Article_AModelForCognitively-basedComp
Article_AModelForCognitively-basedComp
https://doi.org/10.1057/s41285-019-00124-x
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Abstract
Across cultures and belief systems, compassion is widely considered to be beneficial
for the development of personal and social wellbeing. Research indicates that com-
passion-training programs have broad health benefits, but how and why compassion-
training programs are effective is still relatively unknown. This paper describes the
theoretical underpinnings of a specific compassion-training program, CBCT® (Cog-
nitively-Based Compassion Training), and proposes an integrative model that draws
on existing health behavior constructs to identify CBCT’s core components and
hypothesizes their directionality and interaction. The model includes two primary
categories of skill development: (1) intrapersonal skills leading to greater resiliency,
and (2) interpersonal skills leading to greater compassion. It is hypothesized that
these two pathways are mutually reinforcing and both contribute to greater wellbe-
ing. This model provides a foundation for theory-driven research on the underlying
mechanisms in CBCT training. An understanding of CBCT’s mechanisms is a criti-
cal step towards optimizing and personalizing the intervention to meet the needs of
specific populations.
Introduction
The purpose of this paper is to provide a conceptual model that explains how
the components of Cognitively-Based Compassion Training (CBCT®) lead to
greater resilience, compassion, and wellbeing. There is a compelling body of
evidence to support the role of compassion in improving health and wellbeing
(Hofmann et al. 2011; Kirby 2017; Seppala et al. 2017). Prior studies suggest
* Marcia Ash
[email protected]
Extended author information available on the last page of the article
Vol.:(0123456789)
M. Ash et al.
that compassion appears to be a trainable skill; yet methods for cultivating com-
passion vary across compassion-based protocols. Some compassion-training pro-
grams use a process of cognitive reframing to cultivate a wish to see others free
from suffering (Jazaieri et al. 2012; Ozawa-de Silva and Dodson-Lavelle 2011).
Other related practices include loving-kindness meditations (LKM) which involve
a repeated process of wishing others to be happy (Shonin et al. 2014). Current
compassion-training programs, including CBCT (Pace et al. 2009), Compassion
Cultivation Training (Jazaieri et al. 2012), Mindful Self-Compassion (Neff and
Germer 2013), Compassion-Focused Therapy (Gilbert 2014), and Mindfulness-
Based Compassionate Living (Schuling et al. 2018), have been conceptualized
under broad frameworks (e.g., compassion meditation (CM)) with modest empha-
sis on describing the specific practices and pedagogical models.
A recent meta-analysis of existing compassion-based interventions empha-
sized the need for thorough articulations of the theoretical underpinnings of cur-
rent compassion interventions (Kirby 2017). There are relatively few theoretical
accounts that address the relationship among key concepts and practices that are
critical to the science of compassion training (for examples of existing theoreti-
cal accounts, see Mascaro et al. 2015). Just as different types of physical exercise
result in measurably distinct changes to the body, one would expect that different
practices of cultivating compassion may lead to identifiably distinct outcomes.
While CBCT has been studied across diverse populations and settings (Desbordes
et al. 2012; LoParo et al. 2018; Mascaro et al. 2013; Pace et al. 2009; Reddy et al.
2012), no synthesis is published which provides a comprehensive overview of the
CBCT protocol and its theoretical underpinnings. This paper seeks to fill this gap
by providing an overview of the theory and practices guiding CBCT.
While CBCT has been previously shown to be an effective intervention to
reduce depression and other negative health symptoms (Desbordes et al. 2012;
Mascaro et al. 2016), there is a lack of empirical research addressing the mecha-
nisms of change in a CBCT intervention. Here, we adopt the National Institute of
Health (NIH) Science of Behavior Change definition which refers to mechanisms
of behavior change as “malleable targets that play a role in initiating or main-
taining behavior change” (Nielsen et al. 2018). Mechanisms of behavior change
could take place at different levels of analysis including the psychological, social,
cultural or structural. In the case of the CBCT integrative model, ‘mechanisms of
change’ refer to the relationships among psychosocial skills developed through
participation in CBCT that are expected to lead to the outcomes of interest:
resilience, compassion, and wellbeing. The National Institute of Health (NIH)
Science of Behavior Change Program emphasizes the importance of utilizing
mechanism-focused methods to understand how and why interventions work to
effectively optimize and personalize behavioral interventions for specific popu-
lations (Nielsen et al. 2018). In keeping with that approach, this paper has two
aims: (1) to provide a comprehensive overview of the theoretical underpinnings
and practices of CBCT and (2) to propose a conceptual model that identifies the
psychosocial constructs that influence change mechanisms in CBCT training.
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
Defining compassion
CBCT development
CBCT was developed at Emory University in 2005 by Dr. Lobsang Tenzin Negi.
CBCT was created as a secular compassion-based training program adapted from the
Indo-Tibetan Buddhist traditions of lojong (mind training) and lamrim (the stages of
the path for spiritual development) (for an overview see Jinpa et al. 2014; Kyabgon
2007). The definition of compassion adopted in CBCT is in alignment with Indo-
Tibetan Buddhist conceptions of compassion (Dalai Lama 2005), though CBCT is
presented entirely in non-religious terms. CBCT was intentionally designed to be
accessible to persons of any or no faith tradition. Initially, the CBCT protocol con-
sisted of eight distinct modules (Negi 2005). The CBCT protocol has undergone
several revisions since its inception. Currently, the CBCT protocol includes a foun-
dational practice followed by six modules (see Table 1).
