they
change and this effect was not moderated by participants' own
believe others do not share their concern (vs. share their concern).
concern about the topic, ps > 0.10.
Study 2 results extend this finding by revealing that this effect
applies to those upon both ends of the opinion spectrum. Similarly,
7.2.3. Impression management
The effect of others' alleged opinions about climate change on
expectations of appearing competent was moderated by partici- 2
As anticipated, this trend was driven by participants expecting that others
pants' own concern about the topic, b ¼ .23, SE ¼ 0.09, p ¼ 0.01. As would perceive them as warmer when they believed that others shared their
hypothesized, participants expected that others would perceive opinions (vs. did not share their opinions).
N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90 87
Fig. 3. Effect of perceived opinion climate on a) willingness to discuss climate change and b) expectations of being perceived as competent among participants with different
personal opinions about climate change. (Study 2). Values, simple difference tests and standard error bars depicted in the figure were assessed using simple slopes analyses.
Study 2 results replicate and extend Study 1 findings that con- with those who disagree (vs. agree) with them. Study 2 reveals that
cerned participants self-silence because they expect to be this effect occurs for those upon both ends of the opinion spectrum.
perceived as less competent in a conversation about climate change Study 2 further reveals that self-silencing among those concerned
Fig. 4. Expectations of appearing competent and warm as mediators of the effects between opinion congruence and willingness to discuss climate change in Study 2. Perceived
Opinions of Others Personal Opinion reflects the degree to which one's opinion is congruent with perceptions of others' opinions, with higher values reflecting higher perceived
opinion congruence than lower values. **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.
88 N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90
about climate change can be countered by providing accurate in- 8.1. Impression management and self-silencing
formation: those who hold the majority opinion are more willing to
share their opinion when pluralistic ignorance is corrected (vs. The present research indicates that impression management
exacerbated) by providing accurate information. In contrast, this concerns about losing respect (appearing less competent), but not
accurate information is likely to silence the minority if they pre- concerns about being disliked (appearing less warm), explain self-
viously believed that their view was more common than it actually silencing on the topic of climate change. These results differ from
was. previous research on confronting prejudice which revealed the
In contrast to our predictions, there were no effects related to silencing effect of concerns about being disliked (e.g., Shelton &
whether participants were led to believe concern was increasing Stewart, 2004; Swim & Hyers, 1999; Woodzicka & LaFrance,
(vs. decreasing). These results suggest that perceptions that others' 2001). As we describe in the introduction, impression manage-
opinions are changing to become more (vs. less) like one's own ment concerns related to not being respected may play a role in
opinion do not appear to influence willingness to speak up. Instead, self-silencing on climate change due to the scientific nature of the
perceptions that others' current opinions differ from one's own topic.
reduce willingness to engage in discussion. These impression management concerns suggest that
improving individuals' confidence in their ability to talk about
8. General discussion climate change (i.e., self-efficacy) could also counteract the negative
effects of perceiving themselves to be in the minority by allowing
The present research demonstrates the effects of pluralistic individuals to overcome expectations of appearing incompetent.
