Psychology: Holland
Psychology: Holland
•';
r-/
t
Choice
.M)HN^L. HOLLAND
JOHN L. HOLLAND
The
Psychology
Vocational Choice
Raymond G. Kuhlen
Preface
vn
Viii PREFACE
add, is the choice of a wife or a dog. Again: "Vocational choice
is the implementation of a self-concept." But which self-concepts
lead to which choices? In short, typical theoretical statements
about vocational choice have been unspecific and devoid of defini-
tions and psychological content.
I also saw that the need for a way to synthesize our vocational
6. People in Environments 72
7. Life Histories 79
REFERENCES 97
INDEX 131
CHAPTER ONE
BACKGROUND CONCEPTS
Our knowledge of the personal characteristics and situational
forces related to the choice of a vocation has increased enor-
mously in recent years. We
have learned much about people by
the use of vocational interest inventories and of inventories and
devices for describing personality. At present, we know that a
and preferences are associated with
person's vocational interests
a great range of personal and background information. This
knowledge has led to several concepts and assumptions that
underlie the theory.
The choice of a vocation is an expression of personality. For
many years, it was popular to interpret a person's scores on voca-
tional interest inventories and his choice of vocation as a function
of his "vocational interests," as if these interests were different
from or independent of personality. A long history of adherence
to this concept produced an independent literature known as
"interest measurement." The work of Berdie [13], Strong [121],
Darley and Hagenah [27], and Super and Crites [124] epitomizes
the view that interest inventories measure interests, vocational
choices, and vocational preferences.
Growing knowledge about the personal and environmental
factors associated with a person's vocational choice made explicit
the need for a broader conception. This need became clearer
when we learned that vocational preferences are sometimes mod-
erately correlated with personality and originality scales [68],
self-ratings on various personality traits, daydreams about future
accomplishment, responses to certain projective devices, values
and goals, attitudes of parents, and many other personal and
need for a more
situational forces. Various writers suggested the
comprehensive view of vocational preferences and interests, al-
INTRODUCTION TO THE THEORY 3
though their statements were often tentative and limited: "interest
inventon- scores are measures of self-concept" [16]. "vocational
interest measurement is a special case in personality theory" [27],
"vocational choice is the implementation of a self-concept" [125],
and "vocational choice is developmental" [125]. Such statements
imply that a person's vocational choice is the outcome of his life
history and not a decision independent of his past life [106, 17].
If vocational preference is construed as an expression of per-
sonality, then "vocational interests" represent the expression of
personality in work, hobbies, recreational activities, and prefer-
ences. In short, what we have called "vocational interests" are
simply another aspect of personality. Just as we have developed
theories of personality from our knowledge of sex, parental re-
lationships,and behavior, so we can construct theories of per-
sonality from our knowledge of vocational life and reinterpret
as an expression of personality what we have called "vocational
interests."
Perhaps some examples will serve to make clear the meaning
of this reinterpretation. If we think of vocational interests and
personality as independent domains, we will regard "I like to
use tools to build things" as an expression of interest but "I like
to hit people with tools" as an expression of personality. Yet both
statements tell us how the subject uses tools— constructively or
destructively. To ignore the constructive use of tools and similar
activities asan index of personality is to ignore about half of
man's conscious life. To take another example, a "Yes" response
to the item "I would like to be a women's clothing designer" is
regarded as an interest item, but even a naive person would
regard "I like to wear women's clothes" (if answered "Yes" by
a man ) as an important sign of personal aberration, although the
content of both items is similar.
Interest inventories are personality inventories. If vocational
interests are an expression of personality, then it follows that
interest inventories are personality inventories. Forer [40] was
probably the first to develop an inventory to assess personality
from interests and activities and to illustrate how a subject's re-
sponses to apparently neutral content (vocational interests and
4 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
activities) could be interpreted as expressions of various dimen-
sions of personality. Unfortunately, Forer did not put his ideas
to a direct scientific test. He did, however, show that we can
distinguish a great variety of psychiatric and medical groups by
their scoreson various scales of an interest inventory, the Kuder
Preference Record [41].
Forer's theorizing led in part to the construction of Holland's
Vocational Preference Inventory [63], a personality inventory
composed entirely of occupational titles. Generally, scales were
developed by hypothesizing that preferences for occupations are
expressions of personality. The rationale for the development of
the inventory contains a more complete statement of this hy-
pothesis:
The individual scales for the fifth revision of the VPI have use-
ful reliability and validity. And more important, the development
and validation of the VPI make it clear that vocational preferences
are indeed signs of various personality traits. Similar support for
the concept has been provided by Garman [47], who used re-
sponses to the Strong Vocational Interest Blank to develop and
validate a scale measuring anxiety.
To summarize, we have clear evidence that it is useful to
interpret or construe what have been called vocational interest
inventories as personality inventories. Moreover, the content of
vocational interest inventories provides scales whose reliabilities
and validities approximate those obtained for other methods.
INTRODUCTION TO THE THEORY 5
Vocational stereotypes have reliable and important psycholog-
icaland sociological meanings. In the same way that we judge
people bv their friends, dress, and actions, so we judge them by
their vocations. Our everyday experience has generated a some-
times inaccurate but apparently useful knowledge of what people
in various occupations are like. Thus we believe that plumbers
are handy, lawyers aggressive, actors self-centered, salesmen per-
suasive, accountants precise, scientists unsociable, and the like.
SUMMARY
15
,
Table i
Enterprising. The model type has verbal skills for selling, dominat-
ing, leading; conceives of himself as a strong, masculine leader;
avoids well-defined language or work situations requiring long periods
of intellectual effort; is extraceptive; differs from the Conventional
type in that he prefers ambiguous social tasks and has a greater
concern with power, status, and leadership; is orally aggressive.
Vocational preferences include business executive, buyer, hotel man-
ager, industrial relations consultant, manufacturer's representative,
master of ceremonies, political campaign manager, real-estate sales-
man, restaurant worker, speculator, sports promoter, stock and bond
salesman, television producer, traveling salesman.
Artistic. The model type is problems that are highly
asocial; avoids
structured or require gross physical skills; resembles the Intellectual
type in being intraceptive and asocial; but differs from that type in
that he has a need for individualistic expression, has less ego strength,
is more feminine, and suffers more frequently from emotional dis-
* For other sources see references numbered 15, 21, 25, 26, 27, 28, 32, 41,
44, 46, 47, 48, 49, 53, 54, 57, 60, 71, 79, 82, 97, 98, 101, 103, 105, 107,
111, 116, 123, 130, 131, and 140.
THE PERSONALITY TYPES It)
at the time these studies were done, all the information available
on the same variable for each of the six types was used. Obviously,
information about people of one or two types is of little value,
because it does not tell us how the assessed types compare with
the excluded types. Additional empirical study is required before
we will be able to rely on the specific validity of each item in
these empirical summaries;* however, our practice was to derive
what meaning we could from such information by assuming its
validity; had we assumed that this method of classification simply
confounded error, any interpretation would have been meaning-
less.