Each CBCT module employs a meditation exercise (also referred to as ‘con-
templative practice’) as the primary training strategy. The bulk of scientific
research on meditation has addressed mindfulness-based interventions such as
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
CBCT pedagogy
CBCT courses generally consist of 10 sessions taught over a 10-week period, meet-
ing 1.5 h per week. CBCT instructors complete a CBCT certification process that
includes a 1-week retreat and workshop, an 8-week seminar, and a 10-week super-
vised teaching assistantship. CBCT sessions include a pedagogical overview by the
certified instructor, guided meditations, and interactive exercises and discussion.
Participants are provided with guided meditation recordings for each module and are
M. Ash et al.
expected to meditate 15–20 min/day. While this is the standard format for a general,
adult CBCT course, the CBCT course structure and content have been adapted for
other settings and populations.
The CBCT learning process utilizes two meditation strategies: (1) stabilizing medi-
tation (sustained concentration) and (2) analytical meditation. Together, these strate-
gies support the building of skills across three levels: content knowledge, personal
insight, and embodied understanding. See Fig. 1.
Content knowledge
The first level of skill acquisition relies on learning from an outside source such as
reading, hearing, or seeing. At this stage, content is understood on an intellectual
level.
Personal insight
The second level of skill acquisition develops via a process of reflection, person-
alization, and critical thinking. In this process, one critically reflects on the content
knowledge relative to one’s own life experiences. The goal is to understand the con-
tent at a personal level, and the process is successful when it generates a deeply felt
sense of understanding.
instruction
• enduring motivation/commitment
embodied • sustained change strategies
understanding • embodied emotional/behavioral shifts
Embodied understanding
CBCT practices
The CBCT framework consists of a foundational practice and six modules that sys-
temically build upon and integrate skills from prior modules. Each module aims to
train specific skills, each of which are considered to be an active ingredient necessary
for the cultivation of extended compassion. The practice instructions for each module
are described below. A summary of the CBCT modules can be found in Table 1.
Module II practice utilizes attentional control skills cultivated in Module I and applies
them to a less-specific object of focus (i.e., no longer the sensations of the breath).
Participants are asked to observe sensations, thoughts, and feelings as they unfold
and change over time without getting caught up in particular emotions or thoughts.
The objective is to witness mental activity while resisting the urge to control, react, or
judge the content. When such reactivity is observed, the instruction is to release the
thought or feeling and return to observing present-moment experiences.
Module III is the first practice within the CBCT sequence in which participants are
prompted to engage actively in cognitive reframing via analytical meditation. The
goal of Module III is to strengthen a compassionate attitude towards oneself by
M. Ash et al.
After cultivating increased affection for others in Module VI and V, the Module VI
practice offers an opportunity to attune to the variety of ways others are suffering.
Participants are instructed to call to mind someone they see as vulnerable and allow
compassionate feelings to arise. Stabilizing techniques (developed in Module I and
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
II) are then employed to sustain and strengthen the arising feelings of compassion.
With practice, the compassionate feeling should deepen according to the CBCT pro-
cess of skill acquisition (Fig. 1) such that it becomes a more abiding disposition for
relating to others.
The CBCT integrative model (Fig. 2) depicts the core skills developed in each
CBCT module. The lines linking each construct in the model represent the proposed
mechanistic pathway by indicating the direction and interaction of skills developed
in each CBCT module and their contribution to the primary outcomes: resilience,
compassion, and wellbeing. The CBCT integrative model is divided in two prin-
cipal pathways: intrapersonal (Modules I–III) and interpersonal (Modules IV–VI).
Intrapersonal skills, primarily focused on self-regulation skills and self-compassion,
are intended to lead to greater psychological resilience. The latter half of the model
addresses interpersonal skills with the goal of developing extended compassion for
others. This model proposes that both pathways are mutually supportive and con-
tribute to enhanced wellbeing. See Table 2 for definitions of each construct in the
model. In building the CBCT integrative model, several assumptions were made:
(1) CBCT modules are taught linearly, but the skills developed are mutually rein-
forcing and integrated both conceptually and through practice; (2) CBCT skills and
outcomes can be strengthened with ongoing practice according to the proposed pro-
cess of skill acquisition (Fig. 1); and (3) Competency in skills developed in Modules
MODULE I attentional
Attentional Stability
and Clarity control
meta-
awareness
MODULE II
security Insight into the resilience
priming
Nature of Mental
Experience
dereification
MODULE III
self-
Self-Compassion compassion
FOUNDATIONAL
PRACTICE
wellbeing
Resting in a Moment of
Nurturance
MODULE IV
Cultivating
Impartiality
identification
affection
prosocial MODULE V
Gratitude and gratitude
motivation Affection
compassion
MODULE VI
Empathetic Concern
and Engaged
Compassion
empathy
Intrapersonal Security priming The experience of remembered feelings of safety and care
Attentional control The ability to sustain focus on object of interest
Meta-awareness The ability to notice what one is thinking and feeling
Dereification The ability to distinguish thoughts from accurate representations of the world
Self-compassion Feelings of kindness and acceptance directed at oneself
Interpersonal Prosocial motivation The desire to engage in behavior that is beneficial to others
Identification The process of viewing others as similar to oneself
Gratitude Positive feelings directed toward others based on the recognition of received benefits
Affection Feelings of warmth, closeness, and connection directed toward others
Empathy The ability to understand what another person is feeling and/or thinking
Outcomes Resilience The ability to recover, adapt, or grow in response to stressful or adverse experiences
Compassion A sensitivity to the suffering of others coupled with the desire to alleviate it
Wellbeing A state of positive psychosocial health
M. Ash et al.
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
I and II is critical for engagement in analytical practices (Module III–VI) and each
contributes to the principal outcomes: resilience and compassion.
