The Psychology of Interrogative Suggesti
The Psychology of Interrogative Suggesti
Doctor of Philosophy
by
School of Psychology
University of Leicester
December 2009
THESIS ABSTRACT
This thesis uses structural equation modelling to gain an insight into the psychological
investigates why vulnerable interviewees tend towards a negative mindset before and
during interview, which in turn appears to generate the factors that Gudjonsson and
and compliance within the Five-Factor personality model, attachment anxiety and
suggestibility. The key findings are that: (1) answer shifts on the Gudjonsson
Suggestibility Scale (GSS) may sometimes come about through compliance and not
suggestibility. Vulnerable interviewees may not always believe the negative feedback
given by the interviewer and therefore not feel uncertain about their memory.
Uncertainty may not necessarily be a pre-requisite for shifting on the GSS; and (2)
Attachment anxiety and avoidance (as well as trait compliance with respect to answer-
I
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Dr Elizabeth Austin (Chapter 3) and Professor Mark Shevlin (Chapter 4) for their
helpful structural equation modelling advice, during the revision process of these
studies.
The anonymous peer reviewers for their helpful feedback on each of the submitted
manuscripts.
II
TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT .............................................................................................................................. I
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...................................................................................................... II
TABLE OF CONTENTS ..................................................................................................... III
Method ..................................................................................................................................... 35
Results ...................................................................................................................................... 39
Discussion ................................................................................................................................ 42
Limitations and conclusion ...................................................................................................... 46
Discussion ................................................................................................................................ 70
Limitations and conclusion ...................................................................................................... 72
III
CHAPTER 4: LIFE ADVERSITY, NEUROTICISM, COMPLIANCE AND
INTERROGATIVE SUGGESTIBILITY ............................................................................ 75
Method ..................................................................................................................................... 80
Results ...................................................................................................................................... 83
Discussion ................................................................................................................................ 90
IV
CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION
On November 20, 1974, six men were arrested on suspicion of involvement in the
bombing of two public houses by the IRA. The evidence leading to their arrest was a
positive Greise Test (a test which is able to determine whether or not the men had
confessions from four out of the six men. There was also some circumstantial
Another case of wrongful conviction occurred in October 1975 when the Guildford
Four (Paul Hill, Gerry Conlon, Patrick 'Paddy' Armstrong and Carole Richardson)
were sentenced to over 15 years imprisonment for the Provisional Irish Republican
Army's (PIRA) Guildford pub bombing. Again, the court relied almost exclusively on
the confession statements that the four had made during interrogation. At trial the
Guildford Four claimed that they had been tortured into confessing (see Gudjonsson,
2003 for an overview of case). Later the Appeal Court decided that all of which were
(Bull & Milne, 2004; Gudjonsson, 2003). It must be made clear though that this
effectively interviewed (Milne & Bull, 1999, p. 2). What is paramount is the quality
of the investigative interview, as it is this which seems to dictate the extent to which
interviewers adhere to the Police and Criminal Evidence act (1984) and PEACE
1
model of investigative interviewing. The PEACE acronym identifies the steps of the
model, which reflect the interview procedure; these are: Planning and preparation,
Engage and explain, Account, Closure and Evaluate. These Governmental guidelines
Since the introduction of these guidelines the number of cases of wrongful conviction
has decreased within the UK (see appendix I) for a summary of the cases of wrongful
conviction due to suggestibility over the past ten years). The number of cases of
United States, where more persuasive interviewing methods are still legally
acceptable. For example, The Reid Technique has been shown to be the interview
1992; Kassin, 1997). Recent research shows that police induced false confessions
Drizin, Grisso, Gudjonsson, Leo & Redlich, in press). Drizin and Leo (2004)
analysed 125 cases of proven false confessions in US between 1971 and 2002: 81% of
those cases went to trial ended in wrongful convictions. These findings show that
between 1992 and 2002 there were still proven cases of police induced false
confessions in the US (which is in contrast to England and Wales, where they have
(since 1992) become less common [Bull & Milne, 2004; Milne & Bull, 1999]).
In 1992 England and Wales saw the introduction of the Memorandum of Good
Practice [1992] which later became Achieving Best Evidence (Home Office, 2002;
2007). We can see that these guidelines have obviously had a positive effect on the
2
Act of 1984 (PACE), the PEACE model of investigative interviewing, as well the
Governmental guidelines (Home Office, 2002; 2007), seems to have been relatively
effective in achieving its aims to: (i) protect vulnerable/suggestible interviewees from
coercive police interviewing methods; and (ii) instruct Police and practitioners in the
Prior to 1992 the interviewing style was more accusatory (Moston, Stephenson &
designed to obtain a confession from the suspect (Irving, 1980). The interviewee was
passive within the interview, which seemed not to help with the retrieval of accurate
and reliable information. Research also shows that leading questions are present quite
regularly within the interviews (McConville & Hodgson, 1993; Pearse & Gudjonsson,
1996). The pre-1992 interviewing style is quite different from the current style of
interviewing, which seeks to investigate (rather than interrogate) and obtain reliable
The reduction in the use of inappropriate interviewing tactics coincides with the
introduction of PACE (1984). PACE appears to have helped reduce the number of
false and/or unreliable confessions obtained. The emphasis of the “post 1992” style
facilitator (Milne & Bull, 2004; Home Office, 2007). The introduction of audio and
style from being one of “persuading a denying suspect to confess to that of being an
the pre-1992 methods observed within England and Wales). This can be seen within
1
the nine steps of interrogation that Inbau, Reid and Buckley (2001) suggest
interviewers use.
