Agustín Fuentes - Sex Is A Spectrum - The Biological Limits of The Binary-Princeton University Press (2025)
Agustín Fuentes - Sex Is A Spectrum - The Biological Limits of The Binary-Princeton University Press (2025)
Sex Is a Spectrum
t h e biol ogic a l l i m i ts
of t h e bi na ry
agust í n f u e n t e s
pr i ncet on u n i v e r sit y pr e ss
pr i ncet on & ox for d
Copyright © 2025 by Agustín Fuentes
Princeton University Press is committed to the protection of copyright
and the intellectual property our authors entrust to us. Copyright
promotes the progress and integrity of knowledge created by humans.
Thank you for supporting free speech and the global exchange of ideas
by purchasing an authorized edition of this book. If you wish to
reproduce or distribute any part of it in any form, please obtain
permission.
Requests for permission to reproduce material from this work
should be sent to permissions@press.princeton.edu
Published by Princeton University Press
41 William Street, Princeton, New Jersey 08540
99 Banbury Road, Oxford OX2 6JX
press.princeton.edu
All Rights Reserved
ISBN 9780691249414
ISBN (e-book) 9780691249421
British Library Cataloging-in-Publication Data is available
Editorial: Alison Kalett and Hallie Schaeffer
Production Editorial: Natalie Baan
Jacket Design: Chris Ferrante
Production: Jacquie Poirier
Publicity: Kate Farquhar-Thomson and Julia Haav
Copyeditor: Hank Southgate
This book has been composed in Arno
Printed in the United States of America
10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
c on t e n t s
Acknowledgments vii
4 Humans Then 48
5 Humans Now 74
Notes 151
Index 193
Ac k now le d g m e n t s
vii
viii A c k n o w le d g m e n t s
imagine you are a fish called the bluehead wrasse, living off
the coast of Florida. As you grow up, you, just like all to the
other bluehead wrasse your age and size, develop one set of
reproductive organs. You are what we’d call female, so you pro-
duce eggs. Th
ere is only one very large member of your group,
and they are the group male, so produce sperm. But over the
next few weeks you grow r eally fast, becoming the second-
largest fish on your reef. Then the male gets eaten. Almost im-
mediately your body starts to change, your reproductive organs
mold, shift, and alter their form. You become the group’s sperm
producer. As a bluehead wrasse, you can have one body and one
set of DNA, but multiple forms of reproductive biology across
your lifetime.
Bluehead wrasse reproductive biology is not the most com-
mon pattern in the animal kingdom, but it’s also not that weird.
When most people think of the biology of reproduction, they
typically envision two fixed kinds in each species: female and
male. This is (mostly) right when it comes to the reproductive
organs themselves, but not accurate for entire bodies and lives.
1
2 I n t r o du c t i o n
5
6 Chapter 1
Why Sex?
On the face of it, sex is a paradox. W
ouldn’t life be easier if or-
ganisms just kept copying themselves to reproduce? Why add
extra challenges and potential problems? In 1975, a classic book
on sex and evolution began with the comment, “This book is
written from a conviction that the prevalence of sexual repro-
duction in higher plants and animals is inconsistent with evo-
lutionary theory.”4 If sexual reproduction is more difficult and
complicated than asexual reproduction, why did it evolve?
T h e E volu t ion of Se x 7
into a system whereby the ovaries produce ova that are trans-
ported to an oviduct, a long tube with multiple cell types across
different parts of it. As the ova passes through the tube, they
fuse with sperm (if present), acquire various proteins and ma-
terials that make up the yolk and white of the egg, and then stop
in the shell gland. In the shell gland, the internal materials of the
egg finalize, and the hard calcium-rich exterior develops around
to contain them, after which the egg is excreted via the cloaca.
Mammals have even more complicated reproductive physi-
ology. Most have some form of external genitalia derived from
the embryonic tissues called the genital tubercle. In humans,
typically, this tissue mass develops into a penis or clitoris, with
a wide range of variation in the adult endpoints (see chapter 5).
Associated areas of tissue (the labioscrotal/urogenital fold) de-
velop into the scrotum or the labia majora, and other tissues of
the urogenital folds develop into the skin of the penis and the
internal labia (if present). Many mammals have internal testes,
but several of them (including humans) have an external scrotal
sack and testes that reside in it. All ovaries are internal.
A core difference between mammals and other animals is
that mammals gestate. Zygotes develop inside the uterus into
embryos and then fetuses.21 To accomplish this, mammals
evolved a complex system involving connection from the ova-
ries to a fallopian tube and into the uterus. In one lineage of
mammals, including us, the uterus develops a special organ
called the placenta plugging the zygote directly into the maternal
physiology and providing nutrients and removing waste. This
intense placental linkage between maternal and fetal bodies is
distinctive in the animal kingdom. It creates a suite of patterns
that structure the bodies of individuals with this reproductive
anatomy relative to individuals who do not have it.22 At the end
of gestation, one or more fetuses are birthed needing to be fed
18 Chapter 1
22
A n i m a l Se x Biol ogy 23
do guard eggs before they hatch, and other species carry the
eggs in their cloaca until they hatch rather than laying them, but
their care goes no further. Crocodilians are the only exceptions,
with adults doing some caretaking after the eggs hatch.20 It’s in
the animals that go all-out for their offspring posthatching
(birds) and postbirth (mammals) that a w hole new level of sex-
biology complexity emerges.
Birds lay very large eggs, relative to adult body size, that
hatch into young who cannot fend for themselves (this type of
offspring is called “altricial”). Birds must care for their young.
In most birds, parental care is overseen by both the large-and
small-gamete producers. Such care likely emerged in protobirds
(better known as dinosaurs) from some forms of early care of
young that increased and facilitated the development of very
large eggs and highly altricial young.21 While most birds have
biparental care of young, the details of that caregiving vary
widely. How large-and small-gamete producers contribute to
such care is related to differences in social systems, mating
systems, environmental context, and bodies.22 Parental care is
central to bird existence, but across—and occasionally within—
species, there is not a consistent one-to-one correlation be-
tween a bird’s reproductive physiology (being a large-or small-
gamete producer) and how it participates in care for young
once the eggs are laid and after the young hatch.
In many bird species, there is sexual dimorphism (differ-
ences in the shapes, sizes, and/or colors of bodies) between
large-and small-gamete producers, but what form that takes is
not consistent or in a single pattern across all species. Body-size
differences between large-and small-gamete producers often
relate to aspects of social and mating systems, to the patterns of
parental care, and to aspects of the specific ecologies of that
species. Like so many other animals, the patterns of how sex
A n i m a l Se x Biol ogy 31
potential for, and actuality of, gestation result in some very spe-
cific physiological patterns. Mammals also produce and provide
high-quality nutrition to infant(s) during the earliest stages of
life. Large-gamete-producing mammals typically can gestate and
lactate. Small-gamete-producing mammals cannot gestate
(with some variation in intersex individuals, including in
humans) and rarely lactate.26 Mammals also exhibit high levels
and a wide variety of postbirth care of their young, including
care by one adult, care by two adults, care by many adults,
and care by entire groups. It is common, but not at all ubiqui-
tous or uniform, that large-gamete producers are the primary
caretakers relative to other group members.
While sexually dimorphic patterns associated with reproduc-
tion are common in mammals, they are neither ubiquitously
present nor consistent across all species. There is variation in
body shape and size, physiological processes, hormone pat-
terns, genitals, caretaking, and a range of behavior. While one
can certainly assert that t here are typical patterns of reproduc-
tive physiology and behavior in mammals, there is simulta
neously a wide range of variation rejecting the assertion of a
single “way to be” within and between mammalian species.27
There are many successful ways to be reproductively effective
mammals. Hyenas, “monogamous” primates, and naked mole
rats offer fascinating examples.
Unlike the devious characters in the Lion King or the
scoundrels or scavengers often represented in books and nature
documentaries, spotted hyenas are active and successful hunt-
ers, form strong and deeply bonded social groups, and mess
with most peoples’ assumptions about mammalian patterns of
sex biology.28 Spotted hyena large-gamete producers are larger,
heavier, and more aggressive than small-gamete producers, and
socially dominant over them. The external genitalia of these
A n i m a l Se x Biol ogy 33
hyenas include labia that develop into what looks like a scrotum
and a clitoris that develops into what is called a “pseudopenis”
that is fully erectile and through which mating and birth take
place.29 One might think that such differences simply mean that
hyenas with ovaries are more “masculine” than those with tes-
tes. That is to say they get more androgen hormone exposure
during development and thus mimic “typical male mammal”
development. But that’s not the case. The patterns of androgen
concentrations (those sets of hormones often associated with
development of “typical” sex biology for mammals with testes)
in spotted hyenas are complex and vary by and are dependent
on age, w hether animals reside in the group they were born into
or not, their social status within the group, and on reproductive
state.30 Fetal large-gamete-producing hyenas are not “masculin-
ized” by hormones in the womb. Case in point, the develop-
ment of the genitals in hyenas with ovaries is not, as researchers
once thought, controlled by testosterone or other androgen
exposure but is, in fact, mediated by a very complex interplay
between a range of multiple hormones. This distinct physiology
is important b ecause hyenas are not “intersexed” in any manner
or form, or by any biological definition. They are neither
“hermaphroditic” or “masculinized females.” The hyena system
reflects typical genetic and internal reproductive physiology
patterns for large-gamete-producing mammals but demon-
strates a nontypical way to be a successful large-gamete-
producing mammal. Hyenas force a reevaluation of how one
can, or should, define “masculine” or “feminine” in the context
of sexual biology and motivate reflection about the value of
simple models of “male” and “female” in mammals.31
Similarly, there is a w
hole group of primates that challenges
the expected roles of large-and small-gamete producers. Most
primates live in big groups of several males and females and
34 Chapter 2
young, and o thers live in groups of one male and many females
and young. But there is also a range of species that live in small
groups of two adults and their offspring. Th ese so-called “mo-
nogamous primates” include the nighttime dwelling owl mon-
keys, the titi monkeys, the marmoset and tamarin monkeys of
South America, and the gibbons (a small ape) of Southeast
Asia. All are assumed to have a particular pattern of sex biology
and behavior that differentiates them from the assumed mam-
malian pattern of large males and smaller, infant-focused
females. The general argument is that t hese primates have a
“monogamy package” of bodies and behavior consisting of
specific deviations from the mammalian typical pattern. The
variations on the typical mammalian pattern include large-and
small-gamete producers of the same size and shape (mono-
morphic), small-gamete producers who do much or sometimes
all of the infant caretaking, and no dominance differences
across females and males. This “monogamy package,” while
deviating from the typical expected pattern for mammals, is
still based on the assumptions of anisogamy, and thus still fits
into the typical assumptions about mammalian sex biology.
The problem is that this monogamy package does not exist.32
Basically, the only thing that all of the “monogamous” primates
share in common is that they primarily live in groups of two
adults and some young. In some species that live in pairs, sex
is only between the two adults (sexual monogamy). But in
others, sex can happen with individuals outside the two-adult
group (social monogamy). In some pair-living species, small-
gamete producers are larger than large-gamete ones and do
little to no infant care, as in many other nonmonogamous pri-
mates. In others, both types of gamete producers are the same
size, or large-gamete producers might even be a bit bigger, and
small-gamete producers do at least half if not 100 percent of the
A n i m a l Se x Biol ogy 35
38
Hum a ns A r e M essy 39
or color their hair, bind their feet in shoes altering their shape,
change their noses surgically, with ornaments, and via life ex-
perience, eat a range of foods, and participate in activities that
affect growth patterns and limb morphology; they even alter
the shades of their skin by activity, clothing, chemistry, and tat-
toos. The h uman foot may be a collection of tissues, ligaments,
bones, and muscles arising from the interactions of genetics
and development, but for e very human, their foot—including
its form and function—is as much shaped by their cultural
selves as it is part of their biological structure.
How humans taste food, whom they are attracted to, the
sports they play, and how well they play them, are always af-
fected by biology and culture. Taste bud reactivity, muscles and
coordination, and the targets of sexual desire all emerge from
the mutual, and interactive, development of bodies and lived
experiences. Adult height and weight, one’s ability to score well
on standardized exams, views on raising children, resistance (or
lack thereof) to disease-causing bacteria, and even what any
given person thinks of as natural behavior for a man or a w
oman,
are a product of dynamic relationships that interweave the bio-
logical and the cultural, as well as the historical, into a single
result: us.4 Although we are biological organisms, the totality of
the h uman experience cannot be reduced to either specific in-
nate (biological) or external (environmental/cultural) influ-
ences. It is a synthesis of both: humans are biocultural.
The human brain is about 40 percent of its adult size at
birth, relatively smaller than any other mammal. Most human
brain growth happens out in the world, not in the womb. The
brain and the entire nervous system develop, create, and alter
connections and pathways and flows of biochemical signals in
constant exchange with bodies, senses, the social and physical
environments each human inhabits, including their own
42 Chapter 3
Humans Then
Primates Are Us
Primates are mammals1 and thus have a reproductive physiol-
ogy that involves shaping large-gamete-producing bodies for
potential gestation and lactation. Being mammals also indicates
48
Hum a ns Then 49
Hominins
Primates
Mammals
Gibbons are the smallest apes and are humans’ most distant
ape relatives. Gibbon females and males are mostly the same
size with similar-sized canine teeth. However, in a few species,
males are slightly bigger, especially in the siamang, and in many
gibbon species adult males and females have different-colored
fur.15 The most striking difference between many male and fe-
male gibbons are their vocalizations. All gibbon species give
long melodious vocalizations, called songs, but generally males
and females give different versions, often as complementary
duets. Gibbons primarily live in small groups, often pair-
bonded, with just two adults and some young. Female and male
behavior overlaps extensively except that females do most care
of the young, with the exception of siamangs, where males who
do a bit more care of the young than other gibbon males.16
Orangutans shared a common ancestor with the African
apes and h umans more than ten million years ago. Orangutans
are highly sexually dimorphic with adult males up to twice the
size of adult females. But t here are two different types of “male”
orangutan, both basically 3G, but at least one (or two) of the Gs
acts differently, and other aspects of their bodies and behavior
also differ from one another. Most adult males develop a set of
distinctive fatty cheek pads called “flanges” on their face and a
large throat sack that enables them to give deep and powerful
long calls. However, some adult males don’t develop the flanges
or do long calls. This “two kinds of 3G male” is an orangutan-
distinctive sex biology.17 The males that do not develop the
flanges and other aspects of large adult male appearance seem
to not have the circulating levels of a set of hormones (LH,
testosterone, and DHT) necessary for development of the
flanges and a full throat sack. However, t hese unflanged males
do have sufficient hormone levels and patterns (specifically tes-
ticular steroids and FSH) to develop full sexual function and
54 Chapter 4
Humans as Hominins
The hominins separated from other hominoids sometime be-
tween seven and ten million years ago and diversified into a
cluster of lineages of ape-like beings who walked upright on two
legs. B
ecause we are talking about organisms that lived in the
deep past (and all we have are fossils), we can’t truly assess any
of their Gs. When talking about fossils, we use the terms “male”
Hum a ns Then 57
It turns out that t here are two types of pair bond: social and
sexual. The social pair bond is a “strong biological and psycho-
logical relationship between two individuals that is measurably
different in physiological and emotional terms from general
friendships or other acquaintance relationships.” The sexual
pair bond is a “pair bond that has a sexual attraction component
such that the members of the sexual pair bond prefer to mate
with one another over other mating options.”68 In mammals,
including h umans, pair bonds combine social relations with a
range of biological activity.69
Humans have social and sexual pair bonds. H uman social
pair bonding occurs across the entire range of gender/sex and
age categories, with relatives and with unrelated individuals.70
Humans have sexual pair bonds both heterosexually and homo-
sexually. And human sexual pair bonding is not necessarily re-
lated to reproduction. Therefore, the pair bond, sexual and/or
social, while critically important in h umans, is not something
that emerged directly out of our sex biology or something that
is constrained by it.