An overview of each skill included in the CBCT integrative model can be found
in the following section.
The nurturing moment practice has two important functions as the foundational
CBCT practice. Security priming—the ability to engender feelings of safety and
comfort—is intended to ready the participants for learning by generating a sense
of safety and openness. Within the domain of attachment theory (Bowlby 1982),
secure attachment is associated with increased cognitive openness and flexibil-
ity (Collins and Read 1994; Mikulincer 1997). The foundational practice should
allow participants to become more facile in voluntarily generating a sense of
inner calm. In the CBCT integrative model, the ability to volitionally induce feel-
ings of security is considered critical for the development of enhanced resilience.
Second, the foundational practice is intended to strengthen pro-social motiva-
tion by reminding participants of the value of compassion as a basis for develop-
ing and strengthening pro-social motivation. Prior literature supports that secu-
rity priming promotes care-oriented feelings and behaviors (Mikulincer et al.
2005, 2014). The pro-social motivation developed in the foundational practice
is intended to allow participants to more energetically engage in the cultivation
of compassion via the subsequent six modules. Thus, the foundational practice
serves as the basis for the development of both intrapersonal and interpersonal
skills as is depicted in the CBCT integrative model.
In Module II, participants gain insight into the impermanent nature of thoughts and
emotions by observing their unfolding mental activity through a process of meta-
awareness. Meta-awareness has been defined as an ability to observe the current
contents of one’s conscious experience (Schooler 2002; Smallwood and Schooler
M. Ash et al.
Module IV is the first module in the CBCT sequence that explicitly addresses rela-
tions with others. The primary skill developed in Module IV is a more inclusive
sense of identification with others. Participants cultivate the insight that everyone,
even difficult people, are “just like me” in their desire for wellbeing. Module IV
practice is intended to soften feelings of extreme liking or disliking for certain indi-
viduals or groups. In CBCT’s integrative model, the recognition of common human-
ity—that everyone shares the wish to thrive and to avoid suffering—is the basis for
the cultivation of extended compassion.
Identification has been previously linked to increased likelihood of engaging in
helping behavior. For example, experimental research by Levine and colleagues
found that people are significantly more likely to stop and help someone wearing a
T-shirt supporting their favorite sports team than an unmarked or rival group T-shirt
(Levine et al. 2005). In the CBCT integrative model, identification is understood as
a building block for relating to others with affection.
The purpose of Module V is to deepen a sense of gratitude for others. Gratitude has
been previously associated with a host of positive health benefits including better
sleep quality, decreased depression, and overall wellbeing (Wood et al. 2009, 2010).
Gratitude is also associated with increased social affiliation (Bartlett et al. 2012) and
prosocial behavior (Bartlett and DeSteno 2006; Tsang and Martin 2019). While the
benefits of gratitude are well documented, there still remains limited understand-
ing regarding the process for how gratitude can be cultivated; nevertheless, modest
evidence supports that journaling and reflection may provide a means for enhanc-
ing gratitude (Emmons and McCullough 2003; Frias et al. 2011; Sheldon and
Lyubomirsky 2006).
In Module V, participants engage in reflective strategies to extend feelings of
gratitude to others. By considering the ways in which they rely on interdependent
societal systems, Module V encourages participants to see that it does not make
sense to limit appreciation to only small groups of people who provide the most
obvious benefit. In the CBCT integrative model, it is assumed that if one feels a
sense of gratitude for another, then one will naturally begin to relate to that person
with increased affection—a critical ingredient for compassion.
In the CBCT integrative model, identification and gratitude are intended to enhance
the cultivation of affection for others. In this case, affection is being used to translate
the Tibetan word “yid-‘ong” which conveys someone who is pleasing to the mind
(for a more detailed overview, see Cutler and Newland 2015). Under this definition,
affection should not be confused with feelings of romantic love. More appropriately,
affection can be understood as an endearing way of relating to others characterized
M. Ash et al.
by a sense of tenderness. In the CBCT integrative model, affection is the most criti-
cal ingredient to allow for compassion to arise. If one does not relate to another
with some degree of affection, then one will not be motivated to respond to them
compassionately. While participants likely relate to some friends and family with
a degree of affection, CBCT practices aim to extend this quality of affection to a
broader circle of people through processes of critical thinking and cognitive refram-
ing. This emphasis on cognitive reflection as path to affection contrasts common
western notions of affection as state primarily driven by emotions. Both Modules
IV and V cultivate insights to allow for this extension of affection as a natural out-
growth of relating to others with increased identification and gratitude.
