0% found this document useful (0 votes)
4 views14 pages

nihms40349

The document discusses how emotions influence judgment and cognitive processing, emphasizing the affect-as-information hypothesis, which posits that affect provides valuable information about the object of judgment. It highlights that positive affect enhances evaluations and promotes relational processing, while negative affect leads to item-specific processing and can inhibit judgments. The findings suggest that affective states significantly shape decision-making and cognitive tasks across various contexts.

Uploaded by

Pawni Agarwal
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
0% found this document useful (0 votes)
4 views14 pages

nihms40349

The document discusses how emotions influence judgment and cognitive processing, emphasizing the affect-as-information hypothesis, which posits that affect provides valuable information about the object of judgment. It highlights that positive affect enhances evaluations and promotes relational processing, while negative affect leads to item-specific processing and can inhibit judgments. The findings suggest that affective states significantly shape decision-making and cognitive tasks across various contexts.

Uploaded by

Pawni Agarwal
Copyright
© © All Rights Reserved
We take content rights seriously. If you suspect this is your content, claim it here.
Available Formats
Download as PDF, TXT or read online on Scribd
You are on page 1/ 14

NIH Public Access

Author Manuscript
Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Published in final edited form as:
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Trends Cogn Sci. 2007 September ; 11(9): 393–399.

How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought

Gerald L. Clore and Jeffrey R. Huntsinger


Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904, USA

Abstract
Being happy or sad influences the content and style of thought. One explanation is that affect serves
as information about the value of whatever comes to mind. Thus, when a person makes evaluative
judgments or engages in a task, positive affect can enhance evaluations and empower potential
responses. Rather than affect itself, the information conveyed by affect is crucial. Tests of the
hypothesis find that affective influences can be made to disappear by changing the source to which
the affect is attributed. In tasks, positive affect validates and negative affect invalidates accessible
cognitions, leading to relational processing and item-specific processing, respectively. Positive affect
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

is found to promote, and negative affect to inhibit, many textbook phenomena from cognitive
psychology.

Introduction
Across academic fields, from history and literature to economics and neuroscience, a
convergence of opinion has emerged about the importance of understanding emotion (see
Glossary) [1]. In this article, we review behavioral research on how affective reactions,
including moods and emotions, guide human judgment and cognitive processing [2]. The
research shows that people’s judgments often reflect their current moods. In happy moods,
people judge many things, from consumer products [3] to life satisfaction [4], more positively
than when they feel sad.

The affect-as-information hypothesis [5] proposes that affect assigns value to whatever seems
to be causing it (Box 1). For example, affect might assign value to objects of judgment or,
during cognitive tasks, to one’s own thoughts and inclinations. After examining the background
to this research, we review studies of affect and judgment, followed by studies of affect and
cognitive processing.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Theorists commonly assume that people’s attitudes and judgments reflect information about
the object of judgment. But people’s evaluations also reflect information from their own
affective reactions. In social situations, for example, the crucial factor in our evaluation of other
people is often the feelings that they elicit in us. However, knowing and feeling are tightly
linked, and disentangling them requires methods by which affect can be varied independently
of belief [6]. Early experiments using emotion-arousing films to induce mood showed that
affect could influence attraction to other people over and above cognitions about them [7].
Inductions of mood through film, music and writing tasks have since become standard research
methods.

Initial explanations of such mood-congruent judgments focused on the possibility that mood
might prime (activate in memory) material that is mood congruent [8,9]. According to this
‘priming’ hypothesis, moods generate liking or disliking by activating positive or negative

Corresponding author: Clore, G.L. ([email protected]).


Clore and Huntsinger Page 2

beliefs about the object of judgment. Despite abundant research, however, evidence for mood-
congruent priming by affect remains controversial [10].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

By contrast, the ‘affect-as-information’ hypothesis [4] proposes that affective cues of mood
and emotion influence judgments directly by serving as experiential and bodily information
regarding how one feels about the object of judgment. Such experiential information can be
more compelling than thoughts about the object of judgment, and can also be reported faster
than thoughts [11].