ignorance in promoting public silence on the socially relevant This idea is supported by correlational research suggesting that
topic of climate change. Study 1 results reveal the costs of those with greater self-efficacy about their ability to discuss climate
pluralistic ignorance on discussion about climate change among change discuss the topic more frequently (Swim et al., 2014) and
those who do not doubt the science. Survey respondents who did experimental research showing that watching short videos about
not themselves doubt climate change were less willing to discuss climate change which increase individuals' self-efficacy about their
the topic when they inaccurately believed fellow students would ability to contribute to conversations bolster willingness to engage
not share their opinion than when they accurately perceived they in discussions about the topic (Geiger, Swim, & Fraser, 2016). In-
were in the majority. Study 2 results show that when accurate dividuals could potentially increase their confidence about dis-
portrayals of others' beliefs were presented, those who were cussing climate change through learning about the mechanisms by
concerned about climate change were more willing to discuss the which climate change is occurring (e.g., Ramney & Clark, 2016) or
topic relative to concerned individuals led to inaccurately believe receiving information that most scientists agree that human caused
that others would not share their views. Both studies show that climate change is occurring (e.g., Cook & Lewandowsky, 2016),
the reason individuals are more willing to discuss climate change possibilities which could be tested in future research. Confidence
when they perceive that others agree than when they perceive could also be obtained via observing others competently discuss
that others disagree is because they expected to be respected climate change. These role models could decrease concerns about
more (i.e., appear more competent) by the former audience. In appearing incompetent because they could illustrate how to talk
contrast, expectations of being liked (i.e., appearing warm) do not about climate change (Geiger et al., 2016). Observing high status
play a role facilitating discussion in either study. individuals discuss climate change could be another potential
Our findings have practical implications for those who seek to buffer to expectations of appearing incompetent. Incorporating
facilitate greater public engagement with climate change among more high-status characters who discuss climate change into the
the majority of the population that express concern about climate media (e.g., Cameron, Weintraub, & Schwarzenegger, 2014;
change. One way to promote discussion is to correct pluralistic Cornwell, Bajger, & Higgins, 2015) or promoting greater public
ignorance, informing them that a majority of others share their discussion among those holding positions of power (Corner, 2014)
concern. In contrast, correcting pluralistic ignorance is likely to could encourage others to follow their lead. Last, altered social
reduce discussion among those who are not concerned about norms could encourage conversations about climate change if the
climate change. Thus, while the correction of pluralistic ignorance norms promoted discussion about aspects of the topic that were
could increase participation by the silenced majority group it could more accessible to nonscientists, thus increasing individuals' self-
decrease participation by those holding minority opinions if the efficacy about contributing to these conversations.
minority opinion holders previously believed that they were in the
majority, which may not be desirable to those interested in hearing 8.2. Future directions
what individuals who reject the scientific consensus believe.
Consistent with previous research (Larimer, 2010; Oshagan, Future research might address alternate contexts in which ex-
1996), our results suggest that correcting pluralistic ignorance pectations of being disliked following an interaction would play a
may only be effective at facilitating discussion when the opinions of role in silencing. In the present research, Study 1 involved a hy-
the audience with whom one will discuss the topic are made pothetical situation and Study 2 involved a setting where partici-
salient, rather than the opinions of the general American public. pants did not anticipate repeated interactions with the audience in
This points to a practical weakness of accurately informing in- the future. Expectations of being disliked may play a greater role in
dividuals of others' opinions: given geographic and political po- self-silencing in situations where extended contact with others is
larization of climate change concern (Howe, Mildenbarger, Marlon expected. Contexts more similar to those found in the prejudice
& Leiserowitz, 2015), in some subgroups a majority of people are literature could also lead to increased salience of expectations of
unconcerned about climate change. Thus, informing members of being disliked. For instance, in some contexts speaking up about
these subgroups that a majority of others are concerned about climate change would require confronting another's environmen-
climate change may be unbelievable or may be disconfirmed if they tally harmful behavior. In this context, individuals may self-silence
talk about climate change within their subgroup. In these situa- out of concern of being appearing hostile and unfriendly (i.e., cold).
tions, it may be more effective to instead focus on decreasing ex- Individuals may also expect to be disliked or rejected from a group
pectations about losing respect from these audiences in a if speaking up required directly arguing with an in-group member
discussion, as we describe below. who denies the existence of anthropogenic climate change.
N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90 89
Future research might examine whether individual differences Foundation, Grant Number DUE-1239775 to NEAq. Portions of this
moderate effects. Those who are high self-monitors (Snyder, 1974), paper were presented at the German Environmental Association
for instance, may be more likely than others to self-silence due to a Conference in Magdeburg, Germany.
greater tendency to attend to audience opinions. Participant age We would like to thank Gamliel Sassoon and Kayla Warner for
might also moderate the findings: older participants may have their assistance in collecting data and Stephan Lewandowsky and
more practice negotiating difficult interactions or be less influenced two anonymous reviewers for their feedback on the paper.
by peer pressure than the university age students in the present
sample (Sears, 1986). The ubiquity of climate change silence among
References
a variety of demographics (Norgaard, 2011) and Study 2 results
showing that pluralistic ignorance can lead to silencing for those on Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of
both ends of the opinion spectrum suggests that our results may self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: a theoretical analysis
generalize to other demographics not captured in the present
and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888.
samples. Bashir, N. Y., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A. L., Nadolny, D., & Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic
More detailed assessments of self-silencing should also be impact of activists: negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. Euro-
considered in future research to gain a broader insight into this pean Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 614e626.