Empathy is the experience of resonating with or understanding the way another per-
son is thinking or feeling. There is robust evidence that feelings of empathy can lead
to either a response of personal distress or empathic concern (Batson et al. 1994;
Decety et al. 2009; Eisenberg 2000; Eisenberg et al. 1989). An empathic distress
response results when one becomes emotionally distressed in response to the suf-
fering of another; in this case, attention becomes more oriented towards self than
towards the person suffering. In contrast, a response of empathic concern remains
other-oriented; in this case, one feels a sense of concern for the other and remains
focused on their situation. Empathic concern can thus elicit and sustain a motiva-
tion to help. More recent advances in affective neuroscience have shown distinct
differences in neural circuitry patterns when someone experiences an empathic dis-
tress response versus an empathic concern response (Singer and Lamm 2009). In
the CBCT integrative model, the affection developed in Modules IV and V is pro-
posed as a means to strengthen the focus on the other when confronted by suffering
and to buffer against the likelihood of an empathic distress response. We propose
that when one relates to another with increased affection, one will be more focused
on their suffering and how it may be alleviated and not be subsumed by one’s own
stress response. This view is consistent with research by Batson and colleagues who
showed valuing the welfare of others as antecedent to empathic concern. In Batson’s
model, empathic concern relies on perceiving the needs of another, valuing their
welfare, and taking their perspective (Batson et al. 2007).
It is important to note that the merits of empathy have been debated among psy-
chologists, most notably Paul Bloom, who argues that whom we feel empathy for is
often biased and short-sighted which can lead to immoral decision-making (Bloom
2017). We acknowledge the importance of this critique; it is for this reason that
the practice in Module IV: Cultivating Impartiality, wherein participants develop
increased identification with others, is of critical importance towards cultivating
extended compassion that is less subject to pre-disposed biases.
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
Resilience
In the CBCT integrative model, skills developed in the foundational practice and
Modules I–III are expected to enhance individual resilience. Resilience has been
studied across a variety of disciplines and contexts including psychology (Fletcher
and Sarkar 2013; Rutter 1987), sociology (Hall and Lamont 2013), and ecology
(Holling 1973). While definitions vary, in the context of the CBCT integrative
model, we primarily refer to resilience to consider psychological traits and processes
that enhance a person’s ability to recover, adapt, and ultimately grow in response
to adverse life experiences. In CBCT training, the security priming cultivated in
the foundational practice can be used as a tool to regulate the body when aroused
or distressed. Then, the attention cultivated in Module I is intended to foster par-
ticipants’ ability to choose where to place their attention, potentially disengaging
from distracting or harmful thoughts. Skills from Module II support participants
in observing their own mental activity through the use of meta-awareness. Further,
Module II practice is intended to lead to more accurate and granular perception of
one’s thoughts and feelings as well as an understanding that thoughts do not neces-
sarily align directly with external realities. Module III practice goes a step further by
asking participants to actively engage in cognitive re-appraisal strategies to respond
to their own challenges with kindness and self-acceptance based on the understand-
ing that adverse events are part of every human life and are due to many causes
and factors outside of the individual’s full control. Together, the skills cultivated
in the foundational practice and Modules I–III are intended to allow participants to
respond to challenges with enhanced resilience.
Importantly, the skills identified in the CBCT integrative model to promote resil-
ience are limited to components directly targeted in CBCT training. The model is
not intended to reflect an exhaustive list of factors that may promote resilience. For
instance, the prior literature suggests resilience is moderated by other psychologi-
cal factors which the proposed model does not directly address such as self-efficacy
(Schaubroeck and Merritt 1997) or positive affect (Tugade and Fredrickson 2004),
though these two factors in particular are likely encouraged by CBCT practices in
secondary ways. Furthermore, larger systems and structures related to economic
status, environment, cultural norms, and laws and policies are known to signifi-
cantly affect personal and social resilience (Bottrell 2009; Hall and Lamont 2013;
Walker et al. 2006). These findings are consistent with social ecological theories
which illustrate the ways in which behavior is subject to multiple levels of influence
including societal, communal, organizational, social, and personal (Bronfenbrenner
1977; McLeroy et al. 2016). In keeping with this view, we do not espouse that resil-
ience can be understood as simply an intrapersonal trait divorced from factors such
as social support, culture, or socioeconomic status. We acknowledge that social and
systemic factors are of critical importance to the promotion and support of resil-
ience, but they are outside the scope of intervening factors explicitly addressed in
Modules I–III of CBCT training.
M. Ash et al.
Compassion
The skills developed in Modules IV–VI are expected to increase extended compas-
sion for others. Identification (developed in Module IV) and gratitude (developed
in Module V) are both expected to contribute to a sense of affection for a wider
group of people. This sense of affection is paired with a growing awareness of oth-
ers’ vulnerabilities through enhanced empathy (developed in Module VI). As such,
the final three modules train participants to both notice the suffering of others and
feel motivated to help as a result of increased and more inclusive affection. In this
way, skills developed in Modules IV–VI are potentially supportive of forms of social
resilience, which is defined as the ability of groups to sustain wellbeing in the face
of challenges (Hall and Lamont 2013). Module V explicitly trains participants to
consider the ways in which their wellbeing is dependent on other people and sys-
tems, often referred to as “interdependence.” We expect that a growing awareness of
their interdependent nature combined with a motivation to help may foster forms of
social resilience by empowering individuals to appreciate the ways their actions can
affect the communities in which they live. Thus, while CBCT is an intervention that
targets individuals, efforts to expand compassion to growing networks could have
social and societal implications.