The assumed to be
theoretical formulations for each type are
consistent with their empirical summaries, but they were arrived
at by a subjective process. They should be regarded as potentially
useful speculations rather than as substantive accounts of our
knowledge.
The description of each type is organized as follows conceptual
:
1.331-
mathematical aptitudes.
Personal Development: Our knowledge of the family back-
ground is limited. Father and mother tend to be well educated,
relative to parents of other types. Fathers express approval of
son's curiositv and mothers appear to have permissive attitudes
about child training.
Personality (Theoretical Formulations). The Intellectual person
asserts himself by gaining power through knowledge and intel-
lectual achievement. This maneuver frequently leads to extensive
though indirect control over other persons. His intellectuality is
probably in part a compensation for lack of social and motor
skills; it may be the result of the direct approval of his intelligence
[25]-
Aptitudes and Special Abilities: Tends to have high verbal
but low mathematical aptitude.
Personal Development: The comes from
Social type frequently
a rural area. Adolescents with rural backgrounds appear to be
more responsible, less antagonistic toward parents and other
THE PERSONALITY TYPES 7TJ
and leadership.
Achievement and Originality: Relative to the other types, the
Conventional person is one of the two types with the least po-
tential for creative performance. This outcome is extremely con-
sistent with our current knowledge of creativity and of this type
[92,93]-
Outlook and Perception: On tachistoscopic tests, he is inflexible
in readjustment of adaptation level (stereotyped and unoriginal),
subject to position influence (constricted), and unable to re-
organize well. Originality scales imply that he has a simple rather
than a complex outlook and that he is dependent upon others in
his judgments [25].
Aptitudes and Special Abilities: Has more mathematical than
verbal aptitude.
Personal Development: The correlation between family back-
ground and adult personality is closer for the Conventional type
than for any of the other types. The father places a low value
on being curious and independent. The mother, who tends to be
a somewhat isolated person, expresses restrictive attitudes about
child training; she suppresses sexand aggression. She encourages
action rather than reading or intellectual concerns. Although this
evidence comes from a grossly atypical sample, it seems clearly
and psychologically consistent with the passive, unoriginal, con-
stricted attributes of the Conventional person.
Personality (Theoretical Formulation). The Conventional per-
30 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
son enhances himself by identifying with great financial and
business leaders, by acquiring possessions, and by playing the
role of the good subordinate. He denies his dependency and
emphasizes the importance of perfection, prestige, and ambition.
Through this coping behavior he gains social and vocational
status.
The Conventional person lessens stress and anxiety by con-
forming to cultural norms and values and by identifying himself
with them. Likewise, he attains comfort by ingratiating himself
with others and by limiting his social relationships to people he
knows well. By avoiding persuasive and expressive activities, he
avoids dealing with his own and other people's feelings— phenom-
ena he cannot cope with easily because of his repressive early
training. Similarly, he defends himself by maintaining self-control,
being dependent, dealing with externals, repressing and denying,
rigidly restricting his life within narrow boundaries, and limiting
his involvement with his work and with other people. His choice
of rule-oriented vocations, with their explicit standards of right
and wrong, expresses his life style succinctly.
The Conventional person is most closely related to the Enter-
prising and the Social person. He differs from the Enterprising
person in being less sociable, aggressive, dominant, original,
enthusiastic (surgent), impulsive, self-confident, and adventur-
ous. He is also more responsible, dependent, and conservative
than the Enterprising person. The Conventional person differs
from the Social person in that he possesses greater self-control,
is more hard-headed, and is less dominant and nurturant.
self-confidence.
Achievement and Originality: Achieves in athletics and in
persuasive (leadership) and, to a lesser extent, artistic areas.
Relative to other types, his potential for original behavior is about
average.
Outlook and Perception: On tachistoscopic tasks the Enter-
prising person has poor reorganizational ability. His outlook is
colored by intense political convictions and status-oriented values.
Personal Development: The Enterprising person comes from
an urban rather than a rural area. His parents have a high socio-
economic status and are well educated; they have many books in
the home. His mother has pushed his development and sup-
pressed his interest in sex: these attitudes are conducive to some
of his adult attributes. His father wants him to be popular rather
than curious.
This background appears to reinforce the Enterprising person's
needs for recognition, love, and material rewards, which he seeks
in vocations providing similar gratifications. Such a background
helps to explain why the Enterprising person avoids academic
and intellectual vocations and moves toward people.
Personality (Theoretical Formulation). The Enterprising person
asserts and enhances himself by struggling for power and control,
by developing his athletic abilities, by acquiring possessions, and
by exploiting others. Some of these activities lead to the ac-
quisition of social and vocational status.
THE PERSONALITY TYPES 33
He and anxiety through hyperactivity, drinking
lessens stress
and eating, sexual expression, wit and humor, and repression and
denial. His defenses include oral aggression, dependence on
others, narcissism, regression, self-aggrandizement, identification
with strong leaders, and rationalization.
The Enterprising person differs from the Artistic person in that
the latter is more introverted, feminine, self-deprecating, creative,
unstable, independent, unconventional, and unsociable.
ASSESSMENT OF TYPES
ment.
Various quantitative methods have also been used to assess a
person's resemblance to the types. The Realistic, Intellectual,
Social, Conventional, Enterprising, and Artistic scales of the
Vocational Preference Inventory [62] provide a simple, brief
procedure for typing a person. First, the subject indicates the
vocations that appeal to him and those that do not from a list
of 84 occupational titles [14 occupations for each of the six
scales]. The six scales are scored and profiled. The higher a
person's score on a scale, the greater his resemblance to the
type that scale represents. His highest score represents his dom-
inant personality type; his profile of scores (obtained by ranking
- y
C 32
o
CO
I E t/5
3
CJ
•rt
4_> ^
» ^»
£>
3
S
3 C <r. CO
-3 c/)
CJ
C o u
< 23 4-T
uo
cj
4->
^
Pu
rt
CJ
3 CJ
o 3 .,
;/:
2 -3
^
t-i 3 t£ t 4-1
9 i 3CJ
1
o
c
b£
c
—
G
be
«
•
4->
c
^* W5
3
ESS8 CJ
£
s CO
rt S^O
•c > C
0) " rt
> h cu
4-1
Cu
ha
•s.
a
3
4)C §B 5 3
*4->
C « '3
o
•— !-3
Cu
4H
c
W
CJ
4-> a) cu C/5
k> W S
3^ 2Oo CO
4J a;
cj
>. 4^
^^ 3
'3 CJ 3
Ao
c "SO
^ CO rt r
H W a
3
PC 5
-C
W CO Ph pp <! Ph co CO 0)
£cj o ••> rt
cu
bO
3
—
rt
c rt 3 * § o « 3
o O 3 CC
5/5 C 3 O S ajPC-g CJ
CO — o _ *•> 3 rt TD
cu — rt
4-- rr *- 4-> •X3 3
"rt gj 3
>^
>
C
CJ
c
3
> •i-
3
p-4
rt
rt.
s
o
3
3
3
U
O
Gfl
cj
3
y
'
y.