The initial evidence on which this view was based involved a telephone survey of life
satisfaction [4]. Calls were made on either warm and sunny or cold and rainy spring days. The
results demonstrated that mood affects judgment because rainy days depressed both moods
and ratings of life satisfaction. In one crucial condition, however, interviewers first asked
respondents about the weather before asking about life satisfaction. By subtly linking people’s
feelings to the weather in this way, the effects of mood on rating life satisfaction disappeared.
The effect of the weather question was not to change people’s feelings, but to alter what the
feelings seemed to signify.

This experiment has been replicated with several variations. Related studies establishing the
generality of such effects over different emotions and situations include experiments on the
influence of feelings of distress, disgust and sadness on various different judgments.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Box 1. Theoretical summary


Affect as information
According to the ‘affect-as-information’ hypothesis, affect provides compelling
information about the personal value of whatever is in mind at the time.
Affect and evaluative judgments: When making evaluative judgments, people often ask
themselves ‘how do I feel about it?’ [2,5]. In such cases, positive affect signals that the
object of judgment is valuable, leading to a positive evaluation, and negative affect signals
that it lacks value, leading to a negative evaluation.
Affect and cognitive processing: In task situations, affect can be experienced as feedback
about the value of one’s current thoughts and inclinations [5,33]. A default strategy in many
situations is for people to engage in relational processing (relating incoming information to
what is already known or believed). Positive affect tends to reinforce this tendency, leading
to relational (cognitive, interpretive, category-level and global) processing, whereas
negative affect tends to inhibit this tendency, leading to referential (perceptual, item-level
and local) processing [5,34,48].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Other theories
Memory priming: An alternative to the affect-as-information hypothesis is the priming
hypothesis, which proposes that mood primes mood-congruent material in memory, which
then serves as a basis for judgment [8,9]. There is currently little evidence to indicate that
positive mood primes positive material in memory in this way [10]; however, a related
hypothesis is that positive mood engages areas responsible for semantic processing.
Semantic processing: As suggested above, several theories converge on the general idea
that positive mood engages semantic processing. These theories include proposals that
positive mood activates substantive processing [49], semantic associations [50] and
dopamine release [51]. These proposals are convergent with the affect-as-information
characterization of processing in positive moods as relational [34].

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 3

Motivation and attention: The phenomena that the affect-as-information hypothesis


explains in terms of positive and negative affect are explained by some investigators [35]
in terms of the approach–avoidance motivation that is often attendant to positive–negative
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

affect. In addition, some investigators characterize the outcomes that the affect-as-
information hypothesis describes as ‘relational versus item-specific processing’ in terms of
‘broad versus narrow attention’ [52]. These issues raise questions for further research, in
addition to those listed in Box 3.

Affect and judgment


Attributions regulate affective influences
The affect-as-information hypothesis proposes that affective influences on judgment depend
on the affect being experienced as a reaction to the object of judgment. An interesting test has
come from a mock trial in which jurors’ attributions for their feelings were varied.

Accountancy students served as jurors and rendered decisions about the culpability of an
accounting firm in a corporate bankruptcy case [12]. Different versions of the trial transcript
were presented with varying amounts of detail about the distressing consequences of the
bankruptcy. The more distressed the jurors felt about the harmful consequences of the
bankruptcy, the more they judged the accounting firm liable. Some jurors, however, had been
asked before the trial began to rate their anxiety about being a juror. Those jurors were
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

significantly less likely to reach verdicts that went against the firm because their distress seemed
to be about having to render a decision rather than about the effects of the bankruptcy.

Again, we see that whether or not affect influences judgment depends on implicit attributions
about its cause. Without a salient cause, affect tends to be promiscuous, attaching itself to
whatever is available, which is why moods can influence even irrelevant judgments.

Specific emotions have specific effects


The affect-as-information hypothesis applies to affect from specific emotions in addition to
moods. Because different emotions convey different information about the ways in which
events are positive or negative [13], however, emotions of similar valence can have different
effects.

Such effects have been demonstrated in an online study conducted immediately after the
terrorist attacks of 11 September 2001 with a sample of 1000 Americans [14]. Respondents
were induced to focus on either angering or fear-inducing aspects of the events. Angry (but
not fearful) respondents subsequently favored policies of retaliation, whereas fearful (but not
angry) respondents made higher risk estimates for both risks of terrorist attacks and completely
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

unrelated risks.