Wellbeing
Discussion
This article proposes a model for understanding CBCT’s key components and mech-
anisms. While recent research on CBCT has suggested a number of benefits from
its practice, little research has addressed how and why these practices are effective.
The CBCT integrative model proposes a framework for how key skills developed in
each module may lead to three principal outcomes of interest: resilience, compas-
sion, and wellbeing. The NIH Science of Behavior Change Program identified three
target classes that are central to behavior change: self-regulation, stress resilience
and stress reactivity, and interpersonal and social processes (Nielsen et al. 2018).
The proposed CBCT mechanisms of change include skills relevant to each identified
class. Module I through Module III primarily involve developing intrapersonal skills
intended to foster self-regulation and individual-level resilience. Module IV through
Module VI aims to develop other oriented, interpersonal skills that are necessary for
extended compassion. The CBCT integrative model proposes that wellbeing is con-
tingent on both personal resiliency and compassion for others. We expect these two
factors are mutually reinforcing—i.e. greater resilience may prevent burnout and
thereby facilitate the availability of more energy to care for others; likewise, experi-
encing greater compassion for others may allow for enhanced resiliency as individu-
als can respond to their own difficulties from a broader context.
While the CBCT integrative model provides a conceptual model indicating the
proposed relationship between key skills and outcomes developed in CBCT train-
ing, we expect a host of other factors not addressed in the model to likely moderate
effects of CBCT. For example, this model does not consider individual features and
contexts relevant to the participant including demographic variables, health and psy-
chological functioning, or prior exposure to contemplative practices. To date, there
is limited research directly investigating moderating variables pertinent to compas-
sion training. One study led by Goldin and Jazaieri found that demographic varia-
bles did not moderate effectiveness of compassion training, though they did find that
psychological functioning including perceived stress, mindfulness skills, and cog-
nitive reappraisal self-efficacy did moderate reductions in fear of self-compassion
after compassion training (2017). In a different study among a sample of medical
students participating in a CBCT intervention, Mascaro et al. found that those with
higher depression scores at baseline showed the largest increase in compassion for
others after CBCT was complete (2016), suggesting those in mental distress may
particularly benefit from CBCT training.
Similarly, the CBCT integrative model does not account for cultural and environ-
mental factors that may affect participants’ engagement with and benefit from CBCT.
Health behavior interventions at large have been criticized for their emphasis on tar-
geting individual level factors without consideration for the larger systems and social
structures that shape behavior (Holman and Borgstrom 2016; Stokols 1996; Thorlinds-
son 2011). There is increasing research utilizing social network theory to understand
the ways in which social ties and position within a social network influence a myriad
of behaviors (Borgatti et al. 2009). For instance, risk for obesity increases by 57% if a
friend becomes obese (Christakis and Fowler 2007), suggesting a contagion factor for
M. Ash et al.
unhealthful attitudes and behaviors. It is possible that healthful behaviors may also be
transmitted through social networks. We do not yet have sufficient data to understand
the ways in which dynamic social and cultural processes influence engagement with
CBCT as well compassionate behavior, but we advise that future research should con-
sider the impact of social networks and cultural contexts. Given that CBCT’s principal
goal is to enhance compassion for others, it is essential that future research consider
social and systemic factors that may influence potential outcomes. It is our intention
that the CBCT integrative model will provide a theoretical basis to study how different
populations experience and potentially benefit from CBCT training.
Conclusion
The CBCT integrative model provides a framework for future theory-driven research
on CBCT. Previous research on CBCT has primarily examined changes in health out-
comes after participation in CBCT training. It is our goal that the CBCT integrative
model provides a conceptual framework to understand the psychosocial processes
that influence such change. The proposed mechanistic relationships in the CBCT
integrative model are drawn primarily from the Indo-Tibetan Buddhist lojong frame-
work from which CBCT was adapted. It will be important for future research to align
identified constructs with appropriate empirical measures to test the effectiveness of
the model. The constructs outlined in the model are not exhaustive; we expect other
factors to also impact the outcomes of compassion training. Next steps for future
research include addressing the following questions: (1) Can the proposed mecha-
nisms in a CBCT intervention be confirmed? (2) What is the relationship between
identified intrapersonal skills (e.g. resilience) and interpersonal skills (e.g. compas-
sion)? The CBCT integrative model assumes intrapersonal and interpersonal skills
are mutually reinforcing, but this assumption should be addressed empirically. (3)
What are the impacts of dosage and sequence for a CBCT intervention? A 10-week
CBCT intervention is divided roughly into thirds with equal emphasis given to pre-
sent-moment practices (Modules I and II), self-compassion practices (Module III),
and other-oriented compassion practices (Modules IV–VI). It will be important for
future research to examine the impact of sequence and dosage on potential beneficial
outcomes. (4) How should the current CBCT integrative model be revised according
to empirical findings? An improved understanding of the mechanisms present in a
CBCT intervention could lend important insight regarding how to optimize and per-
sonalize the intervention to meet the needs of diverse populations.