'- CO
3
c > cu.£ 3
,
> O
C 3 -£ "3 a
£ "
•— 13
Q o
O U
C
c
o CuS
cj
o > UbJH
o
o > i CJ
— z 4-<
rt
C3 O <W .2 cj rt CU
3
C
o
W) >
3*< ^
Ph
Cu
3 CJ
CJ
v.
- co <u o o
u CJ
- § s O ^^3 o T3
1 •-
-
rt ^3 >% 3 **-! 3
s
o o a
CO
O ^
CO 5
>~*
> ^
CJ
Ua _<
co
tT
<i)
3 ^S
O <D ^ S^J C
3 —-T3 en
o -A "3 CJ
3 ^
^3
< * co
2
o rt o cati •uns
1 —> > CJ W CJ
Eh
f*
*»
ca
"S
o
CO
*S
co
?,
H—
rt*
'
'cj
co
31
W co H
rt
&>
>o ° rX
"S
o
CO
4->
<H-<
c
^ ^
^ o 3
cc
'rt
3 e 'rt cj
rt £
rt a rt
3 •h
3 bO
3 3 -a CJ
3 O cj
3
4-1 —3 CS
O 4->
o cj —O CJ V4H
4->
o cj W3
o
>H
o CJ . 4->
C
CJ Ql, cj T3 O
3 in rt «=« 3 —
— 'a; "11 ^ rt
4-1
O •S, 4-<
S-rt* CO
c c J3
0_ ^ "S
co O Ph
3 «« 3^ <u
•—
-a
-fl.2 S 3 CJ
b04H « 4-1 4-1
CJ
o 'Cu
- rt e ^ •£>•€ •c £
— "3
C 3 a •-H 4-1 o
a > '-'
c 3
rt CJ
—y. i-
C <-> >^ rt
«H
oo CJ
eg 4-> rt -
3'S "«
cj
PC "rt .3 CU
3« C 6C
E
3 «
B CJ §
M «> w CJ
S^x o
cj
OS w< CO S Ph
JS
o Wl
O O £ /
—
0)
-.2 Ph T3
—-
.,. —
o
CJ
^~- 0) 3
3 3 O
s
6
vi>
i—i
P3 3
C
•—a
—
cd
5
Cu
>^
CJ
N8" T3
c o 3 >^ > (U O <- 'rt*
3
— c o C t- £
Ph ^fc -icO > O ^ 3
o £>
CJ
Ch
c
s 2 -3 CO
Gfl -
O ^ "3
V. b «3 3 <
cu
Q -
CJ
u
od
> ^
- o £ 8 o
E
»-
*o - CO ifS ii
CJ
Cj
CO
E co 2 -o s -3 c °
>
c
^ O o U ? B. Z 2 >-o
38 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
the scale scores from highest to lowest) represents his personality
pattern.
The Strong Vocational Blank [121] has also been used
Interest
to assess a person's resemblance to each type. For example, the
Aviator scale of the Strong was used to represent the Realistic
type, the Physicist scale to represent the Intellectual type, the
Social Science Teacher scale for the Social type, the Accountant
scale for the Conventional type, the Sales Manager scale for the
Enterprising type, and the Musician scale for the Artistic type.
In principle, the Kuder Preference Record [84] and other interest
inventories can also be used for this purpose.
The Personal Survey (see Appendix B), a brief experimental
inventory, was designed for the express purpose of defining a
person's type. To develop the inventory, items that best discrim-
inated among types were selected from several studies [67, 68,
69, 72]. They were chosen with the view of obtaining a compre-
hensive assessment of the subject's goals and values, self-concepts,
competencies, coping behavior, identifications, and interests. For
each kind of content, there are an equal number of items. The
survey includes, among other things, a list of self-descriptive
adjectives (for example, aloof, argumentative, introverted), self-
ratings of other traits and abilities (absent-mindedness, mechan-
ical ability, importance of
originality), ratings of the relative
various life goals, and ratings of famous people thought worthy
of emulation. The Personal Survey may in the future provide
more useful definitions and better predictions than the Vocational
Preference Inventory or the Strong Vocational Interest Blank.
Table 2 summarizes the qualitative and quantitative methods for
defining the types.
In short, we have defined a person's resemblance to each type
by his vocational interests as manifested in his vocational and
educational preferences, his current employment, or his scores
on certain interest scales. These definitions are "approximate,"
because they are working definitions whose simplicity lends it-
self to research and redefinition. At present, the Vocational Pref-
Table 3
The Relationships Among the Personality Types As
Estimated by Corresponding Scales of the
Vocational Preference Inventory
1 2 3 4 5 6
Real Int Soc Conv Ent Art
Note: Correlations boys (N = 307) are below the diagonal; those for
for
girls (N = 226) are above the diagonal. Both groups are National Merit
Finalists. Similar patterns of relationships can be observed in Strong's data
for the SVIB. See Table 29, pp. 136-137 of Vocational Interests of Men
and Women [121].
THE PERSONALITY TYPES 41
Table 4
The Coding of Interest Inventory Scales for
the Study of Types and Subtypes
A 10 20 70 60 90 5o 53
B 80 90 20 30 40 10 21
C 10 40 50 35 40 85 63
SUMMARY
The which probably represent common outcomes of
types,
growing up in our culture, have been described in terms of a
theoretical model and an empirical definition. The personality
types are models for organizing knowledge, stimulating research,
and conceptualizing personality.
CHAPTER THREE
Personality Types
42
PERSONALITY TYPES AND PERFORMANCE 43
Enterprising, and Artistic. The primary direction is a function
of the dominant characteristic (that is, the model type he most
resembles) of his personality pattern.
The secondary direction of vocational choice is a function of
the secondary characteristic of his personality pattern: that is,
the model type that the person resembles secondarily. This sec-
ondary direction determines his choice of role within the major
vocational class of his choice. For example, a prospective engineer
mav become a researcher, a supervisor, a teacher, or a consultant
in the field of engineering; his preference constitutes the second-
ary direction. The secondary direction represents a specification
—a narrowing or focusing— of choice; it is analogous to "the funnel
process" that Hahn and MacLean [59] ascribe to vocational expe-
rience. Studies of secondary direction and subsequent develop-
ment have been made for various vocational groups: engineers
Realistic or the Intellectual more than the other four types. For
the person with an inconsistent pattern, the direction of choice
will more frequently be determined by environmental contin-
PERSONALITY TYPES AND PERFORMANCE 45
gencies, because his unstable pattern lessens his self -direction.
In general, resistance to external pressure probably depends
somewhat more on the consistency of the personality pattern
than on the dominant personality type. A person with a consistent
pattern (for instance, a Social-Enterprising type) is less likely
to be swayed by external forces than is a person with an incon-
sistent pattern (for instance, a Social-Intellectual type). Con-
sistent patternsshould probably be regarded as integrated pat-
terns—integrated in the sense that they represent similar or
complementary values, attitudes, and coping mechanisms. Crites'
finding [22] that "ego strength" is positively associated with the
presence of interest patterning on the Strong seems to illustrate
this phenomenon.