Another set of studies has examined the impact of disgust on judgments of morality (S.Schnall
et al., unpublished). In one, disgust was induced by having participants work at a dirty, sticky
desk in a filthy, trash-filled room. Results showed that the room increased disgust among
individuals who habitually focused on their bodily reactions, and this disgust led them to judge
morally ambiguous actions as immoral. This effect was not observed for individuals made to
feel sad.

In these examples, instead of the broad brush effects of general moods, specific emotions had
more targeted influences.

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 4

Emotions can make mountains out of molehills


The studies described so far involve judgment tasks with no right or wrong answers. Other
studies have examined judgments of physical reality (C.Riener, PhD thesis, University of
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Virginia, 2007). In these experiments, sad music heard while standing at the bottom of a steep
hill led participants to overestimate the incline of the hill. The overestimations were similar to
those made by participants wearing a heavy backpack [15].

This tendency to make mountains out of molehills has also been shown for participants
experiencing mild fear from standing at the top of the hill on a skateboard. They overestimated
the incline significantly more that others standing on a stable platform of the same height (J.
Stefanucci, PhD thesis, University of Virginia, 2006).

Conclusions about affective influence on judgment


In summary, we have reviewed studies indicating that affective feelings influence judgments
of life satisfaction, culpability, risk, morality and physical space. Although the principles have
been applied to affective feelings, they apply equally well to non-affective feelings (Box 2).

Most of the affective phenomena reviewed here depend on some kind of misattribution of
affect, which suggests that affect is an unwanted source of bias. The message, however, does
not lie in the method used in these studies. Affect is, in fact, crucial for good judgment. Studies
show that individuals with neurological damage involving deficits in affect show marked
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

distortions of judgment and decision-making [16,17].

Box 2. Affect-as-information principles apply to non-affective feelings


The basic principles of the affect-as-information hypothesis also apply to similar non-
affective phenomena. Familiarity, certainty, confusion, amazement and sleepiness all have
characteristic feeling or experiential aspects. But they are not affective feelings, because
they are not primarily about goodness or badness. Much literature focuses on feelings of
‘ease or difficulty in remembering something’. The experience of ease or difficulty of
retrieving a memory is often more important in judgment and decision-making than the
memory itself [53].
In a recent study [54], for example, participants recalled details of the Oklahoma City
bombing and then indicated when they thought it occurred and how personally important
it was. They were instructed to recall either two or ten details of the bombing to create
experiences of ease and difficulty of retrieval. The intriguing finding was that recalling more
details (ten rather than two) made the bombing seem both more distant in time and less
personally important, because distant and unimportant events are harder to recall. In
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

addition, just as with affective feelings, changing participants’ attributions for these feelings
of retrieval – by making salient the true cause of the ease or difficulty – eliminated their
effects. Thus, the influence of these non-affective feelings on judgments also reflected their
informational value.

Affect and cognitive processing


In addition to influencing judgment and decision-making, affect influences how people process
information. Such influences have been observed across a range of tasks, including problem-
solving [18], stereotyping [19] and persuasion [20]. Despite the variety of cognitive tasks
involved, the influences of affect are relatively consistent. Table 1 lists some of the cognitive
phenomena that have shown affective influences.

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 5

In this section, we describe several experiments showing that when people are made happy
they engage in global, category-level, relational processing, whereas when they are sad they
engage in local, item-level, stimulus-specific processing. Such affective influences are evident
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

in the repetition of a classic experiment (Figure 1). A related experiment examining global–
local focus is described here.

Affect regulates global–local focus


People have a surprising ability to zoom in and out mentally, focusing on either the big picture
or the details. But some people favor a focus on the forest, whereas others favor the trees. For
example, habitually anxious individuals are quick to focus on local stimulus features, especially
when currently feeling anxious [21].