Acknowledgements We thank Bobbi Patterson for her input on an earlier draft of this paper.
Conflict of interest Authors Marcia Ash, Timothy Harrison, and Lobsang Tenzin Negi are employed by
the Center for Contemplative-Science and Compassion-Based Ethics at Emory University. Authors Melis-
sa Pinto and Ralph DiClemente are on the Advisory Board of the Center for Contemplative Science and
Compassion-Based Ethics at Emory University. Lobsang Tenzin Negi is the developer of C BCT®, but
neither he nor any of the other authors hold a financial stake in the intellectual property rights of CBCT®.
A model for cognitively-based compassion training: theoretical…
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,
which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as
you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Com-
mons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article
are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is
not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission
directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licen
ses/by/4.0/.
References
Asaṅga, Rahula W., and S.B. Webb. 2001. Abhidharmasamuccaya. Fremont: Asian Humanities Press.
Ashar, Y.K., J.R. Andrews-Hanna, S. Dimidjian, and T.D. Wager. 2016. Towards a neuroscience of com-
passion: A brain systems-based model and research agenda. In Positive neuroscience, ed. J.D.
Greene, I. Morrison, and M.E.P. Seligman, 1–27. New York: Oxford University Press.
Bartlett, M.Y., P. Condon, J. Cruz, J. Baumann, and D. Desteno. 2012. Gratitude: Prompting behav-
iours that build relationships. Cognition and Emotion 26 (1): 2–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699
931.2011.561297.
Bartlett, M.Y., and D. DeSteno. 2006. Gratitude and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs you. Psy-
chological Science 17 (4): 319–325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x.
Batson, C.D., J.H. Eklund, V.L. Chermok, J.L. Hoyt, and B.G. Ortiz. 2007. An additional antecedent of
empathic concern: Valuing the welfare of the person in need. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 93 (1): 65–74. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.65.
Batson, C.D., J. Fultz, and P.A. Schoenrade. 1994. Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicar-
ious emotions with different motivational consequences. In Reaching out: Caring, altruism, and
prosocial behavior, ed. B. Puka and B. Puka, 57–75. New York: Garland Publishing.
Bloom, P. 2017. Against empathy: The case for rational compassion. New York: Random House.
Borgatti, S.P., A. Mehra, D.J. Brass, and G. Labianca. 2009. Network analysis in the social sciences. Sci-
ence 323 (5916): 892–895. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1165821.
Bottrell, D. 2009. Understanding ‘marginal’perspectives: Towards a social theory of resilience. Qualita-
tive Social Work 8 (3): 321–339.
Bowlby, J. 1982. Attachment and loss: Attachment, vol. 1, 2nd ed. New York: Basic Books.
Bowlby, J. 1988. A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London: Routledge.
Bronfenbrenner, U. 1977. Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psycholo-
gist 32 (7): 513.
Christakis, N.A., and J.H. Fowler. 2007. The spread of obesity in a large social network over 32 years.
New England Journal of Medicine 357 (4): 370–379. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMsa066082.
Collins, N.L., and S.J. Read. 1994. Cognitive representations of attachment: The structure and function
of working models. In Attachment processes in adulthood, ed. K. Bartholomew and D. Perlman,
53–90. London, England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Condon, P., G. Desbordes, W.B. Miller, and D. DeSteno. 2013. Meditation increases compassionate
responses to suffering. Psychological Science 24 (10): 2125–2127. https://doi.org/10.1177/09567
97613485603.
Cutler, J., and G. Newland. 2015. The great treatise on the stages of the path to enlightenment, vol. 1.
Ithaca, NY: Snow Lion Publications.
Dahl, C.J., A. Lutz, and R.J. Davidson. 2016. Cognitive processes are central in compassion meditation.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20 (3): 161–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.005.
Dalai Lama [Tenzin Gyatso] (2005). Essence of the Heart Sutra: The Dalai Lama’s Heart of Wisdom
Teachings (Trans. Ed.) Geshe Thupten Jinpa. Boston: Wisdom Publications.
Decety, J., K.D. Craig, W. Ickes, C.D. Batson, K.S. Blair, R.J.R. Blair, et al. 2009. The social neurosci-
ence of empathy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Desbordes, G., L.T. Negi, T.W. Pace, B.A. Wallace, C.L. Raison, and E.L. Schwartz. 2012. Effects
of mindful-attention and compassion meditation training on amygdala response to emotional
M. Ash et al.
Jinpa, T., S. Gyalchok, and K. Gyaltsen. 2014. Mind training: The great collection, vol. 1. New York:
Simon and Schuster.
Kabat-Zinn, J. 2013. Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body to face stress, pain, and ill-
ness. New York, NY: Bantam Books.
Kirby, J.N. 2017. Compassion interventions: The programmes, the evidence, and implications for research
and practice. Psychology and Psychotherapy 90 (3): 432–455. https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12104.
Klimecki, O.M., S. Leiberg, C. Lamm, and T. Singer. 2013. Functional neural plasticity and associated
changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cerebral Cortex 23 (7): 1552–1561. https://
doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs142.
Kyabgon, T. 2007. The practice of Lojong: Cultivating compassion through training the mind. Boulder:
Shambhala Publications.