Gender?
Gender did not appear out of thin air. H umans assign cultural
meaning to almost everything in the world, and our ancestors
began doing that a long time ago.71 Cultural meaning is assigned
not just to the material world around us but also to different
aspects of the h uman experience, like age and life-stage, body
type, hair patterns, genitals, demeanor, voice tenor, w hether
one gives birth or lactates or not, who one has sex with, caring
for infants and o thers, and much more. The social dynamic that
we call “gender” t oday emerged from complex biocultural evo-
lution in the human lineage. H uman gender concepts and
68 Chapter 4
same kinds of hand stencils often show that many of the artists’
fingers were cut at the knuckles or w
ere missing.89 It is not at all
clear if this age pattern and the mutilation of hands tells us any-
thing about gender, but it does show that the making of this
kind of art was certainly a p rocess that resulted from the inter-
weaving of biological and cultural dynamics.
Many scholars note that heightened social and material com-
plexity (more complicated technology, p olitical systems, econ-
omies) and inequality, all usually associated with patterns of
contemporary gender roles, do start showing up in many, but
not all, societies, in the most recent phase of h uman evolution,
which is the last five to eight thousand years or so.90 This sug-
gests that the current sex biology of the genus Homo, which has
been around for much more than the last eight thousand years,
did not create contemporary gender roles.91
Clearly, body size, strength, and reproductive physiology all
play substantive roles in h uman society and its structuring of
gender roles, but not always in consistent manners nor neces-
sarily in the same manners across time and culture. In the last
five to eight thousand years, contemporary gender roles became
widespread across our species, initially seen in the archeological
evidence of burial patterns and grave goods.92 But the patterns
of inequity and gender roles that emerged and became common
in certain places and times do not always map to the majority
patterns of the present day—remember the Iberian revered
leader from chapter 3 and the many recent archeological finds
of ancient “warriors” that w ere assumed to be 3G male but have
turned out to be 3G female, at least in one or more of the Gs.93
Over the last six thousand years, researchers find increased evi-
dence of difference in the bone and tooth chemistry between
3G-male and 3G-female bodies, suggesting social differences
causing differences in nutritional status. Muscle scars and wear
Hum a ns Then 73
Humans Now
74
H u m a ns Now 75
6
Number of adults
(in hundreds)
5
4
3
2
1
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
US men’s and women’s height
(in feet)
Take, for example, figure 2, which shows the usual graph for
the range of heights in the United States when comparing 3G-
male and 3G-female bodies.2 One can focus on the median or
on the shaded clusters, but what is most significant, and often
overlooked, is that if one takes away the different shading by 3G
category and the median markers, one is left with a distribution
of height wherein ~78 percent of individuals overlap and are
not sortable, by height, into a specific 3G category. So, despite
an average difference in height between 3G male and 3G fe-
males in the United States, height is an extremely unreliable
way to identify any individual’s 3G category.
The majority of human sex biology works in the same way as
height does: it is morphology with a range of expression across
human bodies. This range of variation can often be broken
76 Chapter 5
menopause are both associated with changes in fat mass and its
patterns of deposition, but the specifics of how this pattern
plays out vary dramatically across geographic and cultural
contexts. Th ere is mounting evidence that multiple X and Y
chromosome genes (plus many other genes), epigenetic pro
cesses, sociocultural stressors, and trauma influence specific
adipose deposition and metabolism. And t here are even mosaic
effects (where a person has two or more genetically different
sets of cells in their body) on different adipose locations within
the same individual.33 Adipose deposition, fat, while showing
some key average patterns, is quite variable across and within
3G-sex categories in humans.
It’s worth noting that h uman breasts and butts are distinctive
largely b ecause of fat and bipedalism. And breasts and butts
often play culturally salient roles in our gender/sexed lives. The
basic mammalian pattern of adipose deposition, the fact that
humans walk upright on two legs, and that we only have two
nipples (most mammals have multiple sets), combined with the
pull of gravity creates a distinctive look for humans. This “look”
involves breasts in most 3G females postpuberty (and in some
3G males) and pronounced buttocks for all humans, with po-
tentially relatively larger buttocks in t hose with ovaries and
uteruses. This gives human bodies a different look than other
primates. The more frequent and greater development of breast
tissue in 3G females, the slightly greater adipose deposition
rates postpuberty, and average body-size differences (smaller)
accentuate the appearance of breasts and butts on their bodies.
This small pattern of variation in fat deposition frequently
forms a basis for perceptions of 3G bodies as being more differ
ent than they are. Another visible part of our bodies that plays
an outsized role in our interpretation of gender/sex and as-
sumptions about differences is hair.
H u m a ns Now 89
Human Hair
here are two key types of hair in humans. Vellus hair—
T
nonpigmented, soft, and small—covers much of the body (and
is hard to see). Terminal hairs are longer, more rigid, more pig-
mented, penetrate further into the dermis than vellus hairs, and
are what are what most think of as “hair.” Growth and thyroid
hormones are the main actors shaping hair patterns, but in cer-
tain areas of the body (face, genital region, upper torso, thighs)
androgens (see “Hormones Are Complicated” below) also play
a major role.
Humans are weird when it comes to hair. We have relatively
scarce amounts on our body core, yet dense patches on our
heads, armpits, and around the genitals. 3G males have, on aver-
age, more and denser terminal hair than 3G females, but t here is
a lot of overlap between individuals within a group and some-
times extreme differences between populations of h umans
around the planet.34 There are groups of humans where both 3G
males and 3G females have very little body terminal hair and
other groups where all members of a group have substantial ter-
minal hair. In some groups, 3G males have higher densities and
distribution of terminal hair on the face than in other groups,
and it is not uncommon for 3G females in t hose groups to also
have greater facial hair. Hirsutism, defined as the presence of
“excess” body or facial terminal hair on 3G females, is m easured
relative to a “standard expectation” for hair growth. This is a cul-
tural, not a biological, m easure. Using this m easure, 10 to
15 percent of those who identify as women get labeled hirsute
across the human species, with some human groups having
~35 percent of 3G females incorrectly labeled as having “male-
like” hair patterns. This is incorrect because “hirsute” patterns of
hair growth are well within the typical range of variation for our
90 Chapter 5
of the pancreas, the gonads (testes and ovaries) and the hor-
mones that each of t hose glands/organs produce. Hormones
come from, and mediate, genetic action and changes in behav
ior and physiology in response to varying aspects of physical,
social, and developmental environments and experiences. And
they do so not by themselves, but via an intricate system of
varying hormone receptors and a network of feedback loops
within the endocrine system and between the endocrine sys-
tem and various organs and other physiological components of
bodies. Thinking that measuring individual hormone types or
levels is sufficient information for understanding how this sys-
tem works is incorrect and unscientific.40 Plus, all h umans have
the same hormones.41 There are no “male” or “female” hormones.
Rather, levels and actions of specific hormones, hormone re-
ceptors, and their outcomes vary with age, gonad physiology,
genetic variation, life experiences, social contexts, nutritional
state, reproductive state, and much, much more. Endocrine sys-
tem activity is deeply biocultural.42
The main hormones secreted by the gonads are estrogens,
progestogens, and androgens.43 Both ovaries and testes pro-
duce all three (and a few o thers), but the patterns of production
vary dramatically, with ovaries capable of producing higher
levels of estrogens and progestogens, and testes capable of pro-
ducing higher levels of androgens (specifically testosterone).
The gonadal production of hormones is directly connected to
and mediated by the hypothalamus and the pituitary. Ovaries
and testes are variants on the same theme, not different kinds
of organ.44 However, gonadal actions and their outcomes vary
in patterned manners.45 Usually, t here is a concordance be-
tween the presence of ovaries or testes and specific other Gs
(genetics and genitals). However, t here is a nonnegligible range
of variation in the specifies of gene action/inaction, gonadal
H u m a ns Now 93
t hose with ovaries, but drop after mini-puberty, and all h umans
have about the same testosterone levels u ntil true puberty. At
puberty, all humans’ testosterone rises, but those with testes
typically undergo a larger spike. As adults, most humans with
testes typically have circulating testosterone levels on average
about ten to e ighteen times higher than t hose with ovaries.
However, t here are a number of variants on this theme, both in
humans with testes who produce very low amounts of testos-
terone and in variations such as PCOS (Polycystic ovary syn-
drome), which occurs in five to twenty percent of human adults
with two X chromosomes and often results in high levels of
androgens (especially testosterone) produced.52 As with all
hormones, the varying levels between bodies at different points
in development and life history can have differing effects on
various aspects of the developing body, physiology, and behav
ior. The patterns of hormone variation matter for human bod-
ies, but they are not binary in nature or form.
One can’t do a review of hormones without talking a bit
more about testosterone. “T” holds an almost mythical role in
the public view, and many think T is what “makes” a “man.” But
is that correct? T is probably the most scientifically researched,
discussed, and debated, and certainly the most publicly misun-
derstood of the gonadal hormones. On the one hand, there are
researchers who believe that testosterone’s main job is to sup-
port the anatomy, physiology, and behavior that increases a
male’s reproductive output—T helps them [males] reproduce
and direct energy to be used in ways that support competition
for mates, in short, that testosterone is the biological and
evolved “difference” between males and females.53 Others argue
that “T” is best understood in the context of a dynamic and
malleable set of relationships. These scholars suggest that T is
neither the creator of masculinity nor the essence of maleness,
96 Chapter 5
Brains/Neurobiology
Minds are a core to how we perceive, interpret, and respond to
the world. And at the heart of the h uman mind is the h uman
brain. There are some patterns of variation between the genders
of men and women across human cultures in behavior, psycho-
logical illness, and the manners in which one perceives sights,
sounds, and other signals from the world. This is often assumed
to be due to biological “differences” in the brains of 3G males
and 3G females. And, as usual, t hose h umans who do not fit into
the 3G categories are largely ignored in this area of research.
Are there male and female brains? A group of researchers
reviewed hundreds of studies (conducted over more than thirty
years) of human brain imaging and physical postmortem brain
analyses. They discovered few patterned variants between 3G
categories despite the strong history of repeated claims of “sex
difference” in the h uman brain.68 As with much in h uman bod-
ies, 3G-male brains are on average larger than 3G-female brains
from birth and stabilize at about 11 percent larger as adults. This
size difference is often assumed to be distinct from the other
commonly reported, but largely inaccurate, “differences” in
brains: a higher white/gray m atter ratio, more intra-versus in-
terhemispheric connectivity, and higher regional cortical and
subcortical volumes in 3G males. There is also some reported
3G-related variation in the connectome (a comprehensive map
of neural connections in the brain), and there are relatively
H u m a ns Now 101
variation but argue that the patterns represented are not dimor-
phic, but are a continuum, with 3G categories exhibiting mean
or variability differences (as with height, but much more over-
lap).74 In other words, even for researchers who think these
results demonstrate significant patterned variation, t here are
not two types of brain (male and female), but rather t here is
one range of brain function and 3G individuals are distributed
along it with extensive overlap and some small differentiation.
Interestingly, a study published a few years before on the same
UK Biobank dataset, but a smaller sample and different meth-
odology, also found some patterns of variation by 3G category,
but they w ere different ones, and many w
ere greatly reduced or
disappeared with size corrections.75 Finally, the researchers
from the large overview presented above redid their key com-
parisons to include the UK Biobank study and five o thers stud-
ies looking at the same dataset or using similar methods and
discovered that the results of all six varied in locations of varia-
tion, direction of the variation, and w hether or not variation
occurred in a given location in the brain.76 The is no single, or
same, pattern of variation across all studies. The current data
and analyses demonstrate that there is little evidence for sub-
stantive structural differences across most of the brain, aside
from those related to overall size.
This is not to say that brains in 3G-male and 3G-female bod-
ies, t hose of p eople who do not fit into 3G categories, t hose of
men and women, and those of very masculine or very feminine
individuals, do not respond, or function, somewhat differently
in response to specific stimuli, contexts, and actions; some-
times they do.77 The fact that brain activity and the functioning
of various aspects of brains varies by life experience, nutritional
status, exposure to trauma, age, gender, degree of masculinity
and femininity, and a myriad of other sociocultural and
104 Chapter 5
DNA
I end this overview of contemporary h uman sex biology with
the place most people want to start: DNA. Specifically, the first
“G” in 3G: the genes on the twenty-third chromosome. This
chromosome pair comes in two forms: X and Y. Th ese chromo-
somes are often called the “sex chromosomes” because they
contain many genes related to the development of gonads and
other elements of the reproductive tract. But as the X and Y
chromosomes themselves are neither sufficient to “produce
sex” nor limited to directing the development of sex biology,
the very heart of the supposed binary of sex is neither a binary
nor just about sex biology.82 The X chromosome has about 155
million base pairs of DNA and about 900 genes, not all of which
H u m a ns Now 105
Normal?
The actual biological variation of any human trait comes with a
set of ideas about what constitutes a “normal” version of that
trait. This idea of “normal” is also almost always accompanied
by opinions about what that trait should be like. But this should
give us pause and makes us ask the question: what is “normal”
human biology?90
The average measure in a range of distribution of a given trait
is called the “mean” of that range. It is not necessarily the best
or ideal form, nor is it necessarily the most functional, adaptive,
or even aesthetic. It is a statistical representation of the sum of
the range of values in a given m easure divided by the total num-
ber of values in the dataset. In many of the traits related to sex
108 Chapter 5
biology that vary across h uman bodies, the mean has a cultural
value, and it is thought to be “normal.” And people are judged
against it. Therefore, when asking about aspects of sex biology,
the range of variation and functions or lack of function as-
sociated with that range should be the focus, not necessarily the
mean or some subset of the typical distribution. Comparing
means of some traits, like height and hair, have far more cul-
tural meaning than biological significance. Other traits, such as
gonad type or specific hormone levels, can have equally salient
cultural and biological significance, but the two might not be
connected in a simple fashion. Still other traits, like muscle
strength, vary in their salience and importance depending on
what specific questions are being asked and what comparisons
are being made. In the reality of human sex biology, means,
ranges, and traits have both cultural and biological implications
that are almost always entangled.