To examine further the impact of momentary mood, participants in one experiment wrote about
either a happy or a sad event in their lives or about a typical day [22]. They then responded to
a global–local perception task [23]. The task involves pictures in which a triangle might be
made of squares or a square might be made of triangles. Respondents select which of two
comparison pictures (e.g. squares made of squares or triangles made of triangles) is most similar
to the original. Participants in sad moods adopted a more local focus than those in happy moods
(Figure 2). Although some studies have also found a difference between happy and neutral
moods [24], in these data both happy and neutral groups showed the normative tendency toward
a global focus [22].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Consistent with affect-as-information principles, drawing participants’ attention to the true


cause of their feelings [25] eliminated the impact of mood on global–local perception. This
finding suggests that the affect-as-information processes that govern judgment also guide
efforts on cognitive tasks.

Emotions modulate stereotyping


A different kind of global–local perception concerns whether people see others as members of
groups or as individuals. Social psychologists have sought to determine when people use
stereotypes in social perception [26]. Contrary to most people’s intuitions, happy moods
promote group stereotyping, whereas sad moods promote a focus on individuals [27,28]. One
relevant study involved a mock trial in which a Latino student was accused of a stereotype-
consistent offense. The results showed that individuals in happy moods were more likely than
those in sad moods to have their verdicts influenced by the stereotype [29].

In this experiment, the stereotyping seems to reflect a general cognitive style rather than
prejudice as such. Indeed, similar findings come from marketing and political science studies
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

showing that happy moods promote reliance on brand names as opposed to product attributes
among consumers [30], and a reliance on political party as opposed to candidate positions
among voters [31].

In addition, a surprising result in the mock jury study [29] was that angry jurors responded like
happy jurors, rather than like sad ones. This finding is consistent with affect-as-information
logic, which always asks about the information inherent in affective states. Despite being a
negative emotion, anger carries positive information about one’s own position. When angry,
one believes oneself to be correct, which should increase confidence in one’s own cognitions.
Thus, anger would be expected to show the same processing effects as happiness [5].

Positive affect promotes dominant responses


The stereotyping research illustrates the ‘affective processing principle’ [5], which proposes
that positive affect promotes, whereas negative affect inhibits, the use of accessible cognitions

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 6

and responses. A crucial test of the principle comes from recent research (J.R.H. et al.,
unpublished data) on ‘chronic egalitarians’, for whom egalitarian rather than stereotyped
responses are habitually the most accessible. As proposed, positive affect empowered and sad
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

mood blocked the most accessible response. Happy mood increased egalitarian rather than
stereotypic responses, and sad moods inhibited such egalitarian impulses, which increased
stereotyping for these individuals.

A further example of how sad mood acts as a stop sign for dominant responses comes from a
behavioral economics experiment in which respondents placed a value on a small gift [32]. In
neutral moods, they showed the ‘endowment effect’ – that is, being willing to pay more to keep
things than to buy them initially. But consistent with affect-as-information logic, sad moods
completely reversed the effect.

These results again suggest that sad and happy affect signal the value of current response
inclinations [5,33]. They also show that accurate predictions require knowledge of what
inclinations are dominant in particular situations.

Affect regulates relational versus item-specific processing


In problem-solving situations, if people engage in relational processing (i.e. relating aspects
of the problem to what they know), then positive affect should confer value on that approach,
resulting in a relationship between positive affect and relational processing. Conversely,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

negative affect should make relational processing seem problematic, resulting in more item-
specific processing [34].

Note that normally, rather than induced mood states, the source of affect would be finding
oneself making progress on a task or encountering difficulty. This ‘affective feedback’ would
then regulate attention [24,35,36] and elicit ‘cognitive tuning’ [2] to meet task demands.

As this hypothesis suggests, whether relational or item-specific thinking is superior should


depend on the task. Thus, sad moods lead to superior spatial memory [37], whereas happy
moods foster creative responses [38]. But positive moods also lead to errors when solving
syllogisms [39] or engaging in schema-guided memory [40].

A final set of experiments employed tasks known to involve relational processing to determine
whether or not relational versus item-specific processing characterizes affective influences on
processing.

Sad moods decrease false memories and forgetting


Even when motivated to be accurate, people can generate false memories, a fact of practical
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

importance in legal testimony. To induce false memories, participants in one experiment


studied lists of words associated with other words that would never be presented, called ‘critical
lures’ [41]. For example, words such as bed, pillow, rest, awake and blanket all activate the
word ‘sleep’, which might then be falsely recalled.