Bassili, J. N. (2003). The minority slowness effect: subtle inhibitions in the
process. Lack of discussion about climate change could be an expression of views not shared by others. Journal of Personality and Social
intentional choice to not speak up, or simply indicate lack of per- Psychology, 84, 261e276.
sonal interest in discussing the topic, a contrast which should be Bergsieker, H. B., Shelton, J. N., & Richeson, J. A. (2010). To be liked versus respected:
divergent goals in interracial interactions. Journal of Personality and Social
teased apart in future research. Future research could also assess Psychology, 99(2), 248.
self-silencing using behavioral measures, such as response latency Bunten, A., & Arvizu, S. (2013). Turning visitors into citizens: using social science for
e the tendency to hesitate before sharing one's opinion (Bassili, civic engagement in informal science education centers. Journal of Museum
Education, 38(3), 260e272.
2003; Rios & Chen, 2014). Response latency could be practically Cameron, J., Weintraub, T., & Schwarzenegger, A. (2014). Years of living dangerously.
important to examine because subtle temporal delays can alter the Electus international.
tone of interactions (Pearson et al., 2008). Using behavioral mea- Capstick, S. B., Demski, C. C., Sposato, R. G., Pidgeon, N. F., Spence, A., & Corner, A.
(2015). Public perceptions of climate change in Britain following the winter 2013/
sures also hold benefits related to not relying on self-report 2014 flooding. Working paper retrieved from http://c3wales.org/wp-content/
measures. uploads/2015/01/URG-15-01-Flood-Climate-report-final2.pdf.
Alternative methods for assessing perceptions of others' opin- Clayton, S., Devine-Wright, P., Stern, P. C., Whitmarsh, L., Carrico, A., Steg, L., et al.
(2015). Psychological research and global climate change. Nature Climate
ions could also be assessed in future research. As we note in section
Change, 5(7), 640e646.
5.3, these perceptions could be assessed as a continuous measure to Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple correlation/
determine whether the relationship between opinion perceptions regression analysis for the behavioral sciences. New York: Routledge.
and willingness to discuss climate change is linear or whether self- Cook, J., & Lewandowsky, S. (2016). Rational irrationality: modeling climate change
belief polarization using Bayesian networks. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1),
silencing primarily occurs only when individuals believe that a 160e179.
majority of others hold opinions directly opposed to their own. Cook, J., Nuccitelli, D., Green, S. A., Richardson, M., Winkler, B., Painting, R., …
These perceptions could also be assessed using the decision by Skuce, A. (2013). Quantifying the consensus on anthropogenic global warming
in the scientific literature. Environmental Research Letters, 8(2), 024024.
sampling model (Stewart, Chater, & Brown, 2006) in which in- Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P. T., Anderegg, W. R., Verheggen, B., Maib
dividuals consider what percentage of relevant others are ach, E. W., … Nuccitelli, D. (2016). Consensus on consensus:a synthesis of
perceived to be less concerned about climate change than they are consensus estimates on human-caused global warming. Environmental Research
Letters, 11(4), 048002.
by comparing themselves to each relevant other (cf. Wood, Brown, Corner, A. (2014). Climate Silence and how to break it. http://www.climateoutreach.
& Maltby, 2012). Wood et al. (2012) found that using this approach org.uk/portfolio-item/climate-silence-and-how-to-break-it/.
predicts perceived riskiness of individuals drinking behavior more Cornwell, J. F., Bajger, A. T., & Higgins, E. T. (2015). Judging political hearts and
minds: how political dynamics drive social judgments. Personality and Social
strongly than participants' perceptions of how their drinking
Psychology Bulletin, 41(8), 1053e1068.
behavior compares to the “average.” Dietz, T. (2013). Bringing values and deliberation to science communication. Pro-
ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(Suppl. 3), 14081e14087.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition:
8.3. Conclusion
warmth and competence. Trends in cognitive sciences, 11(2), 77e83.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype
The present research demonstrates that pluralistic ignorance content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status
can be a barrier to discussions about climate change among those and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878.