Ultimately, there are some important patterns of variation
between 3G categories, but there is a much more overlap in
functions and outcomes than can be captured by the concept
of sexual dimorphism or “sex differences.”91 “Female” and
“male” do mean something biologically but are not two diff erent
kinds.92 X and Y chromosomes, gonads, pregnancy, circulating
hormone levels, genetic processes, developmental dynamics,
musculature, hair growth, adipose deposition, and brain mor-
phology and function all impact the human experience. But
these aspects of our biology, and their variation, are not best
understood as binary sex differences. H umans are biocultural
and have gender/sex. Variation is our norm, and our variation
does not reflect two kinds of human.
6
in 1993, a book called Men Are from Mars, W omen Are from
Venus hit the bestseller list and stayed there for 121 weeks. In it,
the author and relationship counselor John Gray claimed that
men, like the Roman god of war, Mars, are by nature aggressive
and violent, but protectors, and w omen are emotive, coy, and
maternal like the goddess of love, Venus. He told the readers
that successful male-female romantic relationships are based on
recognizing natural (biological) differences in communication,
emotion, and behavior between men and women.1
Sound familiar?
Charles Darwin told us that “man is more courageous,
pugnacious, and energetic than woman” with “more inventive
genius.”2 Angus Bateman claimed males are promiscuous and
females sexually passive, coy, and choosy, and E. O. Wilson told
us that the evolved, biological differences between men and
women naturally led to universal male dominance.3 Too many
biologists and other kinds of scholars across the nineteenth and
twentieth centuries made these kinds of assertions. And these
assertions, just like t hose of John Gray, are wrong.4 We know
that 3G females are not necessarily sexually passive and that 3G
males are not necessarily more courageous or energetic, nor
109
110 Chapter 6
below); it’s just that for us sexual behavior also has substantive
cultural connotations and consequences. And t hese cultural
realties in much of the contemporary world often restrict the
expression of sexuality by t hose classified as female. Thus, mea-
sures of sexual behavior need to take that into account. As the
biologist Anne Fausto-Sterling has eloquently and repeatedly
demonstrated, “Sexuality is a somatic fact created by cultural ef-
fect.”15 In other words, sexuality is not strictly programmed by
our genes, or genitals, but rather emerges in concert with the
gender/sex experience of humans as they develop.
Actual data on sexual activity are difficult to collect and
quantify, but one can look to the 2010 National Survey of Sexual
Health and Behavior, a US-based study of 5,865 adolescents and
adults (2,936 self-identified men [gender] and 2,929 self-
identified w omen [gender] ages fourteen to ninety-four) to get
a general idea.16 In the study, 55 percent of the men reported
masturbation in the past month, and 71 percent in the last year;
31 percent of the w omen reported masturbation in the last
month, and 54 percent in the last year, except t hose over sev-
enty. 85 percent of men in their twenties and thirties reported
having vaginal intercourse in the last year, compared to
74 percent in their forties, 58 percent in their fifties, 54 percent
in their sixties, and 43 percent in their seventies. For the women,
81 percent in their twenties and thirties reported having vaginal
intercourse in the last year, compared to 70 percent in their for-
ties, 51 percent in their fifties, 42 percent in their sixties, and
22 percent in their seventies. Men and women of all age groups
reported engaging in oral sex and masturbation with a partner.
For both oral sex and partnered masturbation, the man/woman
pattern is almost identical. More than 20 percent of the men
between ages twenty-five to twenty-nine reported anal sex, with
younger and older men reporting much lower numbers. More
116 Chapter 6
larger body size and greater upper-body muscle mass and there-
fore can pose a potentially greater risk of harm in purely physi-
cal fights than do smaller 3G females (assuming no training in
hand-to-hand combat). Across many societies, young adult
men (the gender), on average, tend to participate in some cul-
turally defined categories of physical risk-taking behavior at
higher rates than young adult w omen (the gender).28 In many
societies, men engage in overall higher rates of physical aggres-
sion than w omen, especially in conflicts with other men. But in
aggression between heterosexual partners t here is very little
difference in the use of physical aggression, with women being
slightly more likely to use it.29 However, in this same context
(heterosexual partnerships/pairs), physical aggression by men
often causes more injury than that by women. This is not sur-
prising given that 3G males are on average slightly larger than
the woman they are fighting with, and often have a greater
upper-body strength due to both physiological and cultural pat-
terns, and are likely to have more previous experience in physi-
cal fighting. This outcome is a biocultural blend of patterns, not
a smoking gun for evolved differences in physical violence be-
tween 3G categories.
There is a particular consistency in certain reported patterns
in aggression and violence, especially at the level of homicide
and sexual assault/rape, when using the 3G-male/3G-female
comparison.30 Some have argued that this is related to the
differences in circulating testosterone and/or the evolution of
violence and aggression as a basal adaptation of 3G-male behav
ior. However, t here is no solid evidence from the h uman evolu-
tionary record or human physiology to support such an asser-
tion.31 While 3G males are indeed the initiators of the vast
majority of homicide and sexual assault/rape in our species, it
is also true that most 3G males are never involved in such acts.32
120 Chapter 6
are almost always small or very small and usually in the midst of
massively overlapping variation. A focus on means obscures the
likely more biologically relevant realities and dynamics of varia-
tion in h uman bodies and minds. If one seeks to validate as-
sumptions about difference by looking only for difference, then
that is all one sees. The patterns of variation in a forest are often
more important than the average height of certain types of trees.
The same goes for humans and human societies.
When the great overlap (and complexity) in sexuality, ag-
gression, and cognition fails to support the battle of the sexes
argument, its proponents often turn to the costs of reproduc-
tion as a trump card. But as we already know, scientific evidence
about h uman reproduction and the evolution of h uman care-
taking systems rejects the simple “large-gamete producers
versus small-gamete producers” view of the world. To summa-
rize: parental investment in humans is more complex than one
3G male and one 3G female investing in reproduction. There is
massive evidence that the genus Homo evolved complex coop-
erative caretaking of young early in the Pleistocene and that by
the mid-Pleistocene human childhoods had extended substan-
tially b ecause of it.38 The evolutionary implication of this reality
rejects a s imple calculus based on individual 3G females’ costs
of gestation and lactation and includes a caretaking regime
where the mother is one of many caretakers and the range of the
specific infant caretaking energetic burdens is redistributed
across the group. This is not to say that the “costs” of reproduc-
tion are not high in the genus Homo, but rather that the costs
have, in an evolutionary response, been socially and physiologi-
cally mediated and spread across group members to enable the
complex, extended childhood associated with a range of capaci-
ties connected to substantial neurobiological and social devel-
opment after birth.
122 Chapter 6
125
126 Chapter 7
and “binary” are seldom the most effective ways to talk or think
about h uman gender/sex.2 This knowledge is powerf ul, but
only if it is shared and used. In that spirit, this final chapter of-
fers brief examples of a few areas in contemporary society
where binary views of sex biology and gender are invoked, and
it illustrates why such actions are both wrong and harmful.
Making a Family
In 2020, the Republican governor of the US state of Tennessee,
Bill Lee, signed a bill to ensure that foster care and adoption
agencies could exclude LGBTQ families and o thers based on
“religious beliefs.”10 By 2024, there were at least thirteen states
in the United States that had similar laws.11 The laws are based
on false beliefs about what is biologically “natural,” and there-
fore “right” for humans. These laws assume that only hetero-
sexual pairings of certain types of p eople (cisgender) provide
appropriate and successful conditions for the rearing of
children. This is factually incorrect, as the c hildren of LGBTQ
parents fare just as well as c hildren of non-LGBTQ parents.12
But t hese beliefs and real l egal structures are not about the data
or facts; rather, they reflect a deep commitment to the logic of
the sex-biology binary: if female bodies are made for repro-
duction and caretaking, and male bodies for protection and
provisioning, then the biological (natural) basis for f amily is a
heterosexual male and female couple.
The core of mammalian reproduction is gametic fusion in a
body capable of gestating and birthing. But the necessary bio-
logical details for the creation and gestating of an embryo tell
130 Chapter 7
Medicine
Historically in the medical sciences, researchers were often
hesitant to use female animals in laboratory studies. Male lab
animals (mostly mice) outnumber female ones by a factor of at
least five, especially in tests focused on obtaining species-wide
results.14 Traditionally, medical researchers considered female
animals, including humans, too behaviorally and hormonally
unstable and physiologically complicated to give generalizable
results in many cases.15 In the medical binary view of sex biol-
ogy, there are two types in e very species, the male and the fe-
male. The male is the standard, the stable specimen, b ecause his
only contribution to reproduction is sperm. The female, with
her instability, is s haped by her possession of a uterus and her
reproductive cycling; thus approaches to her biology, and
health, are centered around her capacity to reproduce.16
And that capacity is thought to involve messy hormones, be
havior, and physiology, rendering the female too complicated
and a poor subject for general assessment of nonreproductive-
related aspects of biology, health, and well-being. Because of
this belief, many experiments, or rather experimenters, assume
that how something affects a 3G male (be it a mouse or h uman)
is the best proxy for the rest of the species. This approach as-
sumes minimal relevant variation among 3G males and too
much variation among 3G-female bodies. This belief has kept
female animals, and animals who do not fit neatly into 3G-male
132 Chapter 7
Cardiac Disease
Since 1984, more w omen than men have died of ischemic
heart disease and heart failure each year, and yet more men
(here meaning 3G males) have been diagnosed with heart disease
than women (here meaning 3G females).32 Doctors diagnose
heart conditions by a combination of clinical presentation, bio-
markers, and imaging. Obstructive coronary artery disease
(CAD) remains the current focus of most assessment and ther-
apeutic strategies, but women have lower rates of typical CAD
than men. Women also have a higher rate of diverse symptoms
136 Chapter 7
Organ Transplants
While t here is no evidence that most organs originate in a bi-
nary fashion (3G-male and 3G-female organs) in humans, there
are on average worse outcomes for transplants from 3G-female
donors regardless of the 3G sex of the recipient. Also, kidneys
and hearts (but not livers) transplanted from 3G females
have specifically higher failure rates in 3G-male recipients.37
One might take a binary view and argue that this indicates some
level of “female” organs and some kind of incompatibly with
“male” immune systems. And one would likely be wrong. It
W h y t h e B i n a r y V i e w I s a P r ob l e m 137
Not all of those who get pregnant and give birth are classified or
identify as w omen, or 3G females, and t here are also plenty of
individuals who have the physiology for gestation and lactation
but cannot or do not get pregnant and give birth. Thus, the focus
of research into pregnancy and birth should not simply be on
women or 3G females as one of the two categories in a binary.
Rather the focus should be on the patterns, dynamics, and ex-
periences of the range of t hose who can and do get pregnant. For
example, in 2020 the National Academies of Science, Engineer-
ing, and Medicine, examining the data and relevant patterns of
variation changed their terms for research in this arena to “preg-
nant people” or “pregnant individuals” in place of “pregnant
women.”39
While sex biology plays a core role in pregnancy, a mountain
of data supports the assertion that the individual experience of
pregnancy, like so much else in human lives, strongly inter-
twines with social, economic, p olitical, racialized, and related
aspects.40 Even something as specific as where one gives birth
can have dramatic impacts on bodies and lives.41 Pregnancy, like
so much of the human, is also biocultural. There is a substantial
range of physiological variation between individuals who can
and do get pregnant in terms of length of gestation (which varies
by as much as many weeks), hypertensive disorders, labor/
delivery experience, and much more.42 Given this range of varia-
tion in biology, and the experience and context of pregnancy
and birth, rather than seeing w omen, p eople who can get
pregnant, and people who do get pregnant as a monolithic cat-
egory, it makes better scientific and social sense to examine the
patterns and processes of the variation across and among t hose
who do get pregnant to better facilitate healthy outcomes.
Humans who gestate and give birth undergo a suite of physi-
ological dynamics that are not present in the same form and
W h y t h e B i n a r y V i e w I s a P r ob l e m 139
variables that may offer specific benefits. They are also aspects
of human bodies that are affected, often substantially, by variation
in sex biology. Because these variants do manifest, on average,
proportionately differently in 3G females and 3G males, and
these 3G categories are most often societally correlated with the
categories men and women, many sports today have developed
men’s and woman’s sports where the competitions are gender-
uniform, such as basketball, baseball, football (soccer), tennis,
and track and field events.53 Given recent societal changes (in
some places) to accept a broader range of gendered identities,
there are increasing situations where individuals who w ere as-
signed a gender at birth and have changed that gender (are now
transgendered individuals) wish to compete in their current
gender category. At the same time, t here are also individuals, as
with Ms. Semenya, assigned a gender at birth, who remain in
that gender, but do not fully fit the current assumptions about
the biology “underlying” that gender, who are being excluded
from competitions based on a highly specific (and culturally
structured) definition of “woman” (basically, how much testos-
terone the body naturally produces). It is worth noting that
being of the right “sex” is brought up as an issue in women’s
sports but not in men’s.
Before making any further comments on t hese cases, we
must recognize the fact that women’s professional and amateur
sports are relatively new as organizations and institutions, un-
derfunded, and undersupported relative to men’s sports. There is
abundant evidence that societal gender structures and gender/
sex dynamics of girls’ and w omen’s lives structure their bodies
and possibilities affecting their capacities, performances, and
risk of injury in sports differently than those of boys and men
who are trained for sports.54 This gendered training dynamic is
illustrated in running sports, where the gender differentials in
144 Chapter 7
Restrooms
In 2016, the US state of North Carolina passed a law prohibiting
transgender individuals from using the restroom that corre-
sponded to their gender. This action was part of the political,
legal, and public debates over whether transgender individuals
should be able to use public restrooms (including locker rooms
and changing rooms) that match their gender or if they should
be forced to use ones that match a social categorization of the
person based on which genitals they have (or had) and how
they were assigned at birth. Between 2021 and 2023, legislatures
in 34 US states introduced over 300 anti-transgender bills re-
flecting a wide range of proposed restrictions on issues of
health, legal rights, and access for individuals who did not fit
certain beliefs about gender and sex biology.64 By early 2024,
there w ere more than 450 active anti-transgender bills moving
through the legislatures in 41 states.65 In June 2023, the state of
Florida implemented H ouse Bill 1521, which mandates that
transgender individuals must use public restrooms that corre-
spond with the sex assigned to them at birth, even if they have
legally updated their gender on their birth certificate and driv-
er’s license. If they do not comply, they can be charged with
trespassing—punishable by up to a year in jail. In January of
2024, Utah followed with a similar law. These laws and their
associated outcomes are a clear, powerful, and targeted attempt,
148 Chapter 7
151
152 Not e s to Ch a p t er 1
7. Since this book is really just focused on the animals, we won’t even try to get
into the sex biology of plants, which is far more complicated. If you are interested,
read D. Charlesworth. 2002. “Plant Sex Determination and Sex Chromosomes.” He-
redity 88: 94–101. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.hdy.6800016; J. R. Pannell. 2017. “Plant
Sex Determination.” Current Biology 27 (5): R191–R197. https://.doi.org/10.1016/j
.cub.2 017.01.052; and especially B. Subramaniam and M. Bartlett. 2023. “Re-
imagining Reproduction: The Queer Possibilities of Plants.” Integrative and Com-
parative Biology 63 (4): 946–59. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icad012.