Such false memories are assumed to reflect relational or gist processing [42,43]. According to
the affect-as-information hypothesis, positive mood should promote and negative mood should
inhibit such relational processing, making false-memory studies ideal for hypothesis testing.
As predicted, individuals in happy moods do show high numbers of false memories – a
tendency that is significantly reduced in sad moods [44,45] (Figure 3). As often occurs, happy
and neutral participants perform similarly, because even neutral participants usually report
positive resting moods.

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 7

Research on forgetting paints a similar picture. Eyewitnesses who are repeatedly interrogated
about some aspect of an event tend to forget other aspects [46]. Again, positive moods have
been found to sustain such deficits, whereas negative moods eliminate them [47]. This curious
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

tendency for remembering one thing to induce forgetting of others is thought to be promoted
by relational processing and to be inhibited by item-specific processing. The mood moderation
of such forgetting suggests that relational versus item-specific processing is indeed one way
in which affect influences cognitive processing.

Concluding remarks
Affect and emotion are pervasive influences on human judgment and thought. We have
summarized evidence relevant to one hypothesis about the psychological processes involved.
We initially discussed experiments finding influences of moods and emotions on various kinds
of judgment. We then described experiments finding such influences on cognitive processing.
Questions for future research are listed in Box 3. The affect-as-information hypothesis explains
both judgment and processing effects by assuming that affect serves as a compelling form of
information about value. In the case of judgment, value might be assigned to the object of
judgment; in the case of processing, by contrast, value might be assigned to the person’s own
cognitions and inclinations. Experiments consistently show that positive affective information
promotes and negative affective information inhibits the cognitive responses that are accessible
or dominant in a particular situation.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

The tasks commonly used in cognitive psychological research involve relational processing –
that is, relating incoming information to what is already known. Our review shows that many
of the textbook phenomena of cognitive psychology occur when people feel happy, but do not
occur or occur only in a reduced form when people feel even slightly sad. Because these are
the kinds of phenomena on which the cognitive revolution was based, the results suggest –
somewhat ironically – that the cognitive revolution had an emotional trigger.

Box 3. Questions for future research


• How do affective influences on processing interact with culture? Research
frequently finds increased negative affect in East Asian as compared with Western
societies [55]; however, other research also shows a predominance of an holistic
cognitive style in the East, which is thought to typify positive rather than negative
affect [56]. Is the relationship between affect and cognitive processing discussed
in this article common to all cultures, does positive affect trigger culturally
normative styles, or does some other pattern hold?
• What are the respective roles of conscious and unconscious affect? Conscious
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

feelings provide information about implicit attitudes, preferences and decisions.


They are important for planning and self knowledge. However, the connections
defining attitude are formed presumably not in consciousness but implicitly
between neural representations of attended objects and neurochemical reactions
to them [57]. What, then, are the functions of conscious and unconscious affect in
this process? Are judgments more dependent on conscious affect, and processing
decisions more dependent on unconscious affect?
• How do the hedonic and informational roles of affect combine and conflict? In
performance situations, affect serves as feedback about progress towards goals
(affect as information); however, sometimes maximizing positive affect is itself
the goal (affect as goal). Many publications have examined each of these roles of
affect in isolation, but in what kinds of situations is each active? In some situations,
informational and hedonic goals coincide (e.g. as experienced in the pleasure of

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 8

mastery), but in many they might not. When do they function jointly? And when
they conflict, what factors dictate which one will prevail?
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Acknowledgements
Support is acknowledged from the National Institute of Mental Health (grant MH 50074) and the National Science
Foundation (grant BCS 0518835).

References
1. McLemee S. Getting emotional. Chron. Higher Ed 2003;49:A14.
2. Schwarz, N.; Clore, GL. Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In: Higgins, ET.; Kruglanski, K.,
editors. Social Psychology. A Handbook of Basic Principles. 2nd edn. Guilford Press; 2007. p.
385-407.
3. Yeung CWM, Wyer RS Jr. Affect, appraisal, and consumer judgment. J. Consum. Res 2004;31:412–
424.
4. Schwarz N, Clore GL. Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: informative and directive
functions of affective states. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1983;45:513–523.
5. Clore, G., et al. Affective feelings as feedback: some cognitive consequences. In: Martin, LL.; Clore,
GL., editors. Theories of Mood and Cognition: A User’s Guidebook. Erlbaum; 2001. p. 63-84.
6. Griffitt W, Veitch R. Hot and crowded: influences of population density and temperature on
interpersonal affective behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1971;17:92–98. [PubMed: 5542556]
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