Once suspects have been informed of their constitutional rights, they are then
confessions, with the training manual advocating and encouraging the use of
Inbau et al., 2001, the nine steps for „effective interrogation‟ are as follows:
Step 1- Tell the suspect that there is overwhelming evidence, even from witnesses, of
their guilt. Even though this may be a lie, it is done to coax the suspect
Step 2- Try to shift the blame away from the suspect and on to another person(s),
Steps 3- Never allow the suspect to deny guilt. Allowing the suspect to deny guilt is
Step 4- After the suspect has been prevented from denying guilt, the accused will
often give reasons why he or she did not, or could not, commit the crime. The
Step 5- Make sure that the accused thinks that the investigator is sincere; reinforce
4
Step 6- The suspect will then become quieter and listen. Take advantage of the fact
that s/he has become passive and is on the verge of giving up, and try to move
try to focus the suspect‟s mind on central and specific themes surrounding the
reasons why s/he committed the offence. At this point the interrogator should
show signs of sympathy, understanding, and to urge to suspect to tell the truth.
Some suspects cry at this stage, but this should be reinforced and used to the
it is also a good indication that the accused has given up and is ready to
Step 7- “Alternative questions” are then posed to the suspect; these questions allow
one of two answers about committing the crime, both implicating the suspect
and one which is more socially acceptable than the other. Admittedly, the
suspect will more often choose the easier option, however, whichever they
Step 8- Prompt the suspect into admitting their guilt in front of witnesses.
Step 9- Document the suspect‟s admission, and have them sign a confession.
One can see here clearly the potential for unreliable information being extracted from
the emotional state of the suspect. Maximisation involves the interviewer leading the
Minimisation on the other hand is used with more remorseful suspects; in effect they
5
[the suspects] are lulled into a false sense of security and confession by the
employ other subtle manipulative tactics, such as providing the suspect with face-
saving excuses, thereby minimising the role of the accused in the offence.
Despite the explicit nature of the Reid Technique (and the comparatively more
effective PEACE model), recent evidence (chapter 3; Drake, Egan & Bull, in
submission; and chapter 4 [Drake, 2009]; Jakobsson-Ohrn & Nyberg, 2009) points to
the interviewee‟s perception of the interview, which may be most critical during
conjunction with quality of the interview, dictate the extent to which vulnerable
pressure/coercion (see chapter 3 [Drake et. al., in submission] and chapter 4 [Drake,
some vulnerable suspects may still perceive coercion, or at least not feel like they are
given the chance to tell the truth (Jakobsson-Ohrn & Nyberg, 2009). This because
Proving that a seemingly voluntary confession is, in fact, a product of the suspect
6
whether or not a suspect feels or perceives pressure during an interview. Such
individuals may remain vulnerable during interview – even under the PEACE model.
This may be as a result of such interviewees being sensitive to very subtle (often
(e.g. subtle changes in facial expression, minute changes in tone of voice and/ or
sometimes even the mere presence of the interviewer) (Baxter & Boon, 2000; Baxter,
Boon & Marley, 2006; Baxter, Jackson & Bain, 2003). The latest research infers the
potential for yielding unreliable evidence even in countries such as the UK where
investigative interviews are more tightly monitored. Research into understanding why
been conducted into the nature of these two constructs in an attempt to curb their
frequency (see Baxter, 1990; Bruck & Melnyk, 2004; Gudjonsson, 2003; Kassin,
1997), yet, despite this, false confession and wrongful conviction rates remain an
The 15-20% figure mentioned earlier, cited in Kassin et. al. (in press) paper,
represents those false confessions that are: (i) not disproved before trial; (ii) do not
result in a guilty plea; (iii) those in which DNA evidence is not available; and/or (iv)
those given in minor criminal cases, or from juveniles. Studies in Iceland and
Denmark have revealed that false confessions can occur within the student population
questioned by police (Gudjonsson et. al., 2006; 2008; 2009). This percentage (the 15-
20%) probably represents a minority of cases of false confessions that have come to
7
light over the decades; the tip of the iceberg in terms of the actual number of false
Police induced false confessions seem to be (and remains) an issue around the world.
Understanding why certain interviewees are more likely to make false statements and
The notion that individuals could be suggestible was first noted in the work of James
Cattell (1895), who conducted some of the earliest research on the psychology of
had previously seen a staged event, asking them to provide responses and to also rate
their degree of confidence in their answers (Cattell, 1895). Cattell‟s findings revealed
interest amongst other psychologist at the time that went on to conduct further
behavioural concept that could occur within the wakeful/conscious state (and not just
Inspired by Cattell‟s work, Alfred Binet (1900) replicated Cattell‟s research and
studied the results of other similar psychology experiments that applied to Law and
Criminal Justice. Binet used tests of “prestige” to investigate the apparent malleability
face of another person (i.e. the experimenter or interviewer). The progressive weights
8
and lines test on the other hand was somewhat different, and formed part of a battery
suggestibility measured by hypnotisability, body sway, the Hand Press test, amongst
somewhat mixed (see Eysenck & Furneaux, 1945; Benton & Bandura, 1953).
Eysenck and Furneaux questioned the reliability of the tests used to measure
secondary suggestibility, citing the lack of test-reliability as a possible reason for the
less than convincing evidence (for the secondary suggestibility factor) emerging from
This may well be the case, but the absence of a significant secondary suggestibility
factor in Benton and Bandura‟s research may well have been for other reasons (see
Benton & Bandura, 1953). Eysenck and Furneaux used psycho-neurotic male Army
patients. Those participants may well have been considered vulnerable by today‟s
standards, as a result of their high levels of neuroticism and shell-shock, (they were
soldiers from World War 2) (PACE, 1984; Achieving Best Evidence, 2002; 2007).
Evidence suggests that trait anxiety and neuroticism correlates fairly strongly with
Wolfradt & Meyer, 1998). Eysenck‟s participants may have been naturally more
experiment.