8. T. Laqueur. 1990. Making Sex: Body and Gender from the Greeks to Freud. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
9. V. Sanz. 2017. “No Way Out of the Binary: A Critical History of the Scientific
Production of Sex.” Signs: Journal of Women in Culture and Society 43 (1): 1–27.
https://doi.org/10.1086/692517; S. Richardson. 2013. Sex Itself: The Search for Male
and Female in the Human Genome. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; see also
E. Martin. 1991. “The Egg and the Sperm: How Science Has Constructed a Romance
Based on Stereotypical Male-Female Roles.” Signs 16 (3): 485–501.
10. C. Darwin. 1871. The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London:
John Murry.
11. A. J. Bateman. 1948. “Intrasexual Selection in Drosophila.” Heredity 2:349–68.
12. Bateman’s assertions were that females are l imited in how many gametes they
can produce due to the size and energetic costs of ova, and thus their investment in
each gamete is very high. Basically, Bateman (working with fruit flies) argued that
males can produce unlimited amounts of small sperm, while females can only pro-
duce a limited number of large ova. So, given this difference in costs to the organisms,
males should try to mate with as many females as possible (get as much sperm out
there as possible), but females, with costly, l imited ova, should be coy, sexually pas-
sive, and very choosy. This is what Darwin said in 1871 about animal evolution in
general (including humans), but in 1948, Angus Bateman put the cause of this as-
sumed reality squarely on the fact of anisogamy.
13. G. C. Williams. 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press; R. Trivers. 1972. “Parental Investment and Sexual Selection.” In Sexual
Selection and the Descent of Man, edited by B. Campbell, 52–95. Chicago: Aldine.
14. E. O. Wilson. 1975. Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap
Press of Harvard University Press; G. A. Parker. 1979. “Sexual Selection and Sexual
Conflict.” In Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition, edited by M. S. Blum and
N. A. Blum, 123–66. New York: Academic Press. Please note all of the most promi-
nent scholars making t hese arguments are men: Darwin, Bateman, R. L. Trivers,
E. O. Wilson, and G. A. Parker.
15. Z. Tang-Martinez and T. B. Ryder. 2005. “The Problem with Paradigms: Bate-
man’s Worldview as a Case Study.” Integrative and Comparative Biology 45: 821–30.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 1 153
See also P. Gowaty, Y. Kim, and W. Anderson. 2012. “No Evidence of Sexual Selection
in a Repetition of Bateman’s Classic Study of Drosophila melanogaster.” Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109: 11740–45.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1207851109; H. Kokko and M. Jennions. 2003. “It
Takes Two to Tango.” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18 (3): 103–4; C. M. Drea. 2005.
“Bateman Revisited: The Reproductive Tactics of Female Primates.” Integrative and
Comparative Biology 45 (5): 915–23. https://d oi .o rg /10 .1093 /i cb /4 5 .5 .915;
M. Borgerhoff-Mulder. 2004. “Are Men and Women Really So Different?” Trends in
Ecology and Evolution 19 (1): 3–6.
16. M. Ah-King. 2013. “On Anisogamy and the Evolution of ‘Sex Roles.’ ” Trends in
Ecology and Evolution 28 (1): 1–2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2012.0 4.0 04. A.
Fausto-Sterling. 2020. Sexing the Body: Gender Politics and the Construction of Sexual-
ity. London: Hachette; L. Z. DuBois and H. Shattuck-Heidorn. 2021. “Challenging
the Binary: Gender/Sex and the Bio-logics of Normalcy.” American Journal of H uman
Biology 33 (5): e23623. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23623; J. F. McLaughlin,
Kinsey M. Brock, I. Gates, P. Anisha, M. Piattoni, A. Rossi, and S. E. Lipshutz. 2023.
“Multivariate Models of Animal Sex: Breaking Binaries Leads to a Better Understand-
ing of Ecology and Evolution.” Integrative and Comparative Biology 63 (4): 891–906.
https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icad027; C. T. Ross, P. L. Hooper, J. E. Smith, A. V.
Jaeggi, E. A. Smith, S. Gavrilets, F. T. Zohora, et al. 2023. “Reproductive Inequality in
Humans and Other Mammals.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America 120 (22): e2220124120. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas
.2220124120.
17. Much of the biological literature on nonhuman animals still uses the term
“hermaphrodite” for individuals who have both gonad types (or an ovotestis). “Her-
maphrodite” is no longer used for h umans, and increasingly less so for other animals,
due to historical baggage and the fact that the term “intersex” is more expansive and
encompasses multiple biological variants regarding gonads and other sex biology.
18. Except not always. There are a number of fish and reptiles that make these
structures but still store them internally u ntil the zygotes develop into juvenile or-
ganisms and are capable of making it on their own. Then they excrete them.
19. Many egg-laying species also have genitals for assisting in the internal fusion
of gametes.
20. For relatively concise and pretty good overviews of animal reproductive
systems, see G. C. Kent. 2021. “Animal Reproductive System.” Encyclopedia Britan-
nica, November 19. https://w ww.britannica.com/science/animal-reproductive
-system.
21. This system is a bit different in marsupial and placental mammals. In placen-
tals, all the fetal development is internal, but in marsupials the last portions of it take
place in a sack external to the reproductive tract.
154 Not e s to Ch a p t er 1
in True Hermaphrodites and All Male Offspring to Date.” Obstetric Gynecology 113:
534–36. Also, there are a few species of bats where small-gamete producers lactate.
27. Cooke, Bitch; Drea, “Bateman Revisited”; S. R. Zajitschek, F. Zajitschek,
R. Bonduriansky, R. C. Brooks, W. Cornwell, D. S. Falster, M. Lagisz, et al. 2020.
“Sexual Dimorphism in Trait Variability and Its Eco-evolutionary and Statistical
Implications.” Elife (9): e63170. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63170.
28. See C. Delle-Amores. 2024. “Love Them or Hate Them, Hyenas Are Getting
the Last Laugh.” National Geographic Magazine, February 8. https://w ww
.nationalgeographic.com/animals/article/spotted-hyenas-queens.
29. S. E. Glickman, L. G. Frank, J. M. Davidson, E. R. Smith, and P. K. Siiteri. 1987.
“Androstenedione May O rganize or Activate Sex-Reversed Traits in Female Spotted
Hyenas.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
84 (10): 3444–47. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.84.10.3444.
30. A. Conley, N. J. Place, E. L. Legacki, G. L. Hammond, G. R. Cunha, C. M. Drea,
M. L. Weldele, et al. 2020. “Spotted Hyaenas and the Sexual Spectrum: R eproductive
Endocrinology and Development.” Journal of Endocrinology 247 (1): R27–R44.
https://doi.org/10.1530/JOE-20-0252.
31. Conley et al., “Spotted Hyaenas and the Sexual Spectrum.”
32. And, in fact, t here is a solid argument that “monogamy” is not one thing.
There can be social monogamy (but having sex with more than one partner) and/
or sexual monogamy (sex with only one partner). See A. Fuentes. 1999.
“Re-evaluating Primate Monogamy.” American Anthropologist 100 (4): 890–907; A.
Fuentes. 2002. “Patterns and Trends in Primate Pair Bonds.” International Journal of
Primatology 23 (4): 953–78; U. Reichard. 2017. “Monogamy.” In The International
Encyclopedia of Primatology, edited by A. Fuentes, 831–35. London: John Wiley &
Sons; E. Fernandez-Duque, M. Huck, S. Van Belle, and A. Di Fiore. 2020. “The Evo-
lution of Pair-Living, Sexual Monogamy, and Cooperative Infant Care: Insights from
Research on Wild Owl Monkeys, Titis, Sakis, and Tamarins.” American Journal of
Physical Anthropology 171 (suppl. 70): 118–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.24017.
33. M. M. Holmes and B. D. Goldman. 2021. “Social Behavior in Naked Mole-
Rats: Individual Differences in Phenotype and Proximate Mechanisms of
Mammalian Eusociality.” Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology 1319: 35–58.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-65943-1_2.
34. S. E. Glickman, R. V. Short, and M. B. Renfree. 2005. “Sexual Differentiation
in Three Unconventional Mammals: Spotted Hyenas, Elephants and Tammar Wal-
labies.” Hormones and Behavior 48: 403–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. yhbeh.2005.07
.013; Cooke, Bitch; Drea, “Bateman Revisited”; Zajitschek et al., “Sexual Dimor-
phism in Trait Variability.”
35. Cooke, Bitch.
158 Not e s to Ch a p t er 3
15. Nanda, Gender Diversity. See also Hyde et al., “Future of Sex and Gender in
Psychology.”
16. This is especially prominent in gender system of primarily patriarchal socie
ties. A. Saini. 2023. The Patriarchs: The Origins of I nequality. New York: Penguin Press.
17. B. E. deMayo, A. E. Jordan, and K. R. Olson. 2022. “Gender Development in
Gender Diverse C hildren.” Annual Review of Developmental Psychology 4 (1): 207–29;
Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other Lies; A. Fausto-Sterling. 2020. Sexing the
Body: Gender Politics and the Construction of Sexuality. London: Hachette; C. Fine.
2010. Delusions of Gender: How Our Minds, Society, and Neurosexism Create Difference.
New York: W. W Norton.
18. S. M. van Anders. 2015. “Beyond Sexual Orientation: Integrating Gender/Sex
and Diverse Sexualities in Sexual Configurations Theory.” Archives of Sexual Behavior
44: 1177–213.
19. N. Krieger. 2019. “Measures of Racism, Sexism, Heterosexism, and Gender
Binarism for Health Equity Research: From Structural Injustice to Embodied
Harm—An Ecosocial Analysis.” Annual Review of Public Health 41: 37–62; M. Yudell,
D. Roberts, R. DeSalle, and S. Tishkoff. 2016. “Taking Race Out of Human Genetics.”
Science 351 (6273): 564–65. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4951; M. Lock. 2015.
“Comprehending the Body in the Era of the Epigenome.” Current Anthropology 56
(2): 151–77; Fuentes, “Humans Are Biocultural”; Fuentes and Wiessner, “Reintegrat-
ing Anthropology”; Downey and Lende, “Evolution and the Brain”; Leatherman and
Goodman, “Building on the Biocultural Syntheses”; Ingold and Paalson, Biosocial
Becomings.
20. H ere and in the following definitions I am drawing on a range of literature,
but most specifically the excellent summaries of that literature and the definitions
provided in Hyde et al., “Future of Sex and Gender”; L. Z. DuBois and H. Shattuck-
Heidorn. 2021. “Challenging the Binary: Gender/Sex and the Bio-logics of Nor-
malcy.” American Journal of Human Biology 33 (5): e23623. https://doi.org/10.1002
/ajhb. 23623; van Anders, “Gender/Sex/ual Diversity and Biobehavioral Research”;
and A. Fausto-Sterling. 2019. “Gender/Sex, Sexual Orientation, and Identity Are in
the Body: How Did They Get There?” The Journal of Sex Research 56 (4–5): 529–55.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0 0224499.2019.1581883; S. Richardson. 2013. Sex Itself:
The Search for Male and Female in the H uman Genome. Chicago: The University of
Chicago Press.
21. D. Joel. 2012. “Genetic-Gonadal-Genitals Sex (3G-Sex) and the Misconcep-
tion of Brain and Gender, or, Why 3G-Males and 3G-Females Have Intersex Brain
and Intersex Gender.” Biology of Sex Differences 3 (1): 27; DuBois and Shattuck-
Heidorn, “Challenging the Binary.”
22. The study might not explicitly state this, but it is almost always the case that
when the researcher says “male” and “female,” they mean 3G male and 3G female,
Not e s to Ch a p t er 4 161
but in reality, researchers are almost always only taking the “sex at birth” assignment
unless stated otherwise in the a ctual research work.
23. Most researchers assume they are using something like 3G categories, but in
reality, they are often using self-reported “sex” (male/female) or “gender” (man/
woman) or “sex” assessed based on physical observation of overall appearance. This
means that there is usually some small error in the assignments and thus the data are
often slightly askew.
24. M. Blackless, A. Charuvastra, A. Derryck, A. Fausto-Sterling, K. Lauzanne,
and E. Lee. 2000. “How Sexually Dimorphic Are We? Review and Synthesis.” Ameri-
can Journal of Human Biology 12 (2): 151–66.
4. Humans Then
1. Here I leave out the marsupials, who are very fascinating, but not as central to
this discussion as are the placental mammals (as that is what humans are).
2. Relative to other nonmammal animals. Birds do have extended care, but not
at the physiological and behavioral level of mammals.
3. S. Hrdy. 2009. Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Under-
standing. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; E. Fernandez-Duque, C. R.
Valeggia, and S. P. Mendoza. 2009. “The Biology of Paternal Care in H uman and
Nonhuman Primates.” Annual Review of Anthropology 38 (1): 115–30; S. Rosenbaum
and J. B. Silk. 2022. “Pathways to Paternal Care in Primates.” Evolutionary Anthropol-
ogy 31 (5): 245–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21942.
4. Hrdy, Mothers and Others; C. Ross and A. MacLarnon. 2000. “The Evolution
of Non-maternal Care in Anthropoid Primates: A Test of the Hypotheses.” Folia
Primatol (Basel) 71 (1–2): 93–113. https://doi.org/10.1159/000021733; S. Rosen-
baum and L. T. Gettler. 2018. “With a Little Help from Her Friends (and Family) Part
I: The Ecology and Evolution of Non-maternal Care in Mammals.” Physiology &
Behavior 193, Part A: 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.025.
5. E. Clarke, K. Bradshaw, K. Drissell, P. Kadam, N. Rutter, and S. Vaglio. 2022.
“Primate Sex and Its Role in P leasure, Dominance and Communication.” Animals 12
(23): 3301. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12233301.
6. A. Whiten, R. A. Hinde, K. N. Laland, and C. B. Stringer. 2011. “Culture
Evolves.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences
366 (1567): 938–48. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0372; H. Whitehead, K. N.
Laland, L. Rendell, R. Thorogood, and A. Whiten. 2019. “The Reach of Gene–
Culture Coevolution in Animals.” Nature Communications 10 (2405): https://doi.org
/10.1038/s41467-019-10293-y.
7. J. M. Plavcan. 2012. “Sexual Size Dimorphism, Canine Dimorphism, and Male-
Male Competition in Primates.” Human Nature 23: 45–67; but also see A. Fuentes.
162 Not e s to Ch a p t er 4
2021. “Searching for the ‘Roots’ of Masculinity in Primates and the Human Evolution-
ary Past.” Current Anthropology 62: S13–S25. https://doi.org/10.1086/711582; and the
discussion in this and the subsequent chapter in regard to h umans in this context.
8. N. Smit, B. Ngoubangoye, M. J. E. Charpentier, and E. Huchard. 2022. “Dynam-
ics of Intersexual Dominance in a Highly Dimorphic Primate.” Frontiers in Ecology
and Evolution 10: 931226. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.931226.