7. Gouaux C. Induced affective states and interpersonal attraction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 1971;20:37–
4310. [PubMed: 5121164]
8. Bower GH, et al. Emotional mood as a context of learning and recall. J. Verb. Learn. Verb. Behav
1978;17:573–585.
9. Isen A, et al. Affect, accessibility of material in memory, and behavior: a cognitive loop? J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol 1978;36:1–12. [PubMed: 621625]
10. Wyer RS, et al. Affect and information processing. Adv. Exp. Social Psychol 1999;31:1–77.
11. Pham MT, et al. Affect monitoring and the primacy of feelings in judgment. J. Consum. Res
2001;28:167–188.
12. Kadous K. Improving jurors’ evaluations of auditors in negligence cases. Contemp. Account. Res
2001;18:425–444.
13. Clore, GL.; Ortony, A. Appraisal theories: how cognition shapes affect into emotion. In: Lewis, M.,
et al., editors. Handbook of Emotions. 3rd edn.. Guilford Press; (in press)
14. Lerner JS, et al. Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: a national field experiment.
Psychol. Sci 2003;14:144–150. [PubMed: 12661676]
15. Proffitt DR. Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 2006;1:110–
122.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

16. Ramachandran, VS.; Blakeslee, S. Phantoms in the Brain. William Morrow; 1998.
17. Damasio, AR. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. G.P.Putnam; 1994.
18. Schwarz, N.; Skurnik, I. Feeling and thinking: implications for problem solving. In: Davidson, J.;
Sternberg, R., editors. The Nature of Problem Solving. Cambridge University Press; 2003. p.
263-292.
19. Bless H, et al. Mood and stereotyping: the impact of moods on the use of general knowledge structures.
Eur. Rev. Soc. Pychol 1996;7:63–93.
20. Schwarz N, et al. Mood and persuasion: affective states influence the processing of persuasive
communications. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol 1991;24:161–199.
21. Derryberry A, Reed MA. Anxiety and attentional focusing: trait, state and hemispheric influences.
Pers. Indiv. Differ 1998;25:745–761.
22. Gasper K, Clore GL. Attending to the big picture: mood and global versus local processing of visual
information. Psychol. Sci 2002;13:34–40. [PubMed: 11892776]

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 9

23. Kimchi R, Palmer SE. Form and texture in hierarchically constructed patterns. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
Percept. Perform 1982;8:521–535. [PubMed: 6214605]
24. Fredrickson BL, Branigan C. Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought–action
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

repertoires. Cogn. Emotion 2005;19:313–332.


25. Gasper K. Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cogn. Emotion
2004;18:405–421.
26. Fiedler, F. Stereotyping as Inductive Hypothesis Testing (European Monographs in Social
Psychology). Psychology Press; 2004.
27. Bodenhausen, GV. Emotions, arousal, and stereotypic judgments: a heuristic model of affect and
stereotyping. In: Mackie, DM.; Hamilton, DL., editors. Affect, Cognition and Stereotyping.
Academic Press; 1993. p. 13-37.
28. Isbell LM. Not all happy people are lazy or stupid: evidence of systematic processing in happy moods.
J. Exp. Soc. Psychol 2004;40:341–349.
29. Bodenhausen GV, et al. Happiness and stereotypic thinking in social judgement. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol
1994;66:621–632.
30. Adaval R. Sometimes it just feels right: the differential weighting of affect-consistent and affect-
inconsistent product information. J. Consum. Res 2001;28:1–17.
31. Marcus, GE., et al. Affective Intelligence and Political Judgment. University of Chicago Press; 2000.
32. Lerner JS, et al. Heart strings and purse strings: effects of specific emotions on economic transactions.
Psychol. Sci 2004;15:337–341. [PubMed: 15102144]
33. Petty RE, Briñol P. Persuasion: insights from the self-validation hypothesis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