9
There may well be a degree of shared variance between the secondary suggestibility
and the concept of interrogative suggestibility (which will be discussed shortly). Both
neuroticism). This implies that both concepts may share a degree of similarity;
Donnellan, Assad, Robins, & Conger, 2007; Donnellan, Burt, Levendosky & Klump
participants in Eysenck and Furneaux‟s (1945) experiment may well have been more
easily influenced (more vulnerable) and thus more gullible. This could have caused
the secondary suggestibility factor to emerge within their study (but not within Benton
suggestibility in Sweden and found support for the presence of a secondary factor of
10
participants scored high on neuroticism and therefore may have been more likely to
Prior to this time, the concept of suggestibility was primarily used within social and
behavioural responses. It was assumed that every thought could eventually become a
consciousness. Binet‟s experiments (as well as those of Eysenck and Furneaux [1945]
and Benton and Bandura [1953]) generated a paradigm shift: for the first time
Later the paradigm shifted further, demonstrating that suggestible behaviour could be
with pressure. The idea that leading questions could produce distorted recollections
was not what was new. Stern‟s work in 1910 and 1939 was the first to provide
empirical evidence indicating that certain people could come to accept misleading
event – when asked leading questions [by an interviewer]. The notion that external
influence may be required was also not new: What Binet concluded especially was the
importance of several key factors in causing suggestibility during a wakeful state: (i)
the recipient being relatively obedient or open to mental influence, (ii) the recipient
having a tendency to imitate, and (iii) the suggestion somehow paralysing the
recipient‟s critical sense. What was the new idea, however, was the application of the
concept of suggestibility to the forensic setting (in particular the police interview).
The idea emerged that explicit pressure/negative feedback could render an individual
11
more susceptible to misleading information via heightened uncertainty, expectations
Interrogative suggestibility
Gudjonsson, Young & Bramham, 2007). It can manifest during interview in two
ways (Gudjonsson, 1992; 2003): (i) The acceptance of inaccurate information; and (ii)
individuals are more prone to making false statements during questioning, which may
forensic setting.
Across academic and applied forensic settings the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale
suggestibility. The GSS measures a variety of functions: (i) memory recall, (ii)
information and sensitivity to negative feedback. “Yield 1”, “Yield 2” and “Shift” are
the GSS measures most relevant to the current issue in question (details regarding the
scoring of the GSS is presented within the method sections of chapters 2-5). Yield 1
12
Interrogative suggestibility explains differences in eyewitness performance, the
2006).
The Gudjonsson and Clarke 1986 model has been the long-established theoretical
interview. The model supposes that the interviewee (naïve to the GSS task) enters the
interview room with a general cognitive set (or mindset). This is dictated by how they
respond to the novel interview situation, the presence of the interviewer, and may
determine their behavioural response to the GSS interview questions. That is,
several factors are important in eliciting the suggestible response: (i) uncertainty
interviewees may feel that the interviewer expects them to know the correct answer,
(iii) the use of negative feedback during the interview, designed to unnerve the
interviewer and interviewee; this seems to enhance the believability of the negative
feedback, rendering it more penetrative. The Gudjonsson and Clarke model thus
suggestibility during the GSS procedure; that is, how leading questions integrate with
the negative feedback aspect of the model to occasion high suggestibility scores.
Although there has been much research into the individual differences correlates of
13
mechanism governing performance on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (which
This is therefore the focus of the thesis. When questioned on an event previously
memory for events deteriorates over time and is influenced by expectations and
schemas (Loftus & Palmer, 1974; Milne & Bull, 1999). This is especially the case
when given distracter tasks to divert attention and thought from the event/narrative in
question (Fruzzetti, Toland, Teller & Loftus, 1992). When subsequently questioned
inevitable. Similarly, when it comes to expectations of success – feeling that they [the
interviewee] should know the answer; what might be the basis of this [heightened]
Uncertainty or expectations of success are relevant, but I am simply arguing that the
than the Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) model offers. It is important to understand the
allow insight into how such vulnerability might be effectively managed during
interview (see chapter 6). It may help practitioners appreciate the complexity of this
behaviour and understand the reasons why vulnerable interviewees are vulnerable.
individuals.
A second unresolved issue is the extent to which the GSS actually measures
14
considered a coping mechanism during arduous situations and/or interpersonal
conflict (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Shifting on the GSS could be a manifestation (or
suggestibility (at least to an extent). If this is the case then the uncertainty factor may
not always be a pre-requisite for answer shifting in response to negative feedback (see
chapters 4 and 5). Some vulnerable individuals may be certain about what they
remember but shift their answers through a desire to avoid conflict (with the
individuals tend to view the GSS task post negative feedback as more arduous
implemented during interpersonal conflict (Costa & McCrae, 1992); uncertainty may
Compliant interviewees are aware that they are being influenced (Gudjonsson, 1989).
Olafsdottir, 2008). The negative feedback phrase (delivered after the first round of
questions on the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale [GSS]) is: “You have made a
number of errors, it is therefore necessary to go through the questions once more and
this time try to be more accurate” (Gudjonsson, 1997). Interviewees reporting intense
adverse life events, scoring high on “shift”, could be complying with the negative
feedback instruction given by the interviewer after the first round of questions (see
chapter 4; Drake, 2009). Vulnerable interviewees on the GSS may not always be
internalising the negative feedback; they may not necessarily believe that their initial
15
answers are wrong – a prerequisite for uncertainty and suggestibility (Gudjonsson &
Clarke, 1986).
One cannot attach too much significance onto a single study, and there are limitations
to that study as well (see chapter 4). However, recent research does provide some
evidence, albeit tentative at this stage, which implies that the Gudjonsson and Clarke
Yield 1 and Yield 2 subscales of the GSS (i.e. the acceptance of misleading
negative feedback to answers given initially) (see chapters 4 and 6 for further
discussion on this).
The GSS will be used throughout this thesis as it is a reliable and consistent measure
There are two parallel forms of the GSS (the GSS 1; [Gudjonsson, 1984] and the GSS
2; [Gudjonsson, 1987]). The GSS 1 will be used throughout this thesis as research
(Gudjonsson 2003, p. 379). This is critical because, as will be seen throughout the
thesis, the studies presented are skewed in terms of the number of males to females
within the opportunity samples used. The GSS 1 narrative is also more forensically
relevant compared with the GSS 2 narrative (see the GSS manual; Gudjonsson, 1997),
which is why the GSS1 has been used continuously throughout this thesis.