9. P. M. Kappeler, E. Huchard, A. Baniel, C. Canteloup, M. J. E. Charpentier,
L. Cheng, E. Davidian, et al. 2022. “Sex and Dominance: How to Assess and Interpret
Intersexual Dominance Relationships in Mammalian Societies.” Frontiers in Ecology
and Evolution 10: 918773. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.918773; R. J. Lewis.
2018. “Female Power in Primates and the Phenomenon of Female Dominance.” An-
nual Review of Anthropology 47: 533–51. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro
-102317-045958; R. J. Lewis. 2020. “Female Power: A New Framework for Under-
standing ‘Female Dominance’ in Lemurs.” Folia Primatologica 91: 48–68. https://doi
.org/10.1159/000500443.
10. G. Parker. 2006. “Sexual Conflict over Mating and Fertilization: An Overview.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences 361: 235–59. https://
doi.org/10.1098/rstb.20 05.1785.
11. C. Campbell, A. Fuentes, K. MacKinnon, S. Bearder, and R. Stumpf. 2011.
Primates in Perspective. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press, chap. 19.
12. Campbell et al., Primates in Perspective, chaps. 14 and 15.
13. L. T. Gettler. 2016. “Becoming DADS: Considering the Role of Cultural
Context and Developmental Plasticity for Paternal Socioendocrinology.” Current
Anthropology 57: S38–S51; E. Fernandez-Duque, C. R. Valeggia, and S. P. Mendoza.
2009. “The Biology of Paternal Care in Human and Nonhuman Primates.” Annual
Review of Anthropology 38 (1): 115–30.
14. In fact, we sometimes see female dominance (such as in many lemurs).
15. T. Q. Bartlett and L. E. O. Light. “Sexual Dichromatism.” In The International
Encyclopedia of Primatology, edited by A. Fuentes. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/9781119179313.wbprim0427.
16. Campbell et al., Primates in Perspective, chap. 17.
17. A. Kralick. 2023. “When Ape Sex Isn’t Simple.” Anthropology News, March 14.
https://www.anthropology-news.org/articles/when-ape-sex-isnt-simple/#citation.
18. A. N. Maggioncalda, R. M. Sapolsky, and N. M. Czekala. 1999. “Reproductive
Hormone Profiles in Captive Male Orangutans: Implications for Understanding
Developmental Arrest.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109 (1): 19–32.
https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199905)109:1<19::AID-A JPA3>3.0
.CO;2-3. And E. M. Thompson, A. Zhou, and C. D. Knott. 2012. “Low Testosterone
Correlates with Delayed Development in Male Orangutans.” PloS One. 7 (10):
e47282. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0047282.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 4 163
53. G. Hohmann and B. Fruth. 2000. “Use and Function of Genital Contacts
among Female Bonobos.” Animal Behavior 60: 107–20. https://doi.org.10.1006/anbe
.2000.1451; L. R. Moscovice, M. Surbeck, B. Fruth, G. Hohmann, A. V. Jaeggi, and
T. Deschner. 2019. “The Cooperative Sex: Sexual Interactions among Female Bono-
bos Are Linked to Increases in Oxytocin, Proximity and Coalitions.” Hormones and
Behavior 116: 104581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104581.
54. Sandel and Reddy, “Sociosexual Behaviour in Wild Chimpanzees.”
55. Sandel and Reddy, “Sociosexual Behaviour in Wild Chimpanzees”; J. Yamag-
iwa. 2006. “Playful Encounters: The Development of Homosexual Behaviour in Male
Mountain Gorillas.” In Homosexual Behaviour in Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective,
edited by V. Sommer and P. L. Vasey, 273–93. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
56. E. A. Fox. 2001. “Homosexual Behavior in Wild Sumatran Orangutans (Pongo
pygmaeus abelii).” American Journal of Primatology 55: 177–81. https://doi.org/10
.1002/ajp.1051.
57. A. Fuentes. 2022. Race, Monogamy and Other Lies They Told You: Busting Myths
about Human Nature. 2nd ed. Oakland: University of California Press; K. Wellings,
M. Collumbien, E. Slaymaker, S. Singh, Z. Hodges, D. Patel, and N. Bajos.
2006. “Sexual Behaviour in Context: A Global Perspective.” Lancet 368 (9548):
1706–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69479-8.
58. Again, for a case study in Neanderthals, see Kubicka et al., “Sexual Behavior
in Neanderthals.”
59. O. Lovejoy. [1981] 2009. “Reexamining Human Origins in Light of Ardipithe-
cus ramidus.” Science 326: 108–15.
60. Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other Lies.
61. For recent versions of this approach, see B. Chapais. 2010. Primeval Kinship:
How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to H uman Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press; and Lovejoy, “Reexamining Human Origins”; see also Ross et al., “Reproduc-
tive Inequality in Humans and Other Mammals.”
62. B. Chapais. 2010. Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to H uman
Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
63. Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other Lies.
64. A. Fuentes. 2009. Evolution of Human Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University
Press; and A. Fuentes, M. Wyczalkowski, and K. C. MacKinnon. 2010. “Niche Con-
struction through Cooperation: A Nonlinear Dynamics Contribution to Modeling
Facets of the Evolutionary History in the Genus Homo.” Current Anthropology 51 (3):
435–44; Fuentes Creative Spark.
65. See A. Fuentes. 1999. “Re-evaluating Primate Monogamy.” American Anthro-
pologist 100 (4): 890–907; and A. Fuentes. 2002. “Patterns and Trends in Primate Pair
Bonds.” International Journal of Primatology 23 (4): 953–78.
66. Less than 3 percent of all mammalian species are monogamous.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 4 167
67. See multiple chapters in P. T. Ellison and P. B. Gray, eds. 2009. The Endocrinol-
ogy of Social Relationships, 270–93. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; and
J. T. Curtis and Z. Wang. 2003. “The Neurochemistry of Pair Bonding.” Current Direc-
tions in Psychological Science 12 (2): 49–53; and S. L. Díaz-Muñoz, and K. L. Bales.
2016. “Monogamy.” In “Primates: Variability, Trends, and Synthesis.” Introduction to
special issue on Primate Monogamy. American Journal of Primatology 78 (3): 283–87.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22463.
68. Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other Lies.
69. See multiple chapters in Ellison and Gray, Endocrinology of Social Relation-
ships, 270–93; and Curtis and Wang, “Neurochemistry of Pair Bonding.”
70. However, just being a relative does not automatically generate a pair bond.
71. Fuentes, Creative Spark; M. W. Conkey. 2010. “Images without Words: The
Construction of Prehistoric Imaginaries for Definitions of ‘Us.’ ” Journal of Visual
Culture 9: 272–83; M. Kissel and A. Fuentes. 2017. “Semiosis in the Pleistocene.”
Cambridge Archaeological Journal 27 (3): 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774
317000014.
72. S. Nanda. 2014. Gender Diversity: Cross-Cultural Variations. 2nd ed. Long
Grove, IL: Waveland Press; W. Wood and A. H. Eagly. 2002. “A Cross-Cultural
Analysis of the Behavior of Women and Men: Implications for the Origins of Sex
Differences.” Psychological Bulletin 128 (5): 699–727; J. Lorber. 2022. The New Gender
Paradox. Oxford: Polity Press.
73. S. Lacy and C. Ocobock. 2023. “Woman the Hunter: The Archaeological Evi-
dence.” American Anthropologist 126 (1): 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1111/aman.13914.
74. L. C. K. Aiello. 2002. “Energetic Consequences of Being a Homo erectus
Female.” American Journal of Human Biology 14: 551–65; L. C. Aiello and J. C. K. Wells.
2002. “Energetics and the Evolution of the Genus Homo.” Annual Review of Anthropol-
ogy 31: 323–38.
75. D. Haraway. 1989. Primate Visions: Gender, Race, and Nature in the World of
Modern Science. Oxfordshire: Routledge; Fuentes, Creative Spark; Conkey, “Images
without Words.”
76. Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Archaeological Evidence.”
77. Stone and Zimmer, “Issues of Gender, Identity, and Agency on Paleopathol-
ogy”; Fuentes, Creative Spark; Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Ar-
chaeological Evidence”; C. Ocobock and S. Lacy. 2023. “Woman the Hunter: The
Physiological Evidence.” American Anthropologist 126 (1): 7–18. https://doi.org/10
.1111/aman.13915.
78. See J. M. Adovasio, O. Soffer, and J. Page. 2007. The Invisible Sex: Uncovering
the True Roles of Women in Prehistory. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Books; Stone
and Zimmer, “Issues of Gender, Identity, and Agency on Paleopathology”; Conkey,
“Images without Words.”
168 Not e s to Ch a p t er 4
79. D. Stout. 2011. “Stone Toolmaking and the Evolution of Human Culture and
Cognition.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366 (1567): 1050–59;
D. Stout and N. Khreisheh. 2015. “Skill Learning and Human Brain Evolution: An
Experimental Approach.” Cambridge Archaeological Journal 25: 867–75. https://doi
.org/10.1017/S0959774315000359; Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The
Archaeological Evidence.”
80. K. W. Arthur. 2010. “Feminine Knowledge and Skill Reconsidered: Women
and Flaked Stone Tools.” American Anthropologist 112 (2): 228–43; J. Gero. 1991.
“Genderlithics: W omen’s Roles in Stone-Tool Production.” In Engendering
Archaeology: Women and Prehistory, edited by J. Gero and M. W. Conkey, 163–93.
Oxford: Blackwell; K. Weedman. 2005. “Gender and Stone Tools: An Ethnographic
Study of the Konso and Gamo Hideworkers of Southern Ethiopia.” In Gender and
Hide Production, edited by L. Frink and K. Weedman, 175–97. Walnut Creek, CA:
Altamira Press.
81. A. Murray, R. Pinhasi, and J. Stock. 2014. “Lower Limb Skeletal Biomechan-
ics Track Long-Term Decline in Mobility across ∼6150 Years of Agriculture in Cen-
tral Europe.” Journal of Archaeological Science 52: 376–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.jas.2014.0 9.0 01. A. A. Macintosh, R. Pinhasi, and J. T. Stock. 2017. “Prehistoric
Women’s Manual L abor Exceeded That of Athletes through the First 5500 Years of
Farming in Central E urope.” Science Advances 3 (11): eaao3893. https://doi.org/10
.1126/sciadv.aao3893; Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Archaeologi-
cal Evidence”; Ocobock and Lacy, “Woman the Hunter: The Physiological
Evidence.”
82. Ocobock and Lacy, “Woman the Hunter: The Physiological Evidence.”
83. E.g. Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Archaeological Evidence”;
Ocobock and Lacy, “Woman the Hunter: The Physiological Evidence”; R. Haas,
J. Watson, T. Buonasera, J. Southon, J. C. Chen, S. Noe, K. Smith, et al. 2020. “Female
Hunters of the Early Americas.” Science Advances 6 (45): eabd0310. https://doi.org
/10.1126/sciadv.abd0310; S. L. Kuhn and M. C. Stiner. 2006. “What’s a Mother to
Do? The Division of L abor among Neandertals and Modern H umans in Eurasia.”
Current Anthropology 47 (6): 953–80. https://doi.org/10.1086/507197; R. Wragg
Sykes. 2020. Kindred: Neanderthal Life, Love, Death and Art. London: Bloomsbury;
A. A. Sigma, S. Chilczuk, K. Nelson, R. Ruther, and C. Wall-Scheffler. 2023. “The
Myth of Man the Hunter: W omen’s Contribution to the Hunt across Ethnographic
Contexts.” PloS One 18 (6): e0287101. https://doi.o rg/10.1371/journal.pone
.0287101.
84. Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Archaeological Evidence”;
Ocobock and Lacy, “Woman the Hunter: The Physiological Evidence.”
85. A. Estalrrich and A. Rosas. 2015. “Division of Labor by Sex and Age in Nean-
dertals: An Approach through the Study of Activity-Related Dental Wear.” Journal
Not e s to Ch a p t er 4 169
of Human Evolution 80: 51–63; see also Kubicka et al., “Sexual Behavior in
Neanderthals.”
86. Spikins et al., “Living to Fight Another Day”; Spikins et al., “Theoretical and
Methodological Approaches.”
87. D. Snow. 2013. “Sexual Dimorphism in E uropean Upper Paleolithic Cave Art.”
American Antiquity 78: 746–61. https://doi.org/10.7183/0 002-7316.78.4.746;
Conkey, “Images without Words”; Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The
Archaeological Evidence.”
88. P. Sorokowski and M. Kowal. 2024. “Relationship between the 2D:4D and Pre-
natal Testosterone, Adult Level Testosterone, and Testosterone Change: Meta-analysis
of 54 Studies.” American Journal of Biological Anthropology 183 (1): 20–38. https://doi.org
/10.1002/ajpa.24852; M. Marczak, M. Misiak, A. Sorokowska, and P. Sorokowski. 2018.
“No Sex Difference in Digit Ratios (2D:4D) in the Traditional Yali of Papua and Its
Meaning for the Previous Hypotheses on the Inter-populational Variability in 2D:4D.”
American Journal of H uman Biology 30 (2): https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23078.
89. B. McCauley, D. Maxwell, and M. Collard. 2018. “A Cross-cultural Perspective
on Upper Palaeolithic Hand Images with Missing Phalanges.” Journal of Paleolithic
Archeology 1: 314–33. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41982-018-0 016-8.
90. Fuentes, Creative Spark; A. Fuentes. 2019. Why We Believe: Evolution and the
Human Way of Being. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press/Templeton Press;
D. Graeber and D. Wengrow. 2021. The Dawn of Everything: A New History of Humanity.
New York: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.
91. Fuentes, Creative Spark; Lacy and Ocobock, “Woman the Hunter: The Ar-
chaeological Evidence”; Ocobock and Lacy, “Woman the Hunter: The Physiological
Evidence.”
92. For example, M. Cintas-Peña and L. García Sanjuán. 2019. “Gender Inequali-
ties in Neolithic Iberia: A Multi-Proxy Approach.” European Journal of Archaeology
22 (4): 499–522. https://doi.org/10.1017/eaa.2019.3; R. Rasteiro and L. Chikhi.
2013. “Female and Male Perspectives on the Neolithic Transition in E urope: Clues
from Ancient and Modern Genetic Data.” PloS One 8 (4): e60944. https://doi.org
/10.1371/journal.pone.0060944.
93. C. Hedenstierna-Jonson, A. Kjellström, T. Zachrisson, M. Krzenwińska,
V. Sobrado, N. Price, T. Günther, M. Jakobsson, et al. 2017. “A Female Viking Warrior
Confirmed by Genomics.” American Journal of Physical Anthropology 164: 853–60.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23308; K. Croucher. 2012. Death and Dying in the
Neolithic Near East. Oxford: Oxford University Press; M. Cintas-Peña, M. Luciañez-
Triviño, R. Montero Artús, A. Bileck, P. Bortel, F. Kanz, K. Rebay-Salisbury, et al.