The potency that a particular environmental pressure will
exert on a person's direction of choice is a function of the dom-
inant type represented in the personality pattern, the degree to
which this type dominates the pattern, and the consistency of
the profile. For example, a consistent personality pattern repre-
sented by a profile peak on the Intellectual scale followed by a
much lower peak on the Realistic scale, and only negligible
elevations on the remaining scales, would be both more predict-
able and more sensitive to those influences which, according to
the hypothesis, most strongly affect the Intellectual type. In con-
trast, a person with an inconsistent personality pattern dominated
by peak on the Intellectual scale followed by a peak on
a profile
the Social scale would be less predictable and less sensitive to
the influences assumed to affect the Intellectual type. The en-
vironmental situations that are likely to be the most and the least
influential with each type are as follows.
influences.
5. The Enterprising type is most sensitive to social, emotional,
enthusiastic, and materialistic influences and least sensitive to
intellectual, humanitarian, and idealistic influences.
6. The Artistic type is most sensitive to personal, emotional
Table 5
Relation Between the Student's High-Point Code and His
Response to the Question "If You Had Unlimited Financial
Resources, Would Your First Choice Be a Different One?"
Would
* 3 4 6
Change * J>
~«
Real
,
Int Soc Conv Ent A
Art x
Choice
Boys (N= 148) 12.3 7.6 18.5 17.1 32.7 22.0 25. 12
Girls (N = 86) 16.4 19.5 9.3 14.8 17.9 32.2 i5-94t
Note: Only 17.8 and 18.7 per cent of the total sample of boys and girls,
respectively, indicated they would change their choices if unlimited funds
were available. The "high-point code" is the scale on which the person
achieves his highest score.
*
p <.ooi.
t p <.oi.
PERSONALITY TYPES AND PERFORMANCE 47
Enterprising codes said they would change their vocational choice
if thev had unlimited financial resources, whereas only 7.6 per
cent of the students with Intellectual codes said they would
change. This extreme contrast seems consistent with the values
expressed by persons in each of the two occupational codes. The
findings for girls are more ambiguous: girls with Artistic choices
would change their goals more readilv than girls with Social
choices. These differences in susceptibility to external influence
may be attributable to cultural biases about the "impracticality"
of Artistic occupations and the "practicality" of Social occupa-
tions for girls.
Vocational Achievement. In addition to determining the direc-
tion of vocational choice, the personality pattern also deter-
mines the person's level of vocational aspiration and achievement.
A consistent personality pattern makes for effective functioning.
We may assume that an integrated pattern makes for stability
and integrated effort, and therefore a person with a consistent
pattern is more likely to survive at a given kind of work.
People who have not onlv a consistent pattern but also a close
resemblance to the Social or the Enterprising type are likely to
have higher aspirations, to achieve more frequently, and to be
more occupationallv mobile. Other things being equal, people
with closer resemblances to these two types achieve more than
people with fewer resemblances. (Here "other things" include the
person's intelligence and social background, factors that are im-
portant determinants of achievement but that are partially incor-
porated in the types themselves. ) No doubt their self-confidence,
enthusiasm, and perceptiveness in dealing with others— common
requirements for achievement in our culture— account for the
greater success of these types.
The level of vocational aspiration is also related to the per-
sonality types. Enterprising, Social, and Artistic types tend to
overevaluate their potential, and thus to have higher aspirations;
Conventional, Intellectual, and Realistic types tend to underrate
themselves. We see here a reflection of the "realism hypothesis"
frequently discussed in vocational counseling: "unrealistic" voca-
tional aspirations (that is, aspirations that are either too high or
48 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
and potential ) are associated
too low in terms of a person's ability
with the extremes of overevaluation and underevaluation. Thus,
we would expect both the Enterprising and the Realistic types
to have "unrealistic" vocational goals.
Moreover, people with consistent codes are more likely to make
"realistic" choices and to have reasonable aspirations, because a
consistent code implies an integrated person who knows himself
well. Consequently, an accurate self-evaluation is one facet of
integrity and stability.
Work History (Career Pattern). From the previous statements
about stability of vocational choice, we can make certain predic-
tions about the character of the work historv. The Realistic and
Intellectual types and people with consistent codes will be more
vocationally stable ( that is, they will change jobs less often ) than
people who resemble the other four types or people with in-
consistent codes.
Work Satisfaction. Expressions of work satisfaction will be as-
sociated with consistent rather than inconsistent codes. Persons
with inconsistent codes may be expected to express dissatisfaction
more often, because they are by definition somewhat dissatisfied
and self-contradictory and because they appear to have conflict-
ing motives.
ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT
CREATIVE PERFORMANCE
SUMMARY
This chapter spells out some of the obvious implications of the
formulations for the personality types. The models can be used
for constructing hypotheses about a number of other human
problems. For example, we might ask what kind of leadership
is ideal for each model? If we wanted to bring about changes
52
THE ENVIRONMENTAL MODELS 53
Because the personality types and the environmental models
share a common set of constructs, it is possible to classify people
and environments in the same terms and thus to predict the
outcome of pairing people and environments. More explicitly: to
predict what will happen when a particular person is put into a
particular environment, we need only to characterize the person
and his environment in terms of the models and to review the
appropriate formulations in order to discover the congruities and
incongruities the models suggest. For example, the interaction of
a Realistic type and a Realistic environment should produce a
number of desirable outcomes, such as work satisfaction, achieve-
ment, and vocational stability.* Our typology gives us a tremen-
dous advantage both in planning research and in interpreting
results. Without it, we would have to deal with a formidable
number of possible person-environment interactions; with it, we
have relatively few variables to juggle and an explicit rationale
to guide us.
The construction model environments
of the rests mainly on
the suggestion made by Linton [89] and others that most of our
environment is transmitted through other people. This idea im-
plies that the character ofan environment is dependent upon the
nature of its members, and that the dominant features of an
environment are dependent upon the typical characteristics of
its members. If we know what kind of people make up a group,
then, we can infer the climate that the group creates. For ex-
ample, an office full of engineers would be expected to have a
different atmosphere than an office full of accountants. Or, an
informal gathering of salesmen would differ in atmosphere from
an informal gathering of ministers.
A large portion of the model environment consists of those
attributes of the model types that affect others. In addition, be-
cause model types are characterized by a preference for special
tasks and situations, the description of model environments by
means of model types results in the delineation of tasks and
Chapters Five and Six provide a complete account of the explanatory
values of the theory for these purposes.
54 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
situations thought to be associated with and congenial to the
different personality types.
The descriptions of the model environments in this chapter
follow the plan for the model types: Conceptual definition; em-
pirical definition; empirical characterization in terms of typical
tasks, situations, and interpersonal relationships; and conceptual
formulation. The information about occupational environments
was obtained from Fine's United States Employment Service
study of 4000 jobs [39, 132]. This information was sorted into
six categories, using the lists of occupations shown in Table 1.
ments are light: talking, reading, and verbal and ideational learn-
ing. Work activities usually take place indoors.
Interpersonal Relationships: Minimal social skills are de-
manded, although ability to give and receive complicated written
and oral instructions is necessary. Need for sensitivity to the
needs and problems of others is minimal. Ability to relate ef-
fectively to Intellectual types is helpful in achieving personal
goals and acquiring vocational status, because the Intellectual
environment is populated principally by Intellectual types.