Interrogative compliance.
with requests and obey instructions that they would rather not do, for some
Milgram demonstrated how readily people are willing to obey instructions even at the
experimenter orders the participant to give what the participant believes are painful
between their need to obey an authority figure (the experimenter) and behaviour
patterns learned from childhood onwards: not to harm others. What was surprising
was that many participants continued to give shocks despite pleas for mercy from the
actor, as long as the experimenter kept on ordering them to do so. Those participants
Compliance and/or obedience to authority figures can often be one of the reasons why
innocents make false confessions during questioning (see the Innocence Project
compliance scores for four out of the six men. The men ended up signing written
other suspects scored low on both interrogative suggestibility and compliance, and
Studies have shown that the two concepts tend to be weakly correlated; Richardson &
Kelly, 2004). Suggestibility and compliance are also both related to insecure
attachment (Gudjonsson et. al., 2008). Insecure attachment may lead to a negative
perception of events and situations (Bowlby, 1969; 1988). The experience of intense
adversity seems to lead to stress generation and ineffective coping (Safford et. al.,
interpersonal conflict (Costa & McCrae, 1992) and it could be relevant to explaining
be that both suggestibility and compliance are behaviours that can come to the surface
interview. The two concepts may well be related (studies have shown that the two
concepts tend to be weakly correlated; Richardson & Kelly, 2004), but suggestibility
goes one step further; with suggestibility it appears that the individual experiences a
gradual decline in their ability to trust their memory in the face of uncertainty (and
therefore gradually acquires the tendency to trust others‟ judgments and memories
rather than their own); with compliance, this is not the case; compliance is also a
McCrae, 1992) – but, and this is the difference: there is no memory distrust; the
18
Suggestible behaviour on the surface bears similarity with compliance, but may have
superficially they are similar. This poses a problem for research using the GSS or,
specifically, for inferring suggestibility from high GSS scores; there is no way of
verifying whether interviewees being administered the GSS actually accept the
misleading suggestions (so believe them to be true) and/or believe the negative
feedback they are given, leading to answer shifts. High GSS scores could be a sign of
may come about though, at least in part, due to compliance. This will be investigated
Past research has frequently demonstrated the detrimental impact of life adversity
upon the behaviour and mindset, of an individual (e.g. Essex, Klein, Cho, & Kraemer,
2003; Andrews & Wilding, 2004; Todman & Drysdale, 2004; Becker, 2006).
experiences may also result in negative expectations about their own performance on
future tasks (i.e. having repeatedly performed poorly at interviews, the person may
Weston, 2007). When faced with the somewhat arduous task of having to recall an
event, and subsequently face questioning (i.e. the GSS procedure), interviewees with
19
Clarke, 1986). In order to deal with that uncertainty, and the threat of inadequate
reliance upon the interviewer for guidance as to whether the interviewee has answered
are told by the interviewer that they have „made a number of errors, and therefore it is
necessary to go over the questions once more, and this time try to be more accurate‟.
strategies when faced with the second round of interview questions. In short, this may
the interview.
Interviewees with the experience of negative life-events may also be more likely to
shift their initial answers in response to the negative feedback delivered (after the first
favourable to the interviewer, and to also meet the perceived expectations of the
interviewer. Interviewees reporting more intensely negative life adversities may feel
that the interviewer expects them to know the correct answers (Gudjonsson & Clarke,
1986). Such interviewees may show a propensity towards trying to meet those
perceived expectations.
20
yielded significant findings. Not only that, but negative correlations have also been
found between the experience of life adversity and self-esteem (Peterson & Taylor,
1980; Cohen, Burt, & Bjork, 1987). The Drake et. al., (2008) study, therefore, not
only investigated the influence of life adversity upon interrogative suggestibility, but
it also explored self-esteem levels in relation to both the experience of negative life-
events and performance on the GSS. Self esteem though was not significantly
This study (my BSc dissertation) showed that the reported experience of intense
adverse life events was particularly related to sensitivity to negative feedback; this
experience of major adverse life events and reported false confessions (Gudjonsson,
Sigurdsson & Sigfusdottir, 2008; 2009). These studies demonstrate that the
experience intense adversity which creates a lesser ability to cope with pressure
interview. So why is the reporting of more intensely negative adverse life events
Suggestibility Scale)?
21
Attachment anxiety and avoidance
Bowlby, 1969, 1988) within each individual. The IWM seems to govern behaviour
during dyadic interactions (i.e. how individuals relate and respond to others) and the
Kaplan, & Kassidy, 1985; Quas. Qin, Schaaf, & Goodman, 1997). Within interview
situations, insecurely attached interviewees have been shown to have greater difficulty
accessing and/or coherently describing memories, and resultantly rely upon the
of commission and omission errors, respectively, during interview (Quas et. al., 1997;
(Bowlby, 1969; 1988). This tendency may well lead to a lesser resilience to pressure.
The Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) specify a negative cognitive set as being central to
the suggestible response. Attachment anxiety in particular has been found to regulate
The IWM also governs interpretation of events such that insecurely attached
individuals may interpret events more negatively, and ergo the report more negative
life events. During the GSS interview phase this propensity towards a negative
post negative feedback – encouraging both (a) answer shifting and (b) yielding to
22
critical feedback and meeting with the perceived expectations of the interviewer may
motivate these behaviours further (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986; Gudjonsson, 2003).
Based on this body of research evidence there seems to be a link between attachment
anxiety, the (reported) experience of more impactful life adversity and interrogative
anxiety (i.e. those scoring high on preoccupied anxious attachment) could report more
intense negative life events), which may occasion a greater sensitivity to the negative
feedback.
The legal ramifications of this could be unreliable evidence and possible coerced false
coupled with the experience of life adversity. There is also now increased concern in
some countries regarding the potential vulnerability of witnesses and victims, and the
2007). In such instances witnesses with an insecure attachment style and history of
pressure, and thus may provide less valid information at interview and/or in Court.