2023. “Amelogenin Peptide Analyses Reveal Female Leadership in Copper Age Iberia
(c. 2900–2650 BC).” Scientific Reports 13 (1): 9594. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598
-023-36368-x.
170 Not e s to Ch a p t er 5
5. Humans Now
1. Note I am specifically stating male and female (3G), not man and woman . . .
if one used only gender categories, t here would be even more mixing and less con-
sistency between one of the categories and height.
2. A. Fausto-Sterling. 2020. Sexing the Body: Gender Politics and the Construction
of Sexuality. London: Hachette.
3. See for example C. Sanchis-Segura and R. R. Wilcox. 2024. “From Means to
Meaning in the Study of Sex/Gender Differences and Similarities.” Front Neuroen-
docrinol 73: 101133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2024.101133.
4. C. Ruff. 2002. “Variation in Human Body Size and Shape.” Annual Reviews in
Anthropology 31: 211–32; A. Fuentes. 2022. Race, Monogamy and Other Lies They Told
You: Busting Myths about H uman Nature. 2nd ed. Oakland: University of California
Press.
5. Fausto-Sterling, Sexing the Body; L. Z. DuBois and H. Shattuck-Heidorn. 2021.
“Challenging the Binary: Gender/Sex and the Bio-logics of Normalcy.” American
Journal of Human Biology 33 (5): e23623; Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other Lies;
W. D. Lassek and S. J. C. Gaulin. 2022. “Substantial but Misunderstood Human
Sexual Dimorphism Results Mainly from Sexual Selection on Males and Natural
Selection on Females.” Frontiers in Psychology 17 (13): 859931. https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2022.859931.
6. See the National Center for Health Statistics. https://www.cdc.gov/nchs
/nhanes/index.htm.
7. C. D. Fryar, M. D. Carroll, Q. Gu, J. Afful, and C. L. Ogden. 2021. “Anthropo-
metric Reference Data for Children and Adults: United States, 2015–2018.” Vital
Health Statistics (36): 1–44.
8. H. M. Dunsworth. 2020. “Expanding the Evolutionary Explanations for Sex
Differences in the Human Skeleton.” Evolutionary Anthropology 29 (3): 108–16.
https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21834.
9. T. M. Flaherty, L. J. Johnson, K. C. Woollen, D. Lopez, K. Gaddis, S. L. Horsley,
and J. F. Byrnes. 2023. “Speaking of Sex: Critical Reflections for Forensic Anthropolo-
gists.” Humans 3: 251–70. https://doi.org/10.3390/humans3040020; E. Garofalo
and H. Garvin. 2020. “The Confusion between Biological Sex and Gender and Po-
tential Implications of Misinterpretations.” https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12
-815767-1.0 0004-3; R. M. Meloro, S. D. Tallman, C. G. Streed Jr., J. Stowell, T. A.
Delgado, J. D. Haug, A. Redgrave, et al. Forthcoming. “A Framework for Incorporating
Diverse Gender Identities into Forensic Anthropology Casework and Theory:
Recommendations for Inclusive Practices.” Current Anthropology.
10. J. L. Nuzzo. 2022. “Narrative Review of Sex Differences in Muscle Strength,
Endurance, Activation, Size, Fiber Type, and Strength Training Participation Rates,
Not e s to Ch a p t er 5 171
22. L. Baskin, J. Shen, A. Sinclair, M. Cao, X. Liu, G. Liu, D. Isaacson, et al. 2018.
“Development of the Human Penis and Clitoris.” Differentiation 103: 74–85. https://
doi .o rg / 1 0 .1016 / j .d iff . 2 018 .0 8 .001; Prum, Performance All the Way Down;
L. Boulanger, M. Pannetier, L. Gall, A. Allais-Bonnet, M. Elzaiat, D. Le Bourhis,
N. Daniel, et al. 2014. “E. FOXL2 Is a Female Sex-Determining Gene in the Goat.”
Current Biology 24 (4): 404–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.12.039; F. Zhao,
H. L. Franco, K. F. Rodriguez, P. R. Brown, M. Tsai, S. Y. Tsai, and H. H-C. Yao. 2017.
“Elimination of the Male Reproductive Tract in the Female Embryo Is Promoted by
COUP-TFII in Mice.” Science 357: 717–20. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aai9136.
23. Fausto-Sterling, Sexing the Body.
24. Specifically, the lack of anti-Müllerian hormone (AMH) and the correct se-
quence of activation of EMX2, HOXA13, PAX2, LIM1, and WNT genes. D. Wilson and
B. Bordoni. 2023. “Embryology, Mullerian Ducts (Paramesonephric Ducts).” Stat-
Pearls [Internet], March 6. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK557727/.
25. T. Kurita. 2010. “Developmental Origin of Vaginal Epithelium.” Differentiation
80 (2–3): 99–105. https://doi.org/1 0.1016/j . diff.2 010.06.007; C. R. Cunha, S. J. R obboy,
T. Kurita, D. Isaacson, J. Shen, M. Cao, and L. S. Baskin. 2018. “Development of the
Human Female Reproductive Tract.” Differentiation 103: 46–65. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.diff.2018.0 9.001.
26. M. Habiba, R. Heyn, P. Bianchi, I. Brosens, and G. Benagiano. 2021. “The
Development of the Human Uterus: Morphogenesis to Menarche.” Human Repro-
duction Update 27 (1): 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1093/humupd/dmaa036.
27. Again, see Fausto-Sterling, Sexing the Body, for an excellent overview of these
patterns and the medical, and cultural, responses to them. See also many sites such
as the Cleveland Clinic for public-facing information on this: “Atypical Genitalia
(Formerly Known as Ambiguous Genitalia).” March 9, 2022. https://my
.clevelandclinic.org/health/diseases/22470-atypical-genitalia-formerly-known-as
-ambiguous-genitalia.
28. L. Scheja and J. Heeren. 2019. “The Endocrine Function of Adipose Tissues
in Health and Cardiometabolic Disease.” Nature Reviews Endocrinology 15 (9): 507–
24. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41574-019-0230-6; C. SturtzSreetharan, A. Brewis,
J. Hardin, S. Trainer, and A. Wutich. 2021. Fat in Four Cultures: A Global Ethnography
of Weight. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
29. T. Tchkonia, T.Thomou, Y. Zhu, I. Karagiannides, C. Pothoulakis, M. D. Jen-
sen MD, and J. L. Kirkland. 2013. “Mechanisms and Metabolic Implications of Re-
gional Differences among Fat Depots.” Cell Metabolism 17 (5): 644–56. https://doi
.org/ 1 0. 1016/ j . cmet. 2 013. 03. 008; N. Boulet, A. Briot, J. Galitzky, and A. Bouloumié.
2022. “The Sexual Dimorphism of H uman Adipose Depots.” Biomedicines 10 (10):
2615. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomedicines10102615.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 5 173
30. R.W. Taylor, A. M. Grant, S. M. Williams, and A. Goulding. 2010. “Sex Differ-
ences in Regional Body Fat Distribution from Pre-to Postpuberty.” Obesity 18:
1410–16.
31. C. M. Pond. 1997. “The Biological Origins of Adipose Tissue in H umans.” In
The Evolving Female, edited by M. E. Morbeck, A. Galloway, and A. L. Zihlman,
147–62. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; Boulet et al., “Sexual Dimor-
phism of Human Adipose Depots.”
32. A. Innocenti, D. Melita, and E. Dreassi. 2022. “Incidence of Complications for
Different Approaches in Gynecomastia Correction: A Systematic Review of the Lit
erature.” Aesthetic Plastic Surgery 46: 1025–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00266-022
-02782-1; D. Joel. 2012. “Genetic-Gonadal-Genitals Sex (3G-Sex) and the Miscon-
ception of Brain and Gender, or, Why 3G-Males and 3G-Females Have Intersex Brain
and Intersex Gender.” Biology of Sex Differences 3 (1): 27.
33. Boulet et al., “Sexual Dimorphism of Human Adipose Depots.”
34. The variation in hair patterns across our species has been mispresented and
misused by racists and specifically “race scientists” to make assertions about the lack
or preponderance of hair on bodies and their relative “value.” Th ese same biases have
been used to discriminate against bodies with “more” hair than culturally “appropri-
ate.” E. Carlin and B. Kramer. 2020. “Hair, Hormones, and Haunting: Race as a Ghost
Variable in Polycystic Ovary Syndrome.” Science, Technology, & H uman Values 45 (5):
779–803. https://doi.org/10.1177/0162243920908647.
35. See B. O. Yildiz, S. Bolour, K. Woods, A. Moore, and R. Azziz. 2010. “Visually
Scoring Hirsutism.” Human Reproduction Update 16 (1): 51–64. https://doi.org/10
.1093/humupd/dmp024. For some methods and data, but be aware of the racial-
izing frame t hose authors see. See also E. Carlin and Kramer, “Hair, Hormones, and
Haunting.”
36. See for example T. Lasisi, J. W. Smallcombe, W. L. Kenney, M. D. Shriver,
B. Zydney, N. G. Jablonski, and G. Havenith. 2023. “Human Scalp Hair as a Thermo-
regulatory Adaptation.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America 120 (24): e2301760120. https://doi.org/10.1073/ pnas. 2301760120;
and also U. Ohnemus, M. Uenalan, J. Inzunza, J. A. Gustafsson, and R. Paus.
2006. “The Hair Follicle as an Estrogen Target and Source.” Endocrine Reviews 27 (6):
677–706. https://doi.org/10.1210/er.2006-0020.
37. A. Melk, B. Babitsch, B. Borchert-Mörlins, F. Claas, A. I. Dipchand, S. Eifert,
B. Eiz-Vesper, et al. 2019. “Equally Interchangeable? How Sex and Gender Affect
Transplantation.” Transplantation 103 (6): 1094–110. https://doi.org/10.1097/TP
.0000000000002655; F. Puoti, A. Ricci, A. Nanni-Costa, W. Ricciardi, W. Malorni,
and E. Ortona. 2016. “Organ Transplantation and Gender Differences: A Paradigmatic
Example of Intertwining between Biological and Sociocultural Determinants.”
174 Not e s to Ch a p t er 5
Axis Activation in Fetal and Neonatal Life.” Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 9: 410.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fendo.2018.00410.
48. R. L. Rosenfield. 2021. “Normal and Premature Adrenarche.” Endocrine Re-
views 42 (6): 783–814. https://doi.org/10.1210/endrev/bnab009.
49. S. Feldman Witchel and A. Kemal Topaloglu. 2019. “Puberty: Gonadarche
and Adrenarche.” In Yen and Jaffe’s Reproductive Endocrinology, 8th ed., edited by
J. F. Strauss and R. L. Barbieri, 394–445. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
50. K. Peacock and K. M. Ketvertis 2022. Menopause. StatPearls Publishing.
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK507826/; K. Clancy. 2023. Period: The
Real Story of Menstruation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
51. A. M. Matsumoto. 2002. “Andropause: Clinical Implications of the Decline in
Serum Testosterone Levels with Aging in Men.” Journal of Gerontology: Biological
Sciences 57 (2): M76–99. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/57.2.m76.
52. M. Liu, S. Murthi, and L. Poretsky. 2020. “Polycystic Ovary Syndrome and
Gender Identity.” Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine 93 (4): 529–37.
53. C. Hooven. 2021. T: The Story of Testosterone, the Hormone That Dominates and
Divides Us. New York: Henry Holt and Company.
54. Van Anders, Steiger, and Goldey, “Effects of Gendered Behavior”; K. O.
Smiley, K. M. Munley, K. Aghi, S. E. Lipshutz, T. M. Patton, D. S. Pradhan, T. K.
Solomon-Lane, et al. 2024. “Sex Diversity in the 21st Century: Concepts, Frame-
works, and Approaches for the F uture of Neuroendocrinology.” Hormones and
Behavior 157: 105445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.y hbeh.2023.105445; K. Casto,
D. Cohen, M. Akinola, and P. Mehta. 2024. “Testosterone, Gender Identity and
Gender-Stereotyped Personality Attributes.” Hormones and Behavior 162: 105540.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2024.105540.
55. Jordan-Young and Karkazis, Testosterone; C. Fine. 2016. Testosterone Rex: Myths
of Sex, Science and Society. New York: W. W. Norton; van Anders, Steiger, and Goldey,
“Effects of Gendered Behavior”; Smiley et al., “Sex Diversity in the 21st Century”;
Casto et al., “Testosterone, Gender Identity and Gender-Stereotyped Personality
Attributes.”
56. M. Schulster, A. M. Bernie, and R. Ramasamy. 2016. “The Role of Estradiol in
Male Reproductive Function.” 2016. Asian Journal of Andrology 18 (3): 435–40.
https://d oi. org/ 1 0. 4103/1 008-682X.173932; S. M. van Anders. 2013. “Beyond Mas-
culinity: Testosterone, Gender/Sex, and Human Social Behavior in a Comparative
Context.” Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 34: 198–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j
.yfrne.2013.07.001; Jordan-Young and Karkazis, Testosterone.
57. J. R. Roney. 2016. “Theoretical Frameworks for Human Behavioral Endocri-
nology.” Hormones and Behavior 84: 97–110; Bribiescas, “Reproductive Ecology”;
L. T. Gettler. 2014. “Applying Socioendocrinology to Evolutionary Models:
176 Not e s to Ch a p t er 5
79. Rauch and Eliot, “Breaking the Binary”; Eliot, “Remembering the Null Hypoth-
esis”; Rippon, Gendered Brain; L. Eliot, A. K. Beery, E. G. Jacobs, H. F. LeBlanc, D. L.
Maney, and M. M. McCarthy. 2023. “Why and How to Account for Sex and Gender in
Brain and Behavioral Research.” Journal of Neuroscience 143 (37): 6344–56. https://doi
.org/1 0.1523/J NEUROSCI.0 020-23.2023; A. Kaiser. 2012. “Re-Conceptualizing ‘Sex’
and ‘Gender’ in the H uman Brain.” Zeitschrift für Psychologie 220: 130–36; D. L. Maney.
2015. “Just Like a Circus: The Public Consumption of Sex Differences.” Current Topics
in Behavioral Neuroscience 19: 279–96. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2014_339.
80. DeCasien et al., “Sex Differences in the Human Brain.”
81. Eliot et al., “Dump the ‘Dimorphism’ ”; Eliot, “Remembering the Null
Hypothesis.”
82. S. Richardson. 2013. Sex Itself: The Search for Male and Female in the H uman
Genome. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; Kung et al., “Prenatal Androgen
Exposure and Sex-Typical Play Behaviour.”
83. Many of the genes on the X chromosome are not related directly or only to
reproductive biology, but most of the genes on the Y are.