(in press)
34. Clore, GL.; Storbeck, J. Affect as information about liking, efficacy, and importance. In: Forgas, J.,
editor. Affect in Social Thinking and Behavior. Psychology Press; 2006. p. 123-142.
35. Förster J, et al. Enactment of approach and avoidance behavior influences the scope of perceptual
and conceptual attention. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol 2006;42:133–146.
36. Fredrickson BL. The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory
of positive emotions. Am. Psychol 2001;56:218–226. [PubMed: 11315248]
37. Gray J. Emotional modulation of cognitive control: approach–withdrawal states double-dissociate
spatial from verbal two-back task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen 2001;130:436–452. [PubMed:
11561919]
38. Isen, AM. On the relationship between affect and creative problem solving. In: Russ, S., editor. Affect,
Creative Experience, and Psychological Adjustment. Taylor & Francis; 1999. p. 3-17.
39. Melton RJ. The role of positive affect in syllogism performance. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 1995;21:788–
794.
40. Bless H, et al. Mood and the use of scripts: do happy moods really make people mindless? J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol 1996;71:665–678. [PubMed: 8888596]
41. Roediger H, McDermott K. Creating false memories: remembering words not presented in lists. J.
Exp. Psychol. Learn 1995;21:803–814.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

42. Brainerd C, et al. Dual retrieval processes in free and associative recall. J. Mem. Lang 2002;46:120–
152.
43. Roediger, H., et al. Spreading activation and arousal of false memories. In: Roediger, H., et al., editors.
The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder. American Psychological
Association; 2001. p. 95-115.
44. Storbeck J, Clore GL. With sadness comes accuracy, with happiness, false memory: mood and the
false memory effect. Psychol. Sci 2005;16:785–791. [PubMed: 16181441]
45. Forgas JP, et al. Mood effects on eyewitness memory: affective influences on susceptibility to
misinformation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol 2005;41:574–588.
46. MacLeod MD. Retrieval-induced forgetting in eyewitness memory: forgetting as a consequence of
remembering. Appl. Cognit. Psychol 2002;16:135–149.
47. Bäuml K-H, Kuhbandner C. Remembering can cause forgetting – but not in negative moods. Psychol.
Sci 2007;18:111–115. [PubMed: 17425528]

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 10

48. Fiedler, K. Affective states trigger processes of assimilation and accommodation. In: Martin, LL.;
Clore, GL., editors. Theories of Mood and Cognition: A User’s Handbook. Erlbaum; 2001. p. 85-98.
49. Forgas, J. The affect infusion model (AIM): an integrative theory of mood effects on cognition and
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

judgments. In: Martin, L.; Clore, GL., editors. Theories of Mood and Cognition: A User’s Guidebook.
Erlbaum; 2001. p. 99-134.
50. Kuhl, J. A functional-design approach to motivation and self–regulation: the dynamics of personality
systems interactions. In: Boekaerts, M., et al., editors. Handbook of Self–Regulation. Academic
Press; 2000. p. 5-134.
51. Ashby F, et al. A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition. Psychol.
Rev 1999;106:529–550. [PubMed: 10467897]
52. Derryberry, D.; Tucker, DM. Motivating the focus of attention. In: Neidenthal, PM.; Kitayama, S.,
editors. The Heart’s Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. Academic Press; 1994.
p. 167-196.
53. Schwarz N, et al. Metacognitive experiences and the intricacies of setting people straight: implications
for debiasing and public information campaigns. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol 2007;39:127–161.
54. Sanna LJ, Schwarz N. Metacognitive experiences and hindsight bias: it’s not just the thought (content)
that counts! Soc. Cogn 2007;25:228–249.
55. Diener, E.; Suh, EM., editors. Culture and Subjective Well-Being. MIT Press; 2000.
56. Choi I, et al. Culture and judgment of causal relevance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol 2003;84:46–59.
[PubMed: 12518970]
57. Winkielman P, Berridge KC. Unconscious emotion. Curr. Direct. Psychol 2004;13:120–123.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

58. Bartlett, FC. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge University
Press; 1932.
59. Hanze M, Hesse F. Emotional influences on semantic priming. Cogn. Emotion 1993;7:195–205.