Findings from chapter 3 also suggest that it is the interviewee‟s perception of the
investigative interview that matters the most. The findings in chapter three are
23
affecting the reliability of their information. This could largely be because vulnerable
(see Baxter et. al., 2000; 2003; 2006) that, to “non-vulnerable” individuals, seems like
or not feeling like they were given the chance to tell the truth (Jakobsson-Ohrn &
Nyberg, 2009), rather than whether or not the interview actually fell short of the
Could it be possible that any aspect of the interview may have been perceived as
coercive or oppressive?
Although there is evidence to the contrary, suggesting that the experience of trauma
and adversity can have a positive affect (Joseph & Linley, 2008; Linley, Joseph &
Loumidis, 2005), there is also a growing body of research linking childhood and life
in adulthood (Rosenman & Rodgers, 2004; 2006). Individuals who have experienced
viewing subsequent events as negative, and thus experiencing more negative feelings
and/or emotions with regard to subsequent events than individuals who have
20-24, 40-44, and 60-64, regarding their childhood experiences. Experiences such as
24
physical abuse by parents, parental sexual abuse, excessive physical punishments,
parental drug or alcohol abuse, childhood poverty, and financial hardship were
examined. What came to light was that maternal emotional problems and paternal
substance abuse, along with parental conflict, were the most frequent adverse
experiences reported, and that those experiences, specifically, are linked with elevated
finding are extremely interesting as they demonstrate the influential impact of parental
behaviour upon the psychological state of the child (Olvera, Remy, Power, Bellamy,
& Hays, 2001; Taylor, Clayton, & Rowley, 2004; Lee, Beauregard, & Bax, 2005),
thus determining how well that child learns to cope with subsequent adversity. That
is, how vulnerable a person is, or becomes, to later negative life experiences.
Thus, childhood adversities such as these can negatively affect self-esteem and equip
an individual with a negative mindset, anxiety, and depression (Swearing & Cohen,
1985). Furthermore, the literature seems to suggest that individuals with a more
negative mindset tend towards more negative life experiences; in a sense a self-
fulfilling prophecy occurs in that the lower a person‟s self-esteem (and the higher
their level of depression and negative affect), the more likely they are to experience
(Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987). What this research demonstrates is a strong
psychological vulnerability.
The literature points also to personality traits within the Five Factor Model (FFM;
Costa & McCrae, 1992) as additional factors which may encourage ineffective coping
25
leading to interrogative suggestibility. In particular research has implicated high
neuroticism scores on the NEO - FFI in and the experience of negative life events as
the best predictors of depression and helplessness behaviour on tasks (Hill & Kemp-
Wheeler, 1986; Cemalcilar, Canbeyli, & Sunar, 2003). The idea that N could be
relevant to suggestibility has already been seen in the work of Eysenck and Furneaux
(1945) earlier on in this chapter. Individuals scoring high on N are more likely to
believe that success at a task is independent of their own actions (Seligman, 1992),
such that they fail to believe that they could bring about a positive outcome.
The concept of personality comprising five main factors is fairly established (since
Fiske, 1943 and Norman, 1963). Some have argued though against the orthogonality
of the FFM (e.g. Digman, 1997; Egan, Austin, & Deary, 2000). Costa and McCrae
propose that the five factors are independent of each other, but this may not
necessarily be the case. It is suggested that the five factors may in fact be a reflection
1997), since research has shown the five factors to in many cases be related (e.g. E
and N, A and C with low N). One can immediately see a possible degree of overlap
between these two higher-order factors- and the thinking behind Pavlov‟s work on
two main factors and suggests that extraversion and neuroticism are governed by
If indeed the FFM may be reduced to the two factors proposed by Digman, this
provides evidence for both a biological origin of personality but also that excitation-
The idea that the FFM may be reduced further has not though been consistently
26
supported by empirical evidence (Biessanz & West, 2004; Egan, 2009); method-
effects seem to be possible causes of these varied findings. The extent to which the
five factors emerge as independent appears to be dependent upon the quality and
source of data (see Egan, 2009). The further advantage of the FFM is the ability to
explore each domain in more details. Research suggests that N may well be
associated with the reporting of more intensely negative events and suggestibility
(Drake, in press; chapter 4); the five factors allow investigation into which
endogenous tendency towards stress; Costa & McCrae, 1992) is the aspect on N that
The FFM is one of the more dominant contemporary personality theories. It supposes
that personality reflects five core factors (Neuroticism (N), Extroversion (E),
individuals who are prone to psychological distress and may be observed as several
traits: (i) Anxiety, which measures levels of anxiety, (ii) Angry Hostility: denoting a
tendency to experience anger and related states such as frustration and bitterness. (iii)
guilt, sadness, despondency and loneliness, (IV) Self consciousness: shyness or social
anxiety, (v) impulsiveness: which measures a tendency to act on cravings and urges
27
rather than reining them in and delaying gratification, and (vi) Vulnerability: high
Facets within the N and A factors (compliance; chapters 4 and 5) may be of particular
relevant to the way in which individuals cope during dyadic interactions; especially
interview (where individuals are questioned and given negative feedback i.e. the
GSS). The experience of life adversity seems especially associated with neuroticism
(Lee, Beauregard, & Bax, 2005; Olvera, Remy, Power, Bellamy, & Hays, 2001;
Rosenman & Rodgers, 2004; 2006; Taylor, Clayton, & Rowley, 2004). The
Considering the fact that the interviewer is a stranger and the novelty of the GSS task
vulnerability may especially be an issue during the first round of questions. Levels of
trait vulnerability may affect how interviewees cope in the presence of the interviewer
and affect the extent to which interviewees feel able to correct/resist the erroneous
attachment anxiety and avoidance (Donnellan et. al., 2008). Such individuals may
hold a negative mindset, which may give rise to expectations of success (due to
avoidant behaviour (Maner, et. al., 2007). Risk decision making involves making
28
decisions which could either have a negative or positive outcome. Rejecting
attachment (i.e. it depending upon the interviewer‟s reaction) (Levin & Hart, 2003).