84. Prum, Performance All the Way Down.
85. B. Croft, T. Ohnesorg, J. Hewitt, J. Bowles, A. Quinn, J. Tan, V. Corbin, et al.
2018. “Human Sex Reversal Is Caused by Duplication or Deletion of Core Enhancers
Upstream of SOX9.” Nature Communications 9 (1): 5319. https://doi.org/10.1038
/s41467-018-07784-9; P. Bernard and V. R. Harley. 2007. “Wnt4 Action in Gonadal
Development and Sex Determination.” The International Journal of Biochemistry & Cell
Biology 39 (1): 31–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. biocel.2006.06.007; K. K. Niakan and
E. R. B. McCabe. 2005. “DAX1 Origin, Function, and Novel Role.” Molecular Genetics
and Metabolism 86 (1): 70–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymgme.2005.07.019.
86. Fausto-Sterling, Sexing the Body; Prum, Performance All the Way Down.
87. O. M. Muñoz-A guirre, S. Kim-Hellmuth, V. Wucher, A. D. H. Gewirtz,
D. J. Cotter, P. Parsana, S. Kasela, et al. 2020. “The Impact of Sex on Gene Expression
across Human Tissues.” Science 369 (6509): eaba3066. https://doi.org/10.1126
/science.aba3066.
88. M. Gershoni and S. Pietrokovski. 2017. “The Landscape of Sex-Differential
Transcriptome and Its Consequent Selection in Human Adults.” BMC Biol 15 (7).
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-017-0352-z.
89. Richardson, Sex Itself.
90. A. Wiley. 2021. “Pearl Lecture: Biological Normalcy: A New Framework for
Biocultural Analysis of H uman Population Variation.” American Journal of Human
Biology 33 (5): e23563. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23563.
91. See Fausto-Sterling, Sexing the Body, for an excellent and extensive overview
of these patterns and their explanations and impacts. See also C. Ainsworth. 2015.
“Sex Redefined: The Idea of 2 Sexes Is Overly Simplistic.” Scientific American,
Not e s to Ch a p t er 6 179
February 8. https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/sex-redefined-the-idea-of
-2-sexes-is-overly-simplistic1; and Richardson, Sex Itself.
92. Hyde et al., “The Future of Sex and Gender in Psychology”; and Fausto-
Sterling, Sexing the Body; S. M. van Anders. 2022. “Gender/Sex/ual Diversity and
Biobehavioral Research.” Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, No-
vember 10. Advance online publication. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000609.
6. No Biological B
attle of the Sexes
1. This book has sold more than fifteen million copies, been reprinted many times,
and remains a near best seller in the 2020s. John Gray’s website offers (for a fee) to
help you know who your soul mate is, increase intimacy and passion, create romance
with your partner, and enjoy a lifetime of great sex (Marsvenus.com).
2. C. Darwin. 1871. The Descent of Man, and Selection in Relation to Sex. London:
John Murray, 401.
3. A. J. Bateman. 1948. “Intra-sexual Selection in Drosophila.” Heredity 2: 349–68;
E. O. Wilson. 1979. “Sex and Human Nature.” The Wilson Quarterly 3 (4): 92–105.
4. H. Dunsworth. 2021. “This View of Wife.” In A Most Interesting Problem, edited
by J. DaSilva, chap. 9. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; L. Cooke. 2021.
Bitch: On the Female of the Species. New York: Basic Books; M. Landau. 1991. N arrative
of Human Evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; A. Fausto-Sterling. 1992.
Myths of Gender. New York: Basic Books; M. Midgley. 2003. The Myths We Live By.
Oxfordshire: Routledge; A. Saini. 2017. Inferior: How Science Got Women Wrong—
and the New Research That’s Rewriting the Story. Boston: Beacon Press.
5. W. D. Lassek and S. J. C. Gaulin. 2022. “Substantial but Misunderstood Human
Sexual Dimorphism Results Mainly from Sexual Selection on Males and Natural
Selection on Females.” Frontiers in Psychology 13: 859931. https://doi.org/10.3389
/fpsyg.2022.859931. These authors state specifically that (1) sexual dimorphism in
stature, fat mass, and fat distribution have been significantly s haped by disruptive
natural-selection regimes operating on females and males, with some likely overlay
of subsequent sexual selection acting via mate choice in the case of fat distribution;
(2) sexual dimorphism in lean mass, muscle mass, and strength are largely due to
sexual selection arising from a long history of aggressive male mating competition,
with the some possible influence of divergent natural selection due to sex differences
in foraging ecology; and (3) a large literature seems to have overemphasized the role
of mate choice, and underestimated the role of male competition for mates, in shap-
ing human sex differences. For a version of this same argument that focuses on the
female need to stay healthy to raise young, see h ere: J. F. Benenson, C. E. Webb, and
R. W. Wrangham. 2021. “Self-Protection as an Adaptive Female Strategy.” Behavioral
and Brain Sciences 45: e128. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X21002417.
180 Not e s to Ch a p t er 6
6. See C. Hooven. 2021. T: The Story of Testosterone, the Hormone That Dominates
and Divides Us. New York: Henry Holt and Company, 25.
7. See, for example, R. Jordan-Young and K. Karkazis. 2019. Testosterone: An
Unauthorized Biography. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; C. Fine. 2016.
Testosterone Rex: Myths of Sex, Science and Society. New York: W. W. Norton; S. M. van
Anders. 2013. “Beyond Masculinity: Testosterone, Gender/Sex, and Human Social
Behavior in a Comparative Context.” Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 34: 198–210.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.y frne.2 013.07.001; S. M. van Anders, J. Steiger, and
K. L. Goldey. 2015. “Effects of Gendered Behavior on Testosterone in W omen and
Men.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112:
13805–10; A. Fausto-Sterling. 2020. Sexing the Body: Gender Politics and the Construction
of Sexuality. London: Hachette; S. Richardson. 2013. Sex Itself: The Search for Male and
Female in the H uman Genome. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; L. T. Gettler.
2016. “Becoming DADS: Considering the Role of Cultural Context and Developmen-
tal Plasticity for Paternal Socioendocrinology.” Current Anthropology 57: S38–S51.
8. Meta-analyses are systematic syntheses of many studies.
9. As usual, these studies are mostly on people in North America and Europe.
Thus, they are probably underrepresenting the broader variation in humans across
the planet.
10. While the authors use the terms “male” and “female,” they are actually assess-
ing gender (men/women, boys/girls) in these studies. J. Shibley Hyde. 2005. “The
Gender Similarities Hypothesis.” American Psychologist 60 (6): 581–92; E. Zell,
Z. Krizan, and S. R. Teeter. 2015. “Evaluating Gender Similarities and Differences Using
Metasynthesis.” American Psychologist 70: 10–20. https://d oi. org/1 0.1037/a0038208.
11. Zell, Krizan, and Teeter, “Evaluating Gender Similarities and Differences.”
12. R. W. Wrangham and M. L. Wilson. 2004. “Collective Violence—Comparisons
between Youths and Chimpanzees.” In Youth Violence: Scientific Approaches to P revention,
edited by J. Devine, J. Gilligan, K. A. Miczek, R. Shaikh, and D. Pfaff, 233–56. New
York: New York Academy of Sciences; Hooven, T: The Story of Testosterone, 25; Be-
nenson, Webb, and Wrangham, “Self-Protection as an Adaptive Female Strategy”;
Lassek and Gaulin, “Substantial but Misunderstood H uman Sexual Dimorphism”;
D. Buss and D. P. Schmitt. 2019. “Mate Preferences and Their Behavioral Manifesta-
tions.” Annual Review of Psychology 70: 77–110. See also previous mention of Darwin,
E. O. Wilson, Robert Trivers, e tc. (chapter 3). See also R. Nelson. 2021. “The Sex in
Your Violence: Patriarchy and Power in Anthropological World Building and Every-
day Life.” Current Anthropology 62: S92–S102. https://doi.org/10.1086/711605.
13. M. G. Alexander and T. D. Fisher. 2002. “Truth and Consequences: Using the
Bogus Pipeline to Examine Sex Differences in Self-Reported Sexuality.” Journal of
Sex Research 40 (1): 27–35; H. Donnan and F. Magowan. 2010. The Anthropology
of Sex. Oxfordshire: Routledge.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 6 181
35. G. Rippon. 2019. Gendered Brain: The New Neuroscience That Shatters the Myth
of the Female Brain. London: The Bodley Head Ltd.; D. Joel, Z. Berman, I. Tavor,
N. Wexler, O. Gaber, Y. Stein, N. Shefi, et al. 2015. “Sex Beyond the Genitalia: The
Human Brain Mosaic.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America 112 (50): 15468–73; D. Joel. 2012. “Genetic-Gonadal-Genitals Sex
(3G-Sex) and the Misconception of Brain and Gender, or, Why 3G-Males and
3G-Females Have Intersex Brain and Intersex Gender.” Biology of Sex Differences 3
(1): 27; S. Liu, J. Seidlitz, J. D. Blumenthal, L. S. Clasen, and A. Raznahan. 2020.
“Integrative Structural, Functional, and Transcriptomic Analyses of Sex-Biased Brain
Organization in Humans.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of Americ a 117 (31): 18788–98. https://d oi .o rg /10 .1073 /p nas
.1919091117; M. Hirnstein, K. Hugdahl, and M. Hausmann. 2019. “Cognitive Sex
Differences and Hemispheric Asymmetry: A Critical Review of 40 Years of Re-
search.” Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition 24 (2): 204–52. https://
doi.org/10.1080/1357650x.2018.1497044.
36. Joel et al., “Sex Beyond the Genitalia.”
37. L. Eliot, A. Ahmed, H. Khan, and J. Patel. 2021. “Dump the ‘Dimorphism’:
Comprehensive Synthesis of Human Brain Studies Reveals Few Male-Female
Differences beyond Size.” Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 125: 667–97.
https://d oi. org/ 1 0. 1016/ j . neubiorev. 2021. 02. 026; L. Eliot. 2024. “Remembering the
Null Hypothesis When Searching for Brain Sex Differences.” Biology of Sex Differ-
ences 15:14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-024-0 0585-4.
38. L. S. B. Hrdy. 2009. Mother and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual
Understanding. New York: Belknap; C. Kuzawa, H. T. Chugani, L. I. Grossman,
L. Lipov ich, O. Muzik, P. Hof, R. Patrick, D. E. Wildman, et al. 2014. “Metabolic
Costs and Evolutionary Implications of Human Brain Development.” Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of Americ a 111 (36): 13010–15;
K. R. Rosenberg. 2021. “The Evolution of Human Infancy: Why It Helps to Be Help-
less.” Annual Review of Anthropology 50: 423–40. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev
-anthro-111819-105454.
39. K. L. Bales, C. S. Ardekani, A. Baxter, C. L. Karaskiewicz, J. X. Kuske, A. R. Lau,
L. E. Savidge, et al. 2021. “What Is a Pair Bond?” Hormones and Behavior 36: 105062.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105062; A. Fuentes. 1999. “Re-evaluating
Primate Monogamy.” American Anthropologist 100 (4): 890–907; A. Fuentes. 2002.
“Patterns and Trends in Primate Pair Bonds.” International Journal of Primatology 23
(4): 953–78.
40. See A. Fuentes. 2017. The Creative Spark: How Imagination Made Humans
Exceptional. New York: Dutton; but for a counterargument, see B. Chapais. 2010.
Primeval Kinship: How Pair-Bonding Gave Birth to Human Society. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
184 Not e s to Ch a p t er 7
41. J. S. Hyde, R. S. Bigler, D. Joel, C. C. Tate, and S. M. van Anders. 2019. “The
uture of Sex and Gender in Psychology: Five Challenges to the Gender Binary.”
F
American Psychologist 74 (2): 15. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000307.
7. Once the initial sensory stimulus engages the limbic system, neural stimuli
(brain actions) are transmitted to much of the body via the endocrine system (using
the same hormones, testosterone, vasopressin, and oxytocin, in all humans). This
leads to physical and psychological excitement that includes increased blood flow
and swelling of tissues (called vasocongestion) and a tensing of the muscles (called
myotonia) throughout the body, sporadic increases in blood pressure, lubrication in
the vagina and inner labia, erection of the clitoris and erection of the penis, glandular
secretions across parts of the body, and ultimately (or potentially) a variable suite of
physiological changes associated with orgasm. Fuentes, Race, Monogamy and Other
Lies; see also R. S. Calabrò, A. Cacciola, D. Bruschetta, et al. 2010. “Neuroanatomy
and Function of H uman Sexual Behavior: A Neglected or Unknown Issue?” Brain
and Behavior 9: e01389. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.1389.
8. See S. T. Lindau, L. P. Schumm, E. O. Laumann, W. Levinson, C. A.
O’Muircheartaigh, and L. J. Waite. 2007. “A Study of Sexuality and Health among
Older Adults in the United States.” New E ngland Journal of Medicine 357 (8): 762–74;
and Brewis and Meyer, “Marital Coitus across the Life Course.”
9. C. M. Curley and B. T. Johnson 2022. “Sexuality and Aging: Is It Time for a
New Sexual Revolution?” Social Science and Medicine 301: 114865. https://doi.org/10
.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114865; see also “Sexuality and Intimacy in Older Adults.”
NIH National Institute on Aging, accessed September 8, 2024. https://www.nia.nih
.gov/health/sexuality/sexuality-and-intimacy-older-adults.
10. Associated Press. 2020. “Tennessee Governor Signs Anti-Gay Adoption Bill.”
NBC News, January 24. https://www.nbcnews.com/feature/nbc-out/tennessee
-governor-signs-anti-gay-adoption-bill-n1122436.
11. “Child Welfare Nondiscrimination Laws.” Movement Advancement Project,
accessed September 8, 2024. https://www.lgbtmap.org/equality-maps/foster_and
_adoption_laws.
12. “Same-Sex Parents Are 7 Times More Likely to Raise A dopted and Foster
Children.” UCLA Williams Institute, accessed September 8, 2024. https://
williamsinstitute.law.ucla.edu/press/lgbt-parenting-media-alert/.
13. R. Sear. 2021. “The Male Breadwinner Nuclear Family Is Not the ‘Traditional’
Human F amily, and Promotion of This Myth May Have Adverse Health
Consequences.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society: Biological Sciences 376:
20200020. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2020.0020; E. Hubbard, O. Shannon, and
A. Pisor. 2023. “Non-kin Alloparents and Child Outcomes: Older Siblings, but Not
Godparents, Predict Educational Attainment in a Rural Context.” Evolution and
Human Behavior 44: 597–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2023.0 9
.006; K. L. Kramer. 2021. “The H uman F amily—Its Evolutionary Context and
Diversity.” Social Sciences 10 (6): 191. https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci10060191.
186 Not e s to Ch a p t er 7
14. There are also many other, very serious, problems with animal testing. See
H. Ferdowsian, A. Fuentes, L. Johnson, B. King, and J. Pierce. 2022. “Toward an
Anti-Maleficent Research Agenda.” Cambridge Quarterly of Healthcare Ethics 31 (1):
54–58. https://d oi. org/1 0.1017/S0963180121000487; and B. Keim. 2023. “What Do
We Owe Lab Animals?” New York Times, January 23. https://www.nytimes.com
/2023/01/23/science/what-do-we-owe-lab-animals.html.
15. This is not true of all medical researchers, especially in the past decade or two.
Today, t here are many outspoken advocates of including female nonhuman animals
(and h umans) in research. Most major government funding agencies of Canada, the
United States, and the European U nion now require such inclusion.