ASSESSMENT CRITERIA
SUMMARY
The model environments are useful in suggesting research and
in categorizing the character of an environment. It is assumed
that environments can be characterized by a six-variable typol-
ogy that will provide a simple, empirical tool especially helpful
for predicting and understanding the various outcomes of a given
person's being placed in a given environment.
CHAPTER FIVE
Environmental Models
VOCATIONAL BEHAVIOR
63
64 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
should be qualified: The person is attracted also to those tasks
and situations that gratify his personal needs and give him satis-
faction.
There is some tentative support for the hypothesis that attrac-
tion to a group is a function of the satisfaction of needs provided
by membership in the group [120]. The converse also seems true.
Libo [88] found that people regard as unattractive those groups
in which they have to discuss topics uninteresting to them.
Individual items from various interest inventories illustrate
the attraction of environmental presses. In the Strong Vocational
Interest Blank, for example, the subject is given an opportunity
to express his preference for various kinds of supervision, for
opportunity to use his knowledge and experience, for opportunity
for promotion, for opportunity to seek help about difficulties, and
for similar environmental attributes.
Table 6 shows the degree of positive and negative attractive-
ness that the different environments have to each personal type.
This table makes explicit the hypothesis that a given environment
Table 6
The Attractiveness of the Model Environments to
Different Personality Types
Model Environments
Personality 1 2 3 4 5 6
Types Real Int Soc Conv Ent Art
1 Realistic ++ + — — —
2 Intellectual + ++ — — +
3 Social — ++ — + +
4 Conventional — — + ++ +
5 Enterprising — + — ++ +
6 Artistic — — + — ++
Note: (++) equals "very equals "attractive." ( — ) equals
attractive." ( + )
Environmental Models
1 2 3 4 5 6
Roles Real Int Soc Conv Ent Art
Practitioner 1 2 1 1 4 1
Teacher or therapist 5 4 2 4 3 4
Leader or supervisor 2 5 3 2 1 5
Expert or consultant 3 3 6 3 5 3
Researcher 4 1 5 5 6 2
Multiple roles 6 6 4 6 2 6
ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT
CREATIVE PERFORMANCE
SUMMARY
This attempt to spell out the implications of the model environ-
ments is grossly incomplete. No doubt a variety of other hypoth-
eses about the nature of environments will be apparent to readers
ENVIRONMENTAL MODELS AND INFLUENCES Jl
People m Environments
72
PEOPLE IN ENVIRONMENTS 73
environment "strain" and to formulate hypotheses about the
nature of such strains and their relation to anxiety and changes
in behavior.
Earlier work by Stern, Stein, and Bloom [115] and Sanford
[109] dealt with this question of the psychological "fit" of person
and environment. They proposed some minimal stress is
that
probably necessary for stimulating people to change and to cope
with environmental problems. If the environment and person
are an exact fit, then the need for change is negligible. But if the
person and environment are slightly incompatible, the resultant
stress stirs the person to act constructively. However, if the person
and the environment are so grossly incompatible that the person
cannot, even with great effort, succeed, then such an interaction
becomes destructive.
CONGRUENT-INCONGRUENT INTERACTIONS
Generally, congruent person-environment interactions (that is,
PEOPLE IN ENVIRONMENTS 75
CONSISTENT-INCONSISTENT INTERACTIONS
43-44 )
Using six scales of the Strong to represent the types, Holland
[68] found that the consistency of a student's code was related to
his remaining in a major field over a four-year period. Consistency
was also related to achievement, but these findings were equivocal.
In addition, students with consistent codes who attended colleges
with consistent codes (as measured by the EAT) were more
stable in their major field than were any other student-college
combination.
-/6 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
HOMOGENEOUS-HETEROGENEOUS INTERACTIONS
The chief effect of pairing a homogeneous type and a homoge-
neous environment is to make the expected outcomes even more
probable. To be more specific: we have hypothesized that the
interaction of a consistent type and a consistent environment is
Figure 1
99 ^^- 99
l l
COMBINATIONS OF CONGRUENCY,
CONSISTENCY, AND HOMOGENEITY
It is assumed that interactions involving different degrees of
congruency, consistency, and homogeneity will result in different
kinds and degrees of outcomes. At one extreme, the interaction
of a type and a model environment that are congruent, consistent,
and homogeneous will intensify and make more predictable such
outcomes as vocational choice and stability, vocational and
academic achievement, personal stability, and perhaps creative
performance. For example, a Social-Artistic person whose pattern
is consistent and homogeneous, and who enters a Social-Artistic
SUMMARY
The outcomes associated with the interactions of congruent,
consistent, and homogeneous persons and environments have
been outlined. Generally, it is assumed that congruent, consistent,
and homogeneous interactions have more potent and predictable
effects than do incongruent, inconsistent, and heterogeneous inter-
actions. Some evidence in the psychological literature supports
the validity of these assertions.
CHAPTER SEVEN
Life Histories
periods of time: the life history. For our purpose, life history (or
icork history, as we shall sometimes call it)* may be regarded as
representing a particular pattern of living: what Adler has
termed life style.
By using the definitions of the personality types and model
environments, we can, theoretically, trace a person's develop-
ment from the time he can express a vocational preference to his
death and examine the principal environments in which he lives
during his entire lifetime. The person can be assessed by his
vocational preferences at different points in his life. His environ-
ments—family background, school, work situations, and so on—
can be assessed using the environmental models. Although this
theoretical orientation has been little used, it has promise. By
applying the theory, with its small number of classes and rel-
atively simple sets of definitions, to an entire life span, we can
utilize a great range of information about human behavior and
* The terms life history and work history seem preferable to career or
career pattern. The concept of not sufficiently comprehensive.
career is
Wilenskv [139], for example, writes, "When the idea of career is taken
seriously and applied to large populations, we see that few men are gripped
by careers for the entire worklife, only a minority for as much as half
the worklife."
79
80 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
environments, and thus more easily study person-environment
interactions and their outcomes. The application of the theory
to the life history is outlined in the following sections: "The
Longitudinal Model," "Personal Development and Stability,"
"Vocational Behavior," "Academic Achievement," "Creative Per-
formance," "Types and Personality Change."
Figure 2
The Interactions of PcrsoJis and Environments
Y
Churches and Students
Other (1-6)
Institutions
(1-6)
Y
Work Situation Adults
No. 2 (1-6) (1-6)
Choice
Table 9
A More Complex Coding Procedure for Work Histories
Work Histories
Time A B C
Chemist Elementary-school Car salesman
teacher
1 year Social worker Liquor salesman
2 vears Sales person
]
Insurance salesman
3 vears Small business owner
4 vears Insurance salesman
5 vears Real-estate salesman
6 years > r > t i
7 years Chemist Sales person Real-estate salesman
Code 10 21 + 41 = 62 25
Note: When changes of several kinds occur, they can be coded and
summed as in Historv B.