Fiske, S. T., Xu, J., Cuddy, A. C., & Glick, P. (1999). (Dis) respecting versus (dis) liking:
most concerned about climate change and this barrier can be status and interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and
removed by correcting this pluralistic ignorance. Our findings warmth. Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 473e489.
advance the theoretical understanding of self-silencing, showing Geiger, N., & Swim, J. K. (2014). A single-item alternative to the six Americas
screening instrument. In Poster presented at Society for Personality and Social
that with regard to this topic, pluralistic ignorance hinders dis- Psychology Convention, Sustainability Preconference.
cussions because individuals expect to be respected less by a Geiger, N., Swim, J. K., & Fraser, J. (2016). Promoting public discussions about climate
dissenting audience, rather than expectations of being disliked by change. In Paper presented at the Society for the Psychological Study of Social
Issues, Minneapolis, MN.
those who disagree. This suggests that building resilience to Glynn, C. J., & Huge, M. E. (2014). Speaking in spirals:an updated meta-analysis of
communicating with audiences of differing opinions may be facil- the spiral of silence. In W. Donsbach, C. T. Salmon, & Y. Tsfati (Eds.), The spiral of
itated by improving communication skills or boosting efficacy Silence: New perspectives on communication and public opinion (pp. 65e72).
Routledge.
about discussing climate change (Swim et al., 2014; Geiger et al., Goldenberg, S. (2015). US Senate refuses to accept humanity's role in global climate
2016). Indeed, a large-scale effort is currently underway to change, again. The Guardian. Retrieved from http://www.theguardian.com/
develop communication skills and efficacy among interpreters at environment/2015/jan/22/us-senate-man-climate-change-global-warming-
hoax.
and visitors to aquariums and zoos around the US (Bunten & Arvizu,
Goode, C., Balzarini, R. H., & Smith, H. J. (2014). Positive peer pressure: priming
2013; Geiger et al., 2016; Swim & Fraser, 2013, 2014). member prototypicality can decrease undergraduate drinking. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 44(8), 567e578. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12248.
Acknowledgements Habermas, J. (1971). Toward a rational society: Student protest, science, and politics
(Vol. 404). Beacon Press.
Hayes, A. F. (2013). An introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process
This investigation was supported by National Science analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.
90 N. Geiger, J.K. Swim / Journal of Environmental Psychology 47 (2016) 79e90
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley. campus: some consequences of misperceiving the social norm. Journal of Per-
Holoien, D. S., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Downplaying positive impressions: compensa- sonality and Social Psychology, 64(2), 243.
tion between warmth and competence in impression management. Journal of Ranney, M. A., & Clark, D. (2016). Climate change conceptual change: scientific in-
Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 33e41. formation can transform attitudes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 49e75.
Howe, P. D., Mildenberger, M., Marlon, J. R., & Leiserowitz, A. (2015). Geographic Rios, K., & Chen, Z. (2014). Experimental evidence for minorities' hesitancy in
variation in opinions on climate change at state and local scales in the USA. reporting their opinions the roles of optimal distinctiveness needs and
Nature Climate Change, 5(6), 596e603. normative influence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(7), 872e883.
IPCC. (2014). In Core Writing Team, R. K. Pachauri, & L. A. Meyer (Eds.), Climate Rosenberg, S., Nelson, C., & Vivekananthan, P. (1968). A multidimensional approach
change 2014: Synthesis report. Contribution of working groups I, II and III to the to the structure of personality impressions. Journal of Personality and Social
fifth assessment report of the intergovernmental panel on climate change (p. 151). Psychology, 9, 283e294.
Geneva, Switzerland: IPCC. Rowson, J. (2013). Facing up to stealth denial and winding down on fossil fuels.
Jacobson, M. Z., & Delucchi, M. A. (2011). Providing all global energy with wind, Retrieved from https://www.thersa.org/discover/publications-and-articles/
water, and solar power, Part I: technologies, energy resources, quantities and reports/a-new-agenda-on-climate-change/.
areas of infrastructure, and materials. Energy Policy, 39(3), 1154e1169. Salmon, C. T., & Kline, F. G. (1985). The spiral of silence ten years later: an exami-
Kaiser, C. R., & Miller, C. T. (2001). Stop complaining! the social costs of making nation and evaluation. In K. R. Sanders, L. L. Kaid, & D. Nimmo (Eds.), Political
attributions to discrimination. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(2), communication yearbook, 1984 (pp. 3e30). Carbondale: Southern Illinois Uni-
254e263. versity Press.