Glossary
Affect, representations of personal value (i.e. the goodness or badness of things). Such
representations can be neurological, physiological, experiential, cognitive, expressive and
behavioral, among others.; Affective state, the co-occurrence of several such reactions
constitutes an affective state.; Emotion, affective states with objects, reflecting an underlying
appraisal of a particular kind of situation. In addition to value information, experiencing a
specific emotion informs one that a specific set of appraisal criteria has been met. Different
emotions of the same valence can have different effects, which can be predicted on the basis
of the underlying appraisal.; Mood, diffuse, objectless affective states..
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 11
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Figure 1.
Examples of serial reproductions of a drawing from memory showing that the schema of a face
guides the construction of memory. In 1932, Frederick Bartlett [58] showed students a drawing
of an African shield, and asked them to draw it from memory. He gave their drawings to others,
asking them to reproduce the drawings from memory; these drawings were then reproduced
from memory by a third group, and so on. The drawing bore the title ‘Portrait d’homme’, and
reproductions of it gradually began to look more like a portrait of a man and less like an African
shield, as shown above. The schema of a face suggested by the title guided people’s memories,
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

illustrating Bartlett’s theory of constructive memory. This classic experiment has been recently
repeated with the addition of mood [22]. Blind ratings of the drawings that resulted showed
that those reproduced by individuals in happy moods were more face-like than those
reproduced in sad moods. Positive affect thus seems to promote the use of accessible schemas,
whereas negative affect inhibits their use, leading to more local, stimulus-bound processing.
Reprinted with permission from Ref. [58].

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 12
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Figure 2.
Global–local perception experiment. (a) Respondents indicate, in each of 24 trials, the
comparison picture (bottom) with which the target picture (top) goes. In this instance, if
participants base their similarity judgment on global features, they would say that the
comparison picture with the overall shape of a triangle is more similar to the target picture. If
they base their similarity judgments on local features, they would choose the comparison
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

picture in which the component elements are squares. Sample trial taken from Ref. [23]. (b)
Results show a more global focus for happy and neutral than for sad mood groups. Data taken
from Ref. [22].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 13
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Figure 3.
False memory experiment. Thirty-six word lists, each comprising words highly associated with
a non-presented lure, are presented. Relational processing of happy and control groups results
in the false recall of many lures, whereas item-specific processing in sad moods was more
accurate. Figure based on Ref. [44].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.
Clore and Huntsinger Page 14

Table 1
A sample of the cognitive phenomena influenced by affecta
Cognitive phenomenon Influence Refs
Semantic priming Participants in positive moods are more likely than those in negative moods to activate semantically [59]
related concepts (e.g. doctor–nurse) from memory
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Global superiority effect The general tendency to process incoming information in a global manner is empowered by positive [21,22,
mood and inhibited by negative mood 24,25]
Heuristic processing People in positive moods are more likely than those in negative moods to use judgment heuristics. [2,20]
In a persuasion context, for example, participants in positive moods are equally persuaded by strong
and weak appeals, whereas those in negative moods are persuaded more by strong appeals.
False memories When presented with a list of words (e.g. bed, rest) that implies a non-presented critical lure (e.g. [44]
sleep), participants in positive moods are more likely than those in negative moods to recall
incorrectly having seen the critical lure (show false memories).
Schema-guided memory When presented with a situation that activates a schema (e.g. eating out at a restaurant), people in [40]
positive moods are more likely than those in negative moods to use the schema to fill in the blanks
when recalling details of the situation (e.g. ordering dessert)
Retrieval-induced forgetting Retrieval-induced forgetting occurs when rehearsal of a subset of previously observed material [46,47]
inhibits memory for non-rehearsed material; research indicates that this tendency is empowered by
positive moods and inhibited by negative moods
Stereotyping Participants in positive moods tend to rely more on stereotypes to guide their thinking about members [26–29]
of various social groups than do those in negative moods, who tend to rely on individuating
information
a
Many of the hallmark findings of cognitive psychology seem to be moderated by affect. In general, this research indicates that positive affect leads to
relational (cognitive, interpretive, category-level and global) processing, whereas negative affect leads to referential (perceptual, item-level and local)
processing [5,34,48].
NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 July 23.

You might also like