This may make the acceptance of misleading information during the first round of
questions more likely Such interviewees may become overly reliant upon the
interviewer and employ ineffective coping methods as a means of (in their minds)
On receipt of negative feedback after the first round of questions, these negative
expectations may be confirmed. Attachment related anxiety and avoidance could lead
to an intense negative perception of and reaction to the negative feedback (as a result
of the established internal working model; (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Burnette, Davis,
Green, Worthington & Bradfield, 2007; 2009). This may affect resilience to the
negative feedback and cause certain attachment behaviours to surface such as:
own ability (Quas, Qin, Shaaf & Goodman, 1997; Bruck & Melnyk, 2004). Research
also shows that both hugh attachment anxiety and avoidance is linked to interpersonal
towards distress. This may be the basis of a negative mindset within vulnerable
as more intensely negative and by levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance), which
may bring about (and be the basis of) the acceptance of misleading information and
29
Objectives of thesis.
(i) Over the course of the next five chapters it will investigate one of the major
attachment anxiety and avoidance (Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1994), and the
that may give rise to false statements during interview (Gudjonsson, 1992).
30
CHAPTER 2:
childhood, adolescence, and adult life, most research attention to-date has focussed
largely upon the role of individual differences in childhood suggestibility (e.g. Baxter,
1990; Bruck & Melnyk, 2004 for reviews of the literature). By contrast, adult
psychologists (see Gudjonsson, 2003 for reviews of the literature), even though the
Gudjonsson, 1984 a, 1991; Kassin, 1997; Santtila, Alkiora, Ekholm, & Niemi, 1999;
In terms of the formal police interview, interrogative suggestibility can be defined as:
“the extent to which, within a closed social interaction, people come to accept
(Gudjonsson, 1992, p.345). This tendency can be assessed using the Gudjonsson
Suggestibility Scale (GSS; Gudjonsson, 1984; 1987; 1997), which measures two types
which suggest certain answers which, although plausible, are actually false), and (ii)
31
Fundamentally, the GSS is designed to identify vulnerable individuals in need of
to negative emotional states (Swearing & Cohen, 1985; Cohen, Burt, & Bjork, 1987;
Rosenman & Rodgers, 2004; 2006), and quite recently interrogative suggestibility
(Drake, Bull, & Boon, 2008). The latter study was the first to uncover a strong
association between the reported experience of negative life events (NLEs) and
participants from the general population None of the participants had any prior
events yielded more readily to the misleading information prior to and post negative
be exercised when transferring these findings using an opportunity sample onto a real
life forensic sample (due to concerns over the external validity that may arise when
considering laboratory – based results (e.g. Goodman, 2006; Wells, Memon, &
Penrod, 2006), this study indicated that interviewees reporting a high number of
negative life events could be more vulnerable to robust police interviewing tactics.
One of the possible explanations for this finding is that those reporting more intense
NLEs may experience (during the administration of the GSS and during police
and elevated expectations of their own success (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986).
Interviewees may believe that the interviewer expects them [the interviewee] to know
32
the correct answer(s) to the questions (even if this is not actually the case). The
pressure from those expectations of success, in unison with the added uncertainty,
Witkin, Dyk, Faterson, Goodenough, and Karp (1962), and Witkin, Oltman, Raskin,
and Karp (1971) asserted that FD (if you are going to propose acronyms, use them)
individuals have a less well defined sense of self identity, and as a result experience a
greater need for reassurance, guidance, and support from those around them. Reliance
upon external referents becomes even more apparent in social interactions with people
in authority or with a person upon whom the FD individual is dependent in any way
(Emmett, Clifford, & Gwyer, 2003). Moreover, several studies (Singh & Gudjonsson,
1992b; Blagrove, Cole-Morgan, & Lambe, 1994) have also found significant
correlations between field-dependence and both (i) GSS yield 1 (the acceptance of
misleading information) and (ii) GSS total suggestibility, with increased receptivity to
social cues (in FD interviewees) being one of the reasons proposed. Hence, it may be
interviewer for guidance when faced with uncertainty (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986),
interrogative suggestibility.
Research evidence also suggests the presence of an Internal Working Model (IWM)
within individuals which develops during childhood and serves to guide and mediate
responses to situations and behaviour of that person as a child and later as an adult
(Bowlby, 1969, 1988; Quas, Qin, Shaaf, & Goodman, 1997; Bruck & Melnyk, 2004;
interpreting events as negative, and leading to more self-reported NLEs. IWMs affect
how easily memories (particularly traumatic and/or negative memories) are retrieved
and coherently described during police interview (Quas et al., 1997), possibly
dictating the extent to which interviewees may rely upon the interviewer for guidance,
support and approval (indicating their level of field-dependence to the context) during
interview (Howard & Hong, 2002). A link between the self-reported experience of
The first aim of this study is to replicate the Drake et. al. (2008) finding, which shows
an association between the reported experience of NLEs and performance on the GSS.
This is will be conducted with a new and different sample of participants. The second
follows: (i) interviewees reporting a high number of NLEs will score significantly
Method
Participants
The sample consisted of 64 participants, 23 males and 41 females (mean age = 26.36
years; standard deviation = 10.64; range = 18 to 63 years) who were recruited through
advertisement, as well as via the experimental participation scheme within the School
of Psychology. All were educated to GCSE/O-Level standard or above (i.e. all had
Instruments
Memory Recall
The GSS memory recall task is presented in the form of a narrative, split into 40
ideas. That is, the story is made up of 40 small instances, occurring in a specific
correctly articulate that instance. The interviewee does not need to recall each
instance in the order with which they are presented in the story. Furthermore, the
words used (by the interviewee) to recall the instances need not be exactly as written
in the narrative. Of fundamental importance is that the concept, that is what occurred
within each instance, is correctly recalled. The maximum score that can be achieved
is 40, which would indicate that the interviewee has correctly recounted everything
35
Interrogative Suggestibility.
The interview phase begins immediately after the delayed-recall testing. The first
feedback. (The five „true questions‟ are not scored for suggestibility). Immediately
after the first round of 20 questions, negative feedback is given by the interviewer.