Lambert, T. A., Kahn, A. S., & Apple, K. J. (2003). Pluralistic ignorance and hooking Salmon, C. T., & Neuwirth, K. (1990). Perceptions of opinion “climates” and will-
up. Journal of Sex Research, 40(2), 129e133. ingness to discuss the issue of abortion. Journalism & Mass Communication
Larimer, M. E. (2010). Group identification as a moderator of the relationship be- Quarterly, 67(3), 567e577.
tween perceived social norms and alcohol consumption. Psychology of Addictive Scheufele, D. A., Shanahan, J., & Lee, E. (2001). Real talk manipulating the dependent
Behaviors, 24(3), 522e528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0019944. variable in spiral of silence research. Communication Research, 28(3), 304e324.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G., & Rosenthal, S. (2015). Schlenker, B. R. (1975). Self-presentation:managing the impression of consistency
Climate change in the American mind: March, 2015. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni- when reality interferes with self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social
versity and George Mason University (Yale Project on Climate Change Psychology, 32(6), 1030.
Communication). Schroeder, C. M., & Prentice, D. A. (1998). Exposing pluralistic ignorance to reduce
Leiserowitz, A., Smith, N., & Marlon, J. R. (2010). Americans' knowledge of climate alcohol use among college students. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28(23),
change. New Haven, CT: Yale University. Yale Project on Climate Change 2150e2180.
Communication http://environment.yale.edu/climate/files/ Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: influences of a narrow
ClimateChangeKnowledge2010.pdf. data base on social psychology's view of human nature. Journal of Personality
Leviston, Z., Walker, I., & Morwinski, S. (2013). Your opinion on climate change and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515.
might not be as common as you think. Nature Climate Change, 3, 334e337. Sechrist, G. B., & Stangor, C. (2001). Perceived consensus influences intergroup
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. E. (2013). NASA faked the moon behavior and stereotype accessibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
landingdtherefore,(climate) science is a hoax an anatomy of the motivated ogy, 80(4), 645e654.
rejection of science. Psychological Science, 24(5), 622e633. Sechrist, G. B., Swim, J. K., & Stangor, C. (2004). When do the stigmatized make
Lewandowsky, S., Oreskes, N., Risbey, J. S., Newell, B. R., & Smithson, M. (2015). attributions to discrimination occurring to the self and others? The roles of self-
Seepage: climate change denial and its effect on the scientific community. presentation and need for control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Global Environmental Change, 2015(33), 1e13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 87(1), 111.
j.gloenvcha.2015.02.013. Shelton, J. N., & Stewart, R. E. (2004). Confronting perpetrators of prejudice: the
Maibach, E., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., & Mertz, C. K. (2011). Identifying like- inhibitory effects of social costs. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28(3), 215e223.
minded audiences for global warming public engagement campaigns: an Singh, R., Ho, L., Tan, H. L., & Bell, P. A. (2007). Attitudes, personal evaluations,
audience segmentation analysis and tool development. PloS One, 6(3), e17571. cognitive evaluation and interpersonal attraction:on the direct, indirect and
Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz. (2009). Global Warming's six Americas reverse-causal effects. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46(1), 19e42.
2009: An audience segmentation analysis. http://www.climatechangeco Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
mmunication.org/images/files/GlobalWarmingsSixAmericas2009c.pdf. Social Psychology, 30(4), 526.
Mallett, R. K., & Melchiori, K. J. (2014). Goal preference shapes confrontations of Stangor, C., Swim, J. K., Sechrist, G. B., DeCoster, J., Van Allen, K. L., et al. (2003). Ask,
sexism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(5), 646e656. http://dx.doi. answer and announce: three stages in perceiving and responding to discrimi-
org/10.1177/0146167214521468. nation. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 277e311.