84 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
Son Daughter
(54) (24)
Job, (E 54 ) Job x (E 4C )
Time
(54) (24)
Job, (E 54 ) Job 2 (E 45 )
(54) (42)
Job„ (E 34 ) Job n (E 52 )
(54) (42)
VOCATIONAL BEHAVIOR
ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT
It is assumed that high academic achievement is another
product of a stable history. The dominance of Intellectual, Social,
Artistic, and Conventional types in a person's personality pattern
is conducive to success in academic work because such types
have values, goals, and attitudes that are congruent with those of
their teachers. Conversely, personality patterns dominated by
Realistic and Enterprising types are less successful academically
because of their relative incongruence with academic environ-
ments.
CREATIVE PERFORMANCE
congruent.
In the present theory, it isassumed that some types are more
receptive to social experience and thus more prone to change
than are others. The types listed in descending order in terms
of their potential for change are Social, Enterprising, Conven-
tional, Artistic, Intellectual, Realistic. Moreover, the person with
an inconsistent pattern, because his values, needs, and abilities
SUMMARY
The extension of the theory to life patterns provides a way for
conceptualizing personal development and the different outcomes
associated with different patterns of development. Of equal im-
portance, the longitudinal model suggests some of the conditions
that lead to stable or unstable patterns.
The factors determining change in the person and in the en-
vironment are not altogether clear. Levinson's discussion [87]
is useful in showing the complexity of the interaction of person
Applications
89
90 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
At this point the usefulness of the theory is only partially ex-
plored. The empirical been performed have
studies that have
been limited to investigating only a few of the hypotheses and
have dealt with atypical samples [67, 68, 69, 71, 72, no, 118].
We need many more comprehensive studies of the following
aspects of the theory: (1) the personality types and their hy-
pothetical attributes, (2) the model environments and their
hypothetical attributes, (3) the life histories as revealed by
personal histories and work histories, (4) the interaction of
people and environments, especially studies of the same people
over extended time intervals. The individual chapters suggest some
starting points for these studies. Ideally, they should use large
samples, the larger the better, because classifications by sex, type,
and subtype reduce the subjects in any given category to a
number so low that reliable statistical tests are not possible.
We need to study a greater variety of occupations simultane-
ously. Studies using the "closed system"— that is, studies such as
Nachmann's [97] and Segal's [111], which are limited to single
occupations or pairs of occupations— though suited to investigat-
ing special problems, leave much to be desired. They are dif-
ficult to execute because of prohibitive research costs, and they
SUMMARY
Like most psychological theories, the present one will require
extensive investigation to determine its general usefulness. At
this time, several studies suggest that it has some value, but only
after we have applied it to a greater range of populations and
problems will we be able to evaluate its usefulness with any
certainty. Similarly, its practical applications need be extended
to
and verified in a variety of settings: clinical, educational, and
industrial.
H arenas
53, 224-235.
6. Astix, A. W. on the Motivation of
"Differential College Effects
Talented Students To Obtain the Ph.D." Journal of Educa-
tional Psychology, 1963, 54, 63-71.
7. Astix, A. W. "Effects of Different College Environments on the
Vocational Choices of High Aptitude Students." Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 1965, 12, 28-34.
8. Astix, A. W. "Further Validation of the Environmental Assess-
ment Technique." Journal of Educational Psychology, 1963,
54, 217-226.
9. Astix, A. W., and J.
"The Environmental Assessment
L. Holland.
Technique: A Way To Measure College Environments." Jour-
nal of Educational Psychology, 1961, 52, 308-316.
10. Barxette, W. L., Jr. "Occupational Aptitude Patterns of Se-
lected Groups Counseled Veterans." Psychological Mon-
of
ographs, 1961, 65 (Whole No. 322).
97
98 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
11. Beardslee, D. C, and D. D. O'Dowd. "College Student Images
of a Selected Group of Professions and Occupations." Wes-
leyan University, Middletown, Connecticut, i960. (Mimeo)
12. Beilin, H. "Discussion of the Super, Tiedeman, and Hall Papers."
Paper read at the American Personnel and Guidance Associa-
tion meetings, Chicago, 1962.
13. Berdie, R. F. "Validities of the Strong Vocational Interest Blank."
In W. Layton (Ed.), The Strong Vocational Interest Blank:
L.
Research and Uses. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press, i960. Pp. 18-61.
14. Blau, P. M.,
J.
W. Gustad, R. Jessor, H. S. Parnes, and R. C.
Wilcock. "Occupational Choice: A Conceptual Framework."
Industrial Labor Relations Review, 1956, 9, 531-543.
15. Block, J.,
and P. Petersen. "Q-Sort Item Analyses of a Num-
ber of Strong Vocational Interest Inventory Scales." OERL,
AFPTRC, ARDC, 1955.
16. Bordin, E. S. "A Theory of Interests As Dynamic Phenomena."
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1943, 3, 49-66.
17. Bordin, E. S., Barbara Nachmann, and S. J. Segal. "An Artic-
ulated Framework Development." Journal of
for Vocational
Counseling Psychology, 1963, 10, 107-117.
18. Boyd, J. B. "Interests of Engineers Related to Turnover, Selec-
tion, and Management." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1961,
45, 143-149.
19. Clark, K. E. Vocational Interests of Nonprofessional Men. Min-
neapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 1961.
20. Cooley, W. W. "Career Development of Scientists." Cooperative
Research Project No. 436, Office of Education, Graduate
School of Education, Harvard University, 1963.
21. Cottle, W. C. "A Factorial Study of the Multiphasic, Strong,
Kuder, and Bell Inventories Using a Population of Adult
Males." Psychometrika, 1950, 15, 25-47.
22. Crites, O. "Ego-Strength in Relation to Vocational Interest
J.
Development." Journal of Counseling Psychology, i960, 7,
137-143-
23. Crites, J.
O. "Factor Analytic Definitions of Vocational Motiva-
tion." Journal of Applied Psychology, 1961, 45, 330-337.
24. Crites, J.
O. Vocational Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill,
in press.
25. Crutchfield, R. S., D. G. Woodworth, and R. E. Albrecht.
REFERENCES C)C)
Peterson, 1957.
39. Fine, S. A. "The Structure of Worker Functions." Personal and
Guidance Journal, 1955, 34, 66-73.
40. Forer, B. R. "A Diagnostic Interest Blank." Rorschach Research
Exchange, and Journal of Projective Techniques, 1948, 12,
1—11.
41. Forer, B. R. "Personality Dynamics and Occupational Choice."
Paper read at the American Psychological Association meet-
ings, 1951.
42. Forer, B. R. "Personality Factors in Occupational Choice." Edu-
cational and Psychological Measurement, 1953, 13, 361-366.
43. Forsyth, R. P., and G. W. Fairweather. "Psychotherapeutic and
Other Hospital Criteria: The Dilemma." Journal of Abnormal
and Social Psychology, 1961, 62, 598-604.
44. Friend, Jeannette G., and E. A. Haggard. "Work Adjustment
in Relation to Family Background." Psychological Monographs,
1948, No. 16.
45. Fromm, E. Man for Himself. New York: Rinehart, 1947.
46. Galinsky, M. D. "Personality Development and Vocational
Choice of Clinical Psychologists and Physicists." Journal of
Counseling Psychology, 1962, 9, 299-305.