16. For an excellent overview, see E. Cleghorn. 2021. Unwell Women: Misdiagnosis
and Myth in a Man-Made World. New York: Dutton.
17. A. Ghorayshi. 2023. “Guess Which Sex Behaves More Erratically (at Least in
Mice).” New York Times, March 7. https://www.nytimes.com/2023/03/07/science
/female-mice-hormones.html.
18. Which was primarily the United States and E urope until the last fifty years or so.
19. There is a range of solid scholarship on this blend of racism and sexism and
how it s haped contemporary medical practice in the United States; see Cleghorn,
Unwell Women; C. Willoughby. 2022. Masters of Health: Racial Science and Slavery in
US Medical Schools. University of North Carolina Press. See also A. K. Beery and
I. Zucker. 2011. “Sex Bias in Neuroscience and Biomedical Research.” Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (3): 565–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010
.07.002; Ghorayshi, “Guess Which Sex Behaves More Erratically”; D. R. Levy,
N. Hunter, S. Lin, E. M. Robinson, W. Gillis, E. B. Conlin, R. Anyoha, et al. 2023.
“Mouse Spontaneous Behavior Reflects Individual Variation Rather Than Estrous
State.” Current Biology 33: 1–7.
20. L. Greaves and S. A. Ritz. 2022. “Sex, Gender and Health: Mapping the Land-
scape of Research and Policy.” International Journal of Environmental Research and
Public Health 19: 2563. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19052563.
21. This is primarily referring to 3G females, but we know that the term “women”
is not a biological term and not all women are 3G females. This, however, still sup-
ports the statement as the range of biological variation in the term “women” (and in
“men”) is much larger than is envisioned, or engaged, by much medical research.
22. I. Zucker and B. J. Prendergast. 2020. “Sex Differences in Pharmacokinetics
Predict Adverse Drug Reactions in Women.” Biology of Sex Differences 11: 32. https://
doi.org/10.1186/s13293-020-0 0308-5.
23. If this is the case, there are not really any significant differences between male
and female ADEs. T. Rushovich, A. Gompers, J. W. Lockhart, et al. 2023. “Adverse
Drug Events by Sex after Adjusting for Baseline Rates of Drug Use.” JAMA Netw
Open. 6 (8): e2329074. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2023.29074.
Not e s to Ch a p t er 7 187
24. D. J. Greenblatt, J. S. Harmatz, and T. Roth. 2019. “Zolpidem and Gender: Are
omen R
W eally at Risk?” Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 39 (3): 189–99.
https://doi.org/10.1097/JCP.0 000000000001026. PMID: 30939589.
25. H. Zhao, M. DiMarco, K. Ichikawa, M. Boulicault, M. Perret, K. Jillson,
A. Fair, et al. 2023. “Making a ‘Sex-Difference Fact’: Ambien Dosing at the Interface
of Policy, Regulation, W omen’s Health, and Biology.” Social Studies of Science 53 (4):
475–94. https://doi.org/10.1177/03063127231168371.
26. M. Roser, C. Appel, and H. Ritchie. 2013. “Human Height.” OurWorldInData
.org. Last updated January 2024. https://ourworldindata.org/human-height; T. G.
Travison, H. W. Vesper, E. Orwoll, F. Wu, J. M. Kaufman, Y. Wang, B. Lapauw, et al.
2017. “Harmonized Reference Ranges for Circulating Testosterone Levels in Men of
Four Cohort Studies in the United States and E urope.” The Journal of Clinical Endo-
crinology & Metabolism 102 (4): 1161–73. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2016-2935;
I. Janssen, S. B. Heymsfield, Z. Wang, and R. Ross. 2000. “Skeletal Muscle Mass and
Distribution in 468 Men and W omen aged 18–88 Yr.” Journal of Applied Physiology 89
(1): 81–88.
27. Roser, Appel, and Ritchie, “Human Height.”
28. See references for humans in V. Puppo. 2013. “Anatomy and Physiology of the
Clitoris, Vestibular Bulbs, and Labia Minora with a Review of the Female Orgasm
and the Prevention of Female Sexual Dysfunction.” Clinical Anatomy 26: 134–52.
29. D. E. McGhee and J. R. Steele. 2020. “Breast Biomechanics: What Do We
Really Know?” Physiology 35 (2): 144–56.
30. K. Clancy. 2023. Period: The Real Story of Menstruation. Princeton, NJ: Prince
ton University Press.
31. L. Eliot, A. Ahmed, H. Khan, and J. Patel. 2021. “Dump the ‘Dimorphism’:
Comprehensive Synthesis of Human Brain Studies Reveals Few Male-Female Dif-
ferences beyond Size.” Neuroscience Biobehavioral Reviews 125: 667–97. https://doi
.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026; L. Eliot. 2024. “Remembering the Null Hy-
pothesis When Searching for Brain Sex Differences.” Biology of Sex Differences 15:14.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13293-024-0 0585-4.
32. K. Sobhani, D. K. Nieves Castro, Q. Fu, R. A. Gottlieb, J. E. Van Eyk, and
C. Noel Bairey Merz. 2018. “Sex Differences in Ischemic Heart Disease and Heart
Failure Biomarkers.” Biology of Sex Differences 9 (1): 43. https://doi.org/10.1186
/s13293-018-0201-y.
33. L. J. Shaw, R. Bugiardini, and C. N. Merz. 2009. “Women and Ischemic Heart
Disease: Evolving Knowledge.” Journal of the American College of Cardiologists 54
(17): 1561–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jacc.2009.0 4.098.
34. A. Gasecka, J. M. Zimodro, and Y. Appelman. 2023. “Sex Differences in Anti-
platelet Therapy: State-of-the Art.” Platelets 34 (1): 2176173. https://doi.org/10.1080
/0 9537104.2023.2176173.
188 Not e s to Ch a p t er 7
62. J. L. Herman, A. R. Flores, and K. K. O’Neil. 2022. “How Many Adult and
Youth Identify as Transgender in the USA?” UCLA School of Law, Williams Institute.
https://williamsinstitute.law.ucla.edu/publications/trans-adults-united-states/.
63. Jordan-Young and Karkazis, Testosterone; but see Hooven, T: The Story of Tes-
tosterone, for opposite claims.
64. K. Nakajima and C. H. Jin. 2022. “Bills Targeting Trans Youth Are Growing
More Common—and Radically Reshaping Lives.” NPR, November 28. https://www
.npr.org/2022/11/28/1138396067/transgender-youth-bills-trans-sports.
65. The Trans Legislation Tracker. https://translegislation.com/.
66. G. R. Murchison, M. Agénor, S. L. Reisner, R. J. Watson. 2019. “School Rest
room and Locker Room Restrictions and Sexual Assault Risk among Transgender
Youth.” Pediatrics 143 (6): e20182902. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2018-2902;
L. Z. DuBois, J. A. Puckett, D. Jolly, S. Powers, T. Walker, D. A. Hope, R. Mocarski,
et al. 2024. “Gender Minority Stress and Diurnal Cortisol Profiles among Transgen-
der and Gender Diverse People in the United States.” Hormones and Behavior 159:
105473. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2023.105473.
I n de x
193
194 i n de x
“battle of the sexes” concept, 110, 121, body size and strength, 110–11
122–23 bonobos, 54, 55, 56, 64, 128
Beauvoir, Simone de, 42–43 brains, 3, 100–104; size of human, 41
bees, 24, 35, 155n5; reproductive organs
in, 2 California sheepshead fish, 27
behavioral diversity, 54 canine teeth, 50, 53, 70; dimorphism
bimodal definition of sex, 39–40 in, 57, 163n25
binary view of sex biology, 4, 84–85, cardiac disease, binary view of, 135–36
125–50; cardiac disease in, 135–36; cetaceans (whales and dolphins), 18
inadequacy of, 3–4, 21, 39–40, childcare: animals and, 18–20, 32, 49;
149–50; making family as part of, humans and, 55, 66, 110; role of
129–31; in medical research and group in 59–63, 68, 71
treatment, 131–35; organ transplant chimpanzees, 54–55, 56, 64; sexual
as aspect of, 136–37; pervasiveness behavior in, 64; subspecies of,
of, 125; pregnancy in, 137–39; sex 165n51
contextualism in, 139–40; sexuality chromosomes, 4, 5, 158n1; multiple
and sexual orientation in, 126–29; sets of, 25, 155n8; sex, 104;
sports and, 141–47 twenty-third, 44, 46, 85, 99, 104,
biocultural framing, 45, 46 106; X, 38, 88, 95, 104–5, 108; XX, 85,
biocultural view: of humans, 40–42, 95, 105, 158n1; XY, 46, 99, 105, 106,
47, 109; of organs, 90–91 137; Y, 38, 88, 104–5
biological binary, 8 clitoris, 16, 17, 33, 45, 86, 114
biological differences in communica- cloaca, 16, 30
tion, 109 clothing, 41, 43
biological sex, 158n1; gonads and, 86 cognition: battle of the sexes
biology, 11, 40; animal, 3–4, 22–37, 47; argument on, 121; differences
gender and, 45; human, 84–86; between males and female in,
reproductive, 1–2, 3–4, 11, 12. See also 111–12; as nonbinary, 113
sex biology “Cohens d” measure, 112–13
biomedical research, 140 communication: biological differences
biopsies, 85 in, 109; dynamics in, 112
bipedalism, 88 comparative approach, defined, 48
birds, 16, 23; heterosexuality in, 128; connectome, 100
parental care by, 18, 19–20, 30–31, coprodeum, 16
161n2. See also particular species cortisol, 98
birth mothers, reproductive process crocodilians: infant care by, 18, 30; sex
in, 130 biology in, 28–29
bisexuality, 127 cultural meaning, assignation of, 67
Bitch: On the Female of the Species cyanobacteria, 5
(Cooke), 36 Cytochrome P450 (CYP), 134
i n de x 195
orangutans, 53–54, 64; sex biology of, biology and behavior in, 49–50;
54; social lives of, 54; types of, 2 sexual behavior of, 128; sexual
organs: sex biology and, 90–91; selection in, 51; shapes and sizes of,
transplants of, as aspect of binary 52; social behavior and relationships
view problem, 136–37 among, 33–34, 50; social sex in, 63
ova, 8, 13, 15 proctodeum, 16
ovaries, 15–16, 17, 92, 94 progesterone, 92, 94, 97
ovotestis, 16, 153n17 prokaryotes, 5, 6
ovulation, 97–98 prolactin, 99
oxytocin, 185n7 protobirds, 30
pseudogynecomastia, 87
pair bonds, 52, 53, 65–67, 122, 130, pseudopenis, 33
167n70 psychiatric conditions, 135
pancreas, 92 psychological states and behaviors, 112
Pan genus, 54–55, 64 puberty, 87, 93, 94–95; mini puberty,
parathyroid glands, 91 93, 94
Parker, G. A., 10, 152n14
parthenogenesis, 12, 25 race, 91, 186n19
PAX2 gene, 172n24 religious beliefs, 44, 129
peafowl, 110 reproduction: asexual, 2, 5, 6, 7, 28, 127;
pelvic girdle, 59, 79 organs in, 1–2; patterns and trends
penis. See genitals associated with, 23; sexual, 6, 7, 8;
periodic table of elements, 22–23 sexually dimorphic patterns
Phelps, Michael, 145 associated with, 32; types of organs
phenotypes, development of bodily, 91 in, 2
physiology, reproductive, 23, 156n26 reproductive biology, 11, 20; differ-
pituitary gland, 91, 92 ences in, 2, 4
placenta, 17 reproductive physiology, 23, 156n26
placental mammals, 153n21 reproductive skew, 63
polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS), 95 reptiles: infant care by, 29–30; sex
polyploidy, 25 biology in, 28–30
power-lifting competitions, 82
pregnancy, 98–100, 108; binary view salamanders, 13
of, 137–39 scrotum, 33, 85
primates, 48–52; f ather as caretaker in, sea horses, 13
49; h umans as, 48; masturbation in, Semenya, Caster, 141, 143
114–15; monogamy in, 34, 35, 36; sensory stimulus, 185n7
mother as caretaker in, 49; sex: as bimodal, 39–40; evolution of,
pair-living, 52; patterns of care in, 4, 5–21; purposes of, 6–7, 11, 63–65;
52; reproductive sex in, 64; sex self-reported, 161n23
200 i n de x
testosterone, 53, 54, 91, 92, 94–97, 119, reproductive strategy of “more
185n7; range of functions in, sex, less investment,” 113; sexual
146–47; sexual arousal and, 117–18; nature of, 109–10; testosterone
sports and, 141, 142, 144; variation in, 111; types of, 53. See also men;
in circulating levels of, 134, 141–42, males
146 thyroid glands, 91
3G categories, 46–47, 161n23 tissues, adipose, 87–88
3G females, 46–47, 50, 56–57, 186n22; transgender individuals: early
battle of the sexes argument and, acceptance of, 43–44; growth in
122; biology associated with, 50; legislation against, 147; restroom
bodies of, 62; brains of, 100; cardiac prohibitions and, 147–49; sporting
disease in, 135–36; as correct events and, 144–45
nucleus of family, 130; defined, 46; transplantation: heart, 137; hemato-
differences from 3G males, 62, 65; poietic stem cell, 135; liver, 137;
effect of drugs on, 132–33; height of, lung, 137
74–75, 112; human hair in, 89, 90; Trivers, R. L., 10, 152n14
infant care by, 110; in laboratory turtles, 28
research, 131–32; muscle strength in, two sex model, 9
80–84; nutritional and caloric
needs of, 68; organs in, 90; organ UK Biobank dataset, 102–3
transplants in, 136–37; pelvic girdle undescended testes, 141
in, 79; physical description of, 68; unflanged males (orangutan), 54
pregnancy and related physiological urodeum, 16
changes in, 137–39; sexual nature of,
109–10. See also females; women vaginas, 16, 45
3G males, 46–47, 56–57; battle of the vasocongestion, 185n7
sexes argument and, 122; biology vasopressin, 185n7
associated with, 50; bodies of, 62; vellus hair, 89
brains of, 100; cardiac disease in, vertebrates: egg laying in, 16–17;
135–36; as correct nucleus of family, gonads in, 15–16
130; defined, 46; differences from vocalizations, 53
3G females, 62, 65; DNA in, 110; voluntary activation, 81, 83
effect of drugs on, 132–33; height of,
74–75, 112; hyperaggression in, 120; wasps, 24
initiation of homicide and sexual WEIRD (Western, Educated,
assault by, 119–20; in laboratory Industrialized, Rich, Democratic)
research, 131–32; muscle strength in, nations, 84
80–84; organs in, 90–91; organ Wilson, E. O., 10, 109, 152n14
transplants in, 136–37; pelvic girdle Wnt4, 106
in, 79; physical description of, 68; WNT gene, 172n24
202 i n de x
This book has been composed in Arno, an Old-style serif typeface in the
classic Venetian tradition, designed by Robert Slimbach at Adobe.