The interviewee is told “You have made a number of errors, and it is therefore
necessary to go through all of the questions once more and this time try to be more
accurate”. All 20 questions are then repeated, in order to see how readily the
interviewee shifts their initial answers in response to the questions asked. A yield 2
score is also obtained, depicting the number of the 15 misleading questions yielded to
post-negative feedback.
(1) Yield 1. For each of the misleading questions that are answered in the
affirmative the first time round, or in the case of false alternative questions
where one of the alternatives is chosen, one Yield point is obtained. Thus, the
(3) Shift. Changes in response to any of the 20 questions (i.e. including the five
„true questions‟), after their administration the second time, contribute to the
„shift‟ score. Thus, the „shift‟ score can range from 0 to 20.
36
Life Events Questionnaire (LEQ) (Norbeck, 1984)
Sarason, Johnson, and Siegel (1978) of the modification being nine items of particular
relevance to women, such as “Major difficulties with birth control pills or devices”,
“Custody battles with former spouse or partner”, and “Being a victim of a violent rape
or assault”. The items in the LEQ were modified to reduce gender bias. Participants
were required to go through all the events listed, and if they had experienced them at
any point of their life, to circle whether it had been a “good” experience or “bad”
experience. Following that, participants were instructed to rate the extent to which
those events had an effect on their lives at the time. The ratings went for 0 to 3, 0
The questionnaire was originally designed to examine life events experienced over the
past year. However, research has shown the importance of studying life events during
changes that are significant to the individual (Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987).
its basis in childhood, and one strong theory is that this is established through social
interactions and events that occur in childhood, and is moderated by life experiences
The GSS implicitly measures trait suggestibility; in its description as a“… stable
37
which suggests that they may well be rooted in childhood, and affected by subsequent
events.
In order to assess the relationship between life events and interrogative suggestibility,
examining events spanning the entire life of the individual was deemed more
appropriate. As a result, participants were given the instruction to “read through the
events listed, and mark the ones that have occurred throughout your whole life, not
The Group Embedded Figures Test (GEFT; Witkin, Oltman, Raskin, & Karp, 1971)
GEFT can also be administered on an individual basis. The test consists of two test
booklets (each with nine problems), and a practice sheet (with two problems).
Participants are required to locate simple figures (found on the back page of the test
booklets) within their corresponding complex background. Once the simple figure
has been identified, the participant is required to trace the outline of that simple figure
within the complex background. The practice sheet consists of two problems (two
simple figures and two corresponding complex backgrounds), following which the
test booklets are administered (containing nine more difficult items each). The GEFT
is a timed-test, with participants being allowed two minutes to complete the practice
sheet, and five minutes for each test booklet. Scoring is achieved by summing the
simple figures correctly traced, producing a maximum score of 18. High scores
prophecy formula) between the times taken to complete the 9-items in both test-book
38
one and two have been calculated. Results revealed a reliability estimate of .82 for
both males (N=80) and females (N=97) for the time required to complete the task.
Procedure
The participants were recruited for the ostensible purpose of a decision making study,
and delayed-recall phases of the GSS, participants completed the GEFT followed by
the LEQ.
Results
Mean scores
Table 1 presents the means of the GSS scores, NLE and FD/I, all of which fall within
the normal range expected for participants with an average or above IQ (see Witkin,
39
Table 1. Mean (M) and standard deviation scores (SD) for the GSS scores, NLEs, and
FD/I scores.
N=64
M SD
IR 14.3 5.71
DR 12.9 5.74
Correlational data
One of the aims of this study was to re-investigate the previously found novel
relationship between the experience of NLEs and suggestibility scores on the GSS-
specifically, yield 1, yield 2, shift, and total suggestibility. In support of the previous
40
study, NLEs were significantly related to yield 1, yield 2, shift, and total
p<0.01, respectively.
The second objective was to examine scores of FD/I in relation to both the
suggestibility components of the GSS and NLEs. Contrary to our expectations, and
the past literature, FD/I was not significantly correlated with yield 1, yield 2, shift, or
total suggestibility. The correlation between NLEs and FD/I was also non-significant.
NLE scores did, however, correlate significantly with age; r = .385; p< 0.05. Age also
correlated significantly with (i) shift scores; r = .292; p< 0.05, and (ii) total
As the latter two correlations found age to be significant, partial correlation was used
to explore the relationship between (i) age and shift and (ii) age and total
suggestibility, whilst controlling for both cognitive decline (i.e. the total number of
items of the GSS narrative freely recollected as well as accuracy of recall) and the
reported experience of NLEs. The issue in question was whether cognitive decline or
the frequent experience of NLEs had the greater impact upon the age - shift and age -
With regard to cognitive decline, the other control variables included within the model
were: immediate and delayed-free recall (obtained directly from the GSS), and an
output bound measure of memory accuracy (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994; 1996;
Professor Ron Fisher, personal communication; 2007). The role of memory was also
negative NLEs and greater GSS scores. In this study memory ability was defined in
terms of both (a) the total number of items recollections and (b) accuracy of
41
recollection (Koriat & Goldsmith, 1994, 1996). In order to calculate the output-bound
measure of memory accuracy for each interviewee, the total number of items reported
correctly (i.e. with the exact wording as in the original GSS narrative) was divided by
Partial Correlation
When controlling for NLEs alone, the correlations between (i) age and shift and (ii)
age and total suggestibility reduced in strength; r =.074, n=61, p>0.5, and r =.069,
n=61, p>0.5 respectively, implying that the two significant zero- order correlations
between age and shift (r=.292, n=62, p<.05) and age and total suggestibility (r=.366,
With respect to the alternative possibility of memory impairment (and thus cognitive
decline), the control variables were shown to have a marginal effect upon the size of
the original zero-order correlations; r=.292, n=62, p<.05 and r=.366, n=62, p<.05;
partial correlations; r=.226, n=56, p<0.5 and r=.306, n=56, p>0.5. This suggests that
cognitive decline has less of a moderating effect on the relationships between age,
Discussion
The original objectives of this study were to (i) replicate the Drake et. al. (2008)
finding of a significant association between NLE and GSS scores, using a new sample
scores on the GSS and the reported experience of NLEs; field-dependence being a
possible coping mechanism implemented during dyadic interactions (e.g. the GSS
42
both misleading information (i.e. relatively high yield 1 and 2 scores) and
Results seem to replicate the Drake et. al. (2008) finding. They show a highly
significant relationship between NLE scores and GSS suggestibility components (that
is, the yield, shift, and total suggestibility subscales). These data suggest that
1991; Kassin, 1997; Santtila, Alkiora, Ekholm, & Niemi, 1999; Henkel & Coffman,
2004). These findings are also important as there seems to be growing concern in
some countries, (expressed in the Achieving Best Evidence document (Home Office,
part of the general public) and, the need for them to be formally assessed (e.g. using
Bystander witnesses, with a reported history of intense life adversity could be more
suggestible during interview. In such cases, the present findings would seem
especially relevant.