47. Garman, G. A., and L. Uhr. "An Anxiety Scale for the Strong
Vocational Interest Inventory: Development, Cross- Validation
and Subsequent Tests of Validity." Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 1958, 42, 241-246.
48. Gee, Helen H. "A Correlational Matrix for the Strong Vocational
Interest Blank and the Edwards Personal Preference Sched-
ule." Association of American Medical Colleges, Evanston,
Illinois, 1957.
49. Gee, Helen H. "Differential Characteristics of Student Bodies
—Implications for the Study of Medicine." Paper read at the
Conference on Selection and Educational Differentiations,
May, 1959, Berkeley, California, sponsored by the Field
Service Center and the Center for the Study of Higher Edu-
cation.
50. Gehlmann, F. "Objective Tests As Indicators of Personality and
Temperament." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University
of Chicago, 1951.
REFERENCES 101
65. Holland, J.
L. "A Classification for Occupations in Terms of
Personality and Intelligence." American Psychologist, 1959,
14, 476. (Abstract)
66. Holland, J.
L. "The Meaning of Interests." Paper presented at
102 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
the American Personnel and Guidance Association meetings,
Chicago, March, 1962.
67. Holland, J. L. "Some Explorations of a Theoiy of Vocational
Choice: I. One- and Two-Year Longitudinal Studies." Psy-
chological Monographs, 1962, 76, 26 (Whole No. 545).
68. Holland, J.
L. "Some Explorations of a Theory of Vocational
Choice and Achievement: A
Four-Year Prediction Study."
II.
109
HO THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
found by inspecting the correlations between each occupational
title and the six scales— Realistic, Intellectual, Social, Conven-
Part I
1. Realistic Occupations
College Teachers Of
Engineering and engineering physics
Agriculture
Mining
Animal husbandry
Miscellaneous
Draftsman (engineering, maps, etc.)
Surveyor
Weather observer
Detective, FBI agent
Mail carrier
Policeman, fireman
Laboratory technician (not medical technologist or tester)
2. Intellectual Occupations
Related Scientists
Anthropologist
Archeologist
Architect
Computer design and programer
Dentist
Experimental psychologist
Inventor
112 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
Mathematician (mathematics in business or industry 4)
Philosopher
Scientific research worker
Statistician
Veterinarian
Medical technologist
Medical researcher
Science Writers
Writer of scientific articles
Editor of scientific journal
Science-fiction writer
College Teachers Of
Physical and biological sciences
Research science
Experimental psychology
Mathematics
Philosophy
Premedical (girls only, boys are 3)
Predental
Computer design and programing
Astrophysics
3. Social Occupations
Religious Workers
Minister, foreign missionary (including medical missionary)
Chaplain in armed services
Social worker
YMCA secretary, Boy Scout official, director of welfare agency
College Tcaehcrs Of
Theology
Premedical (boys only, girls are 2)
Home economics, dietetics
Education
Sociology
Psychology (except experimental 2)
Nursing
Speech therapy, speech correction
Miscellaneous
College teacher when field is not specified
Conciliator (employee-employer)
Employment interviewer
Judge
Psychologist when field is not specified
Public health officer
Truant officer (education)
4. Conventional Occupations
Financial Workers
Accountant (certified public accountant, actuary, auditor, book-
keeper)
Allbank employees except officials
Budget reviewer, financial analyst
Cashier
Cost estimator, cost engineer
Credit investigator
Rate analyst
Secretarv-treasurer of firms
Tax expert, Internal Revenue agent
Office Workers
Clerk (post office, payroll, shipping and receiving, etc.)
IBM equipment operator
) )
College Teachers Of
Accounting
Banking
Business (not administration or management 5)
Commerce and finance
Economics
Miscellaneous
Customs liquidator
Efficiency expert
Inventory controller
Quality control
Records supervisor
Real-estate appraiser
Statistician (except where theoretical or nonapplied statistician is
specified 2)
Proofreader
Adjudicator
Supply officer
5. Enterprising Occupations
Travel consultant
(Not pharmacist)
)
APPENDIX A 115
Managers and Supervisors
Business executive and manager (not secretary-treasurer 4)
Sales manager, sales engineer
Foreman, supervisor and other men (not machines 1)
of production
Directors ( of research and development laboratories, etc.
(Not office manager 4)
Lawyers (attorney, counsel or private or corporation, not judges 3)
College Teachers Of
Business administration and management (only, see 4)
International relations (foreign service programs)
Political science and government
Prelaw, Law
History
Miscellaneous
Master of ceremonies
Politician (congressional lobbyist, political campaign manager, state
governor)
Radio -TV program director, announcer, producer
College president or dean
Diplomat, foreign service officer, United Nations officer (not secre-
tary 4, translator 6)
Personnel manager
Labor relations man (industrial relations)
Insurance claims adjustor
Industrial psychologist
6. Artistic Occupations
Creative Artists
Writer, editor (novelist, journalist, newspaper reporter, advertising
copy writer)
Artist, designer, decorator (portrait artist, furniture or clothing de-
signer, window decorator, interior decorator, advertising layout
man)
Theatrical artists (actor, stage director)
Musician (arranger, composer, pop singer, etc.)
College Teachers Of
English
Theater, dramatics
Il6 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
Art
Music
Journalism
Speech (general)
Miscellaneous
Art and music critic
Art dealer
Cartoonist
Humorist
Linguist, translator, interpreter
Pharmacist
Professional student
Tramp
Playboy
Unspecified civil service or government work
Part II
1. Realistic Occupations
* If an item had its highest correlation with the Realistic Scale and its
2. Intellectual Occupations
3. Social Occupations
(code 36 )
Florist (.40)
English teacher (.36)
120 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
4. Conventional Occupations
(code 41)
5. Enterprising Occupations
(code 51)
(code 54)
Liquor salesman (.57)
Insurance manager (.70)
Private investigator (.46)
Banker (.69)
Treasury agent ( .46)
Stockbroker (.67)
Counter-intelligence man (.41)
Headwaiter (.61)
Gambling dealer (.33)
Route salesman (.44)
Armv officer (.41)
(code 56)
TV producer (.83)
Radio announcer (.62)
6. Artistic Occupations
Part III
Agriculturef Agriculture
Agricultural education Agricultural education
Industrial artsf Physical education
Engineering:]: Recreation
Forestry f Industrial arts
Trade and industry Engineering
Animal husbandry! Forestry
Mining Trade and industry
Architecture Architecture
Biological sciences! Biological sciences
Geography Geography
t
APPENDIX A 123
2. Intellectual EAT Majors
9
3. Social EAT Majors
Accounting! Accounting
Secretarial Secretarial
00 Business and commercial
Business and commercial
(general and unclass.) (general and unclass.)
Business education Business education
Economics! Library science
Finance!