Lama, D. 2005. Essence of the heart sutra: The Dalai Lama’s heart of wisdom teachings (Trans. Ed.)
Geshe Thupten Jinpa. Boston: Wisdom Publications.
Lazarus, R.S. 1991. Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
Leiberg, S., O. Klimecki, and T. Singer. 2011. Short-term compassion training increases prosocial behav-
ior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS ONE 6 (3): e17798. https://doi.org/10.1371/journ
al.pone.0017798.
Levine, M., A. Prosser, D. Evans, and S. Reicher. 2005. Identity and emergency intervention: How social
group membership and inclusiveness of group boundaries shape helping behavior. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin 31 (4): 443–453. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271651.
LoParo, D., S.A. Mack, B. Patterson, L.T. Negi, and N.J. Kaslow. 2018. The efficacy of cognitively-
based compassion training for African American suicide attempters. Mindfulness. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s12671-018-0940-1.
Lopez, A., R. Sanderman, A.V. Ranchor, and M.J. Schroevers. 2018. Compassion for others and self-
compassion: levels, correlates, and relationship with psychological well-being. Mindfulness 9 (1):
325–331. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0777-z.
Lutz, A., J. Brefczynski-Lewis, T. Johnstone, and R.J. Davidson. 2008a. Regulation of the neural cir-
cuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE 3 (3):
e1897. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001897.
Lutz, A., A.P. Jha, J.D. Dunne, and C.D. Saron. 2015. Investigating the phenomenological matrix of
mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. American Psychologist 70 (7):
632–658. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039585.
Lutz, A., H.A. Slagter, J.D. Dunne, and R.J. Davidson. 2008b. Attention regulation and monitor-
ing in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (4): 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
tics.2008.01.005.
MacBeth, A., and A. Gumley. 2012. Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis of the association between
self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review 32 (6): 545–552. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.cpr.2012.06.003.
Mascaro, J., A. Darcher, L.T. Negi, and C. Raison. 2015. The neural mediators of kindness-based
meditation: A theoretical model. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 109. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg
.2015.00109.
Mascaro, J., S. Kelley, A. Darcher, L.T. Negi, C. Worthman, A. Miller, and C. Raison. 2016. Medita-
tion buffers medical student compassion from the deleterious effects of depression. The Journal of
Positive Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760.2016.1233348.
Mascaro, J., J. Rilling, L.T. Negi, and C. Raison. 2013. Compassion meditation enhances empathic accu-
racy and related neural activity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8 (1): 48–55. https://
doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss095.
McLeroy, K.R., D. Bibeau, A. Steckler, and K. Glanz. 2016. An ecological perspective on health promo-
tion programs. Health Education Quarterly 15 (4): 351–377. https://doi.org/10.1177/1090198188
01500401.
Mikulincer, M. 1997. Adult attachment style and information processing: Individual differences in curi-
osity and cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 72 (5): 1217–1230. https
://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.5.1217.
Mikulincer, M., P.R. Shaver, O. Gillath, and R.A. Nitzberg. 2005. Attachment, caregiving, and altruism:
Boosting attachment security increases compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology 89 (5): 817–839. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.817.
Mikulincer, M., P.R. Shaver, N. Bar-On, and B.K. Sahdra. 2014. Security enhancement, self-esteem
threat, and mental depletion affect provision of a safe haven and secure base to a romantic partner.
M. Ash et al.
Shonin, E., W. Van Gordon, A. Compare, M. Zangeneh, and M.D. Griffiths. 2014. Buddhist-derived
loving-kindness and compassion meditation for the treatment of psychopathology: A systematic
review. Mindfulness 6 (5): 1161–1180. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0368-1.
Singer, T., and C. Lamm. 2009. The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of
Sciences 1156 (1): 81–96. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x.
Smallwood, J., and J.W. Schooler. 2015. The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the
stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology 66 (1): 487–518. https://doi.org/10.1146/
annurev-psych-010814-015331.
Stokols, D. 1996. Translating social ecological theory into guidelines for community health promotion.
American Journal of Health Promotion 10 (4): 282–298.
Strauss, C., B. Lever Taylor, J. Gu, W. Kuyken, R. Baer, F. Jones, and K. Cavanagh. 2016. What is com-
passion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clinical Psychology
Review 47: 15–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.004.
Thorlindsson, T. 2011. Bring in the social context: Towards an integrated approach to health promotion
and prevention. Scandinavian Journal of Public Health 39 (6_suppl): 19–25.
Tsai, M.H., and W.L. Chou. 2016. Attentional orienting and executive control are affected by different
types of meditation practice. Consciousness and Cognition 46: 110–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
concog.2016.09.020.
Tsang, J.-A., and S.R. Martin. 2019. Four experiments on the relational dynamics and prosocial con-
sequences of gratitude. The Journal of Positive Psychology 14 (2): 188–205. https://doi.
org/10.1080/17439760.2017.1388435.
Tugade, M.M., and B.L. Fredrickson. 2004. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back
from negative emotional experiences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86 (2): 320.
van den Hurk, P.A.M., F. Giommi, S.C. Gielen, A.E.M. Speckens, and H.P. Barendregt. 2010. Greater
efficiency in attentional processing related to mindfulness meditation. Quarterly Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology 63 (6): 1168–1180. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903249365.