Markowitz, E. M. (2012). Is climate change an ethical issue? Examining young Stewart, N., Chater, N., & Brown, G. D. A. (2006). Decision by sampling. Cognitive
adults' beliefs about climate and morality. Climatic Change, 114(3e4), 479e495. Psychology, 53, 1e26.
Marshall, G. (2014). Don't even think about it: Why our brains are wired to ignore Swim, J. K., Eyssell, K. M., & Murdoch, E. Q. (2010). Self-silencing to sexism. Journal of
climate change. USA: Bloomsbury Publishing. Social Issues, 66(3), 493e507.
Miller, D. T., & McFarland, C. (1987). Pluralistic ignorance: when similarity is Swim, J. K., & Fraser, J. (2013). Fostering hope in climate change educators. Journal of
interpreted as dissimilarity. Journal of Personality and social Psychology, 53(2), Museum Education, 38(3), 286e297.
298. Swim, J., & Fraser, J. (2014). Zoo and aquarium professionals' concerns and confi-
Moy, P., Domke, D., & Stamm, K. (2001). The spiral of silence and public opinion on dence about climate change education. Journal of Geoscience Education, 62(3),
affirmative action. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 78(1), 7e25. 495e501.
Neighbors, C., Labrie, J. W., Hummer, J. F., Lewis, M. A., Lee, C. M., Swim, J. K., Fraser, J., & Geiger, N. (2014). Teaching the choir to sing: use of social
Sruti, D., … Larimer, M. E. (2010). Group identification as a moderator of the science information to promote public discourse on climate change. Journal of
relationship between perceived social norms and alcohol consumption. Psy- Land Use & Environmental Law, 30(1), 91e117.
chology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(3), 522e528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ Swim, J. K., & Geiger, N. (2016b). Perceptions of the prototypical six Americas:
a0019944. Gendered and valenced attributes (Unpublished data).
Noelle-Neumann, E. (1993). The spiral of silence: Public opinion e our social skin (2nd Swim, J. K., Gervais, S. J., Pearson, N., & Stangor, C. (2009). Managing the message:
ed.). Chicago/London: University of Chicago Press. using social influence and attitude change. Strategies to confront interpersonal
Norgaard, K. M. (2011). Living in denial: Climate change, emotions, and everyday life. discrimination. In F. Butera, & J. M. Levine (Eds.), Coping with minority Status:
The MIT Press. Responses to exclusion and inclusion. New York: Cambridge University Press.
O'Gorman, H. J., & Garry, S. L. (1976). Pluralistic ignoranceda replication and Swim, J. K., & Hyers, L. L. (1999). Excuse medwhat did you just say?!: women's
extension. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40(4), 449e458. public and private responses to sexist remarks. Journal of Experimental Social
Oreskes, N. (2004). The scientific consensus on climate change. Science, 306(5702), Psychology, 35(1), 68e88.
1686. Taylor, D. G. (1982). Pluralistic ignorance and the spiral of silence: a formal analysis.
Oshagan, H. (1996). Reference group influence on opinion expression. International Public Opinion Quarterly, 46(3), 311e335.
Journal of Public Opinion Research, 8(4), 335e354. Todorov, A., & Mandisodza, A. N. (2004). Public opinion on foreign policy: the
Pearson, A. R., West, T. V., Dovidio, J. F., Powers, S. R., Buck, R., & Henning, R. (2008). multilateral public that perceives itself as unilateral. Public Opinion Quarterly,
The fragility of intergroup relations: divergent effects of delayed audiovisual 68(3), 323e348.
feedback in intergroup and intragroup interaction. Psychological Science, 19(12), Wood, A. M., Brown, G. D., & Maltby, J. (2012). Social norm influences on evaluations
1272e1279. of the risks associated with alcohol consumption: applying the rank-based
Petri
c, G., & Pinter, A. (2002). From social perception to public expression of decision by sampling model to health judgments. Alcohol and alcoholism,
opinion: a structural equation modeling approach to the spiral of silence. In- 47(1), 57e62.
ternational Journal of Public Opinion Research, 14(1), 37e53. Woodzicka, J., & LaFrance, A. (2001). Marianne. Real versus imagined gender
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1993). Pluralistic ignorance and alcohol use on Harassment. Journal of Social Issues, 57(1), 15e30. Blackwell Publishers.