) )
6 1 Aloof F3 16 Helpful
5 2 Argumentative 17 Inflexible
Fi 3 Arrogant 18 Insensitive
F3> 3 4 Capable 19 Introverted
F4 5 Commonplace F6, 6 20 Intuitive
F4 6 Conforming Fi 21 Irritable
4 7 Conscientious 22 Kind
2 8 Curious 15 23 Mannerly
4 9 Dependent Fi 24 Masculine
4 10 Efficient F6 25 Nonconforming
1 11 Enduring F4 26 Not artistic
s 12 Energetic 27 Not cultured
F6 13 Feminine F4 28 Not idealistic
F 3> 3 14 Friendly 29 Not popular
F3 15 Generous 30 Original
* The number opposite an item indicates the type assessed by the item.
All items are weighted +1. To score, simply add together all the correct
responses for each type. All items are scored for both sexes unless they are
preceded by an F. Such items (F) are scored for females only.
125
126 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
F2 31 Pessimistic F5 39 Stable
5 32 Pleasure-seeking 5 40 Striving
F2 33 Precise F .s 41 Strong
F6 34 Rebellious 4 42 Suspicious
Fi 35 Reserved F2 43 Thorough
F2 36 Scholarly 1 44 Unassuming
2 37 Slow-moving 6 45 Unconventional
F538 Social
1 Absent-mindedness 1 1
6 Artistic ability 1 1
4 Clerical ability 1 1
4 Conservatism 1 1
3 Cooperativeness 1 1
6 Expressiveness 1 1
5 Leadership 1 1
2 Mathematical ability 1 1
1 Mechanical ability 1 1
6 Originality 1 1
2 Research ability 1 1
2 Scientific ability 1 1
5 Self-confidence (social) 1 1
1 Self-understanding 1 1
3 Understanding of others 1 1
4 Neatness 1 1
APPENDIX B 127
Some-
Very what Little
Essen- Impor- Impor- Impor-
4
1
Becoming happy and content
Inventing or developing a
1100
tial
1100
tant tant tance
3
useful product or device
Helping others who are in
1100
1100
difficulty
1100
2 Becoming an authority on a
my
1100
special subject in field
2
Following a formal religious code
Making
to science
Making
a theoretical contribution
a technical contribution
1100
1100
6
to science
Writing good fiction (poems,
1100
0011
1
4
3
novels, short stories, etc.)
Being well read
Producing a
Contributing to
lot of
human
work
welfare
1100
1100
6
6
Producing good artistic
(painting, sculpture,
decorating, etc.)
Becoming an accomplished musi-
work
1100
1100
5
3
cian (performer or composer)
Becoming an expert
and commerce
Finding a real purpose
in finance
in life
1100
1100
128 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF VOCATIONAL CHOICE
D. From the following list of 12 famous people, check the one
whose life you would most like to emulate.
E. Circle L for those school subjects you like and D for those you
dislike.
L D L D
F6, 6 4 1. Art 1 5. Industrial Arts
F4 2. Business F5 6. Modern History
F2 3. Chemistry 2 7. Physics
Fi 4. General Science F3,3 8. Social Studies
4 Business 1
6 Arts 2
2 Science 3
5 Leadership 4
3 Human relations 5
1 Mechanics 6
3 Mechanics 1
5 Science 2
1 Human relations 3
APPENDIX B 12C)
6 Business 4
2 Leadership 5
4 Arts 6
would you find most frustrating or would make you feel the most
uncomfortable? (Circle one only):
VOCATIONAL GOALS
J.
Complete the following statements as explicitly as you can:
10. List below all the vocations you have ever considered
in thinking about your future. List the vocations you
have daydreamed about as well as those you have talked
to others about. Try to give a history of your tentative
choices and daydreams. Put your present choice on line 1
and work backward to the first vocation you ever con-
sidered.
Vocation At About
What Age?
(Code responses according to
Appendix A)
1.
2.
3. _— _
_
.
4.
5. .
6.
7. __ _
8. _
)
Index
131
) )
132 INDEX
Kuder Preference Record 4, 7, 11, SHELDON, W. H. 10
37, 39, 93 Social environment 56-57
Social type 25-27
LAURENT, H. 5 SPRANGER, E 10
LECKY, P. 44 STEIN, M. I. 73
LEVINSON, D. J. 88 STERN, G. G. 73
LIBO, L. 64 STRONG, E. K JR. 2, 82
LINTON, R. 53 Strong Vocational Interest Blank 4,
6-7,11,37-39,64,89
MAC LEAN, M. S. 43 Subtypes ( see Personality pattern
MIERZWA, J. A. 13 SUPER, d. e. 2
MURRAY, H. A. 52
Theoretical concepts 18-19, 52—54
NACHMANN, B. 5, 90 aptitude 13
background concepts 2—7
Personal development 50, 70, 84-86
definitions 37, 41, 60
Personal Survey 37-38, 89
intelligence 13
Personality pattern (subtypes) 10-
origins 8, 15
11,44-50
sex 13
consistent 44
summary 8-12
definition 10-11, 16, 37-38, 40-
Type see Personality type
(
41
inconsistent 44
VANCE, 44
f. l.
subtypes 40
Vocational achievement 47, 67- 68,
Personality types (models) 9-10,
85-86
16-39, 42-50
Vocational choice 42-44, 73-78
assessment 10-11, 36-39
change in 80-83
change 87-88
direction of 63
descriptions 16-35
interclass change 80-81
dominant type 36
intraclass change 80
homogeneity 76
primary direction of 42
influences 44—47
process of 11-12
interrelationships 39-40
secondary direction of 43
origins 10, 15
stability of 43, 66-67, 80
predispositions 10
Vocational Preference Inventory 4,
PIERSON, r. r. 82
8,11,36-38,39,89
Realistic environment 54-55 VOLSKY, T. C, JR. 44
Realistic type 19-22
roe, a. 5, 89
WARREN, J. R. 82
rosenberg, m. 82 WEISSMAN, M. P. 50
WILENSKY, H. 79
sanford, n. 73 Work history48, 68, 79-80, 85-86
SEGAL, S. I. 5, 90 Work satisfaction 48, 68, 86
ABCDEFGHIJ 7069876
DEAN OF STUDENTS
OFFICE OF THE
GOLDEN WEST COLLEGE
TOPICS IN EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY
The Psychology
of Vocational Choice
JOHN L. HOLLAND
A new theory of vocational behavior. Integrates the burgeoning
literature in the field and outlines practical applications of our cur-
rent knowledge. Examines personality types and environmental
models from a fresh point of view, and avoids the truisms and
cliches that have hitherto marred contributions to the subject. In-
tended for students and professional audiences and as a help to
the intelligent reader.
FORTHCOMING TITLES
Basic Ideas About Statistics As Encountered in the Social Sciences
by Victor E. McGee
Cognitive Development by Charles D. Smock
A Phenomenological Interpretation of the Educative Process by
Donald Snygg
The Psychology of Adolescence by W. Cody Wilson
I N PREPARATI ON
Identification and Development of Creative Talent by James J.
Gallagher
Evaluation and Measurement by Donald Medley
Specification and Measurement of Learning Outcomes by David E.
Payne
Methods of Studying the individual Child by John W. M. Rothney
The Psychology of the Elementary-School Child by Irving Sigel