Walker, B., J. Anderies, A. Kinzig, and P. Ryan. 2006. Exploring resilience in social-ecological systems
through comparative studies and theory development: Introduction to the special issue. Ecology
and Society 11 (1): 13.
Weng, H.Y., A.S. Fox, A.J. Shackman, D.E. Stodola, J.Z. Caldwell, M.C. Olson, et al. 2013. Compas-
sion training alters altruism and neural responses to suffering. Psychological Science 24 (7): 1171–
1180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612469537.
Williams, J.M.G. 2010. Mindfulness and psychological process. Emotion 10 (1): 1–7. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0018360.
Winkielman, P., and J.W. Schooler. 2011. Splitting consciousness: Unconscious, conscious, and metacon-
scious processes in social cognition. European Review of Social Psychology 22 (1): 1–35. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2011.576580.
Wood, A.M., J.J. Froh, and A.W. Geraghty. 2010. Gratitude and well-being: A review and theoretical inte-
gration. Clinical Psychology Review 30 (7): 890–905. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005.
Wood, A.M., S. Joseph, J. Lloyd, and S. Atkins. 2009. Gratitude influences sleep through the mecha-
nism of pre-sleep cognitions. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 66 (1): 43–48. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.09.002.
Wynn, K., P. Bloom, A. Jordan, J. Marshall, and M. Sheskin. 2017. Not noble savages after all: Lim-
its to early altruism. Current Directions in Psychological Science 27 (1): 3–8. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0963721417734875.
Zessin, U., O. Dickhauser, and S. Garbade. 2015. The relationship between self-compassion and well-
being: A meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being 7 (3): 340–364. https://doi.
org/10.1111/aphw.12051.
Zhang, H., N.N. Watson-Singleton, S.E. Pollard, D.M. Pittman, D.A. Lamis, N.L. Fischer, et al. 2017.
Self-criticism and depressive symptoms: Mediating role of self-compassion. Omega. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0030222817729609.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published
maps and institutional affiliations.
M. Ash et al.
Marcia Ash MPH is a doctoral student in the Department of Behavioral Sciences and Health Education
at the Rollins School of Public Health at Emory University. Ash is also a certified Cognitively-Based
Compassion Training (CBCT®) instructor. Ash holds a Bachelor of Arts in Philosophy, Neuroscience
and Psychology from Washington University in St. Louis and a Masters in Public Health (MPH) from the
Behavioral Sciences and Health Education Department at the Rollins School of Public Health at Emory
University.
Timothy Harrison M.Arch. is the Associate Director for C BCT® (Cognitively-Based Compassion Train-
ing) at the Center for Contemplative Science and Compassion-Based Ethics at Emory University. He
oversees CBCT programs, including multiple research initiatives and the teacher certification process. He
regularly teaches CBCT nationally and internationally in academic institutions and healthcare settings,
and he advises on the tailoring of CBCT for research with unique populations. He holds a master’s degree
from Harvard University.
Melissa Pinto Ph.D., RN, FSAHM, FAAN is an Associate Professor in the Sue & Bill Gross School of
Nursing at the University of California, Irvine. She has expertise in the area of adolescent and young
adult behavioral health, self-management, development and assessment of eHealth and technology-based
behavioral health interventions, and psychosocial barriers to mental health treatment for adolescents. She
completed her PhD in Nursing Science at the University of Louisville and a KL2 Award at Case Western
Reserve University. Dr. Pinto is a Fellow of the Society for Adolescent Health and Medicine and the
American Academy of Nursing.
Ralph DiClemente Ph.D. is Chair of the Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences and Associate
Dean of Public Health Innovation at New York University. Dr. DiClemente has focused on developing
intervention packages that blend community and technology-based approaches that are designed to opti-
mize program effectiveness and enhance programmatic sustainability. He has published extensively in the
area of adolescent health, particularly HIV/STD prevention, among adolescents and young adults, and in
adolescent vaccine programs for influenza and HPV prevention. Dr. DiClemente holds a PhD in Health
Psychology from the University of California, San Francisco after completing a SCM at the Harvard
School of Public Health.
Lobsang Tenzin Negi Ph.D. is a Professor of Pedagogy in the Department of Religion at Emory Univer-
sity. He is also the Executive Director of the Center for Contemplative Science and Compassion-Based
Ethics as well as the developer of CBCT® (Cognitively-Based Compassion Training). He is a former
monk who completed his monastic education at Drepung Loseling Monastery in South India where he
received the Geshe Lharampa Degree—the highest academic degree granted in the Tibetan Buddhist tra-
dition. He completed his PhD at Emory University where he studied traditional Buddhist and contempo-
rary western approaches to emotions and their impact on wellness.
Affiliations
1
Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, 1531 Clifton Rd. NE, Room 549, Atlanta,
GA 30322, USA
2
Center for Contemplative Science and Compassion‑Based Ethics, Emory University, 1599
Clifton Rd. NE, Atlanta, GA 30322, USA
3
Sue & Bill Gross School of Nursing, University of California, Irvine, 284 Berk Hall, Room
100D, Irvine, CA 92967, USA
4
Department of Social and Behavioral Sciences, College of Global Public Health, New York
University, New York, NY, USA