A reason for this finding could be that the experience of heightened uncertainty and
NLEs could also harbour negative performance expectations, due to past experiences
sensitivity to negative feedback. The latter may manifest itself in the avoidance of
43
further critical feedback, and in turn elevated GSS shift scores (McCall & Struthers,
and perhaps even the interview tactics employed within the GSS itself (Gudjonsson,
Kopelman, & MacKeith, 1999); this resultant low judgement confidence could
This study also shows that NLEs impact more substantially upon vulnerability to
scores). It could be that the negative feedback (incorporated into the GSS) mimics the
and in the introduction, interviewees with higher NLEs may come to expect to
perform inadequately (ever more so the more NLEs experienced, Gudjonsson &
Clarke, 1986). When met with negative feedback after the first round of GSS-
Subsidiary findings to emerge from this study are the correlations between (i) age and
shift scores and (ii) age and total suggestibility. As age increases, the (reported)
experience of more intense NLEs increases, which intensifies the negative mindset
(the GSS negative feedback) may result. Safford, Alloy, Abramson, and Crossfield,
(2007) have shown that the experience of intense adversity tends to bring about a
negative cognitive set. This underlying negative mindset has been found to predict
negative life events and stress generation. Stress tends to bring about ineffective
44
coping in the face of negative feedback (and a lesser resilience to it) (Gudjonsson,
1995).
What is also important to note, and has not previously been demonstrated, is that
when the reporting of more intensely negative NLE scores are partialled out of the
association, the correlations between age and shift and age and total suggestibility
contributing factor to these relationships than poor memory (in terms of both the
number of items recollected and the accuracy of recollection). In fact, controlling for
memory impairment had a marginal effect upon the size of the original correlations
between (i) age and shift and (ii) age and total suggestibility. These findings suggest
that, irrespective of memory impairment and cognitive decline occurring with age (see
Dumas & Hartman, 2003; Fleischman, Wilson, Gabrieli, Bienias, & Bennett, 2004),
such that, over time, through the accumulation of NLEs, interviewees become ever
number of NLEs, may be even more vulnerable during investigative interviews than
younger adults.
speaking, the reason for this finding could lie within Attachment Theory (Bowlby,
1969; 1988). Insecurely attached individuals, through their internal working model
(IWM), are more prone to interpreting events negatively (thus self-reporting more
NLEs). Such individuals, however, fall into two main categories of attachment style;
45
those exhibiting attachment avoidance are characteristically more field-dependent,
(Vermigli & Toni, 2004). These two attachment groups occupy both ends of the
linear correlation between (the reported experience of) NLEs and FD/I would be
participants, which may also explain why past research has yielded inconsistent
results with respect to FD/I and GSS scores (see Gudjonsson, 2003, for a review of
the findings). It could well be that attachment anxiety may be relevant in the
relationship between the reporting of more intense NLEs and GSS scores. This will
be explored in chapter 3.
A point for argument could though be the direction of the relationship between the
for sense. So far the interpretation of these findings has been through the Gudjonsson
and Clarke (1986) model of interrogative suggestibility; more intensely negative NLE
negative feedback. However, correlation does not imply causation, and it could well
be that individuals who are more suggestible report more intensely negative NLEs.
Garry, Manning, Loftus and Sherman (1996) found that the act of imagining events
increased the feelings that they had actually occurred in the past. This effect seemed
to become even more prominent when the events that participants were asked to
46
imagine were plausible (Mazzoni, Loftus & Kirsh, 2001). The therapy often used to
retrieve such memories tends to generate many suggestions and images. In the
current study participants were not asked to imagine past negative events; thus, no
suggestion or imagery was used. The participants were simply asked to go through a
list of events and to rate the impact those experiences had (if they had experienced the
events). They were not instructed to imagine that they had experienced a particular
situation or experience, and then to recall whether or not they had actually
Interrogative suggestibility is defined as: “the extent to which, during a closed social
(Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986, p. 84). Interrogative suggestibility and the tendency to
produce false memories seem to be correlated (Brown, 1995). However, some studies
(1997) found similar results with adult female psychiatric patients. The studies so far
show differing results, but a key finding is that false memories and interrogative
to the controversial field of recovered memories of child sexual abuse; “false beliefs
The idea that individuals who are more suggestible may have a tendency to produce
false memories and therefore report more intense NLEs is not supported by the
47
Clarke model of interrogative suggestibility would interpret the findings of this first
study thus: individuals reporting more intensely negative NLEs may be more inclined
questioning and pressure. This may encourage ineffective coping during GSS
The current data does show correlations suggesting this, but the theory would imply
causation from the reporting of more intense NLE to interrogative suggestibility, and
Sigurdsson and Sigfusdottir (2008; 2009) has recently demonstrated a link between
the reporting of intense adverse life events and reported false confessions. They argue
the importance of victimisation and intense adverse life events in giving a false
confession during police interview “because they [are] likely to have insecure
This current study and the earlier Drake et. al. (2008) study both found the impact of
similar types of adverse life events to be relevant. The types of adverse events
assessed within the LEQ (Norbeck, 1984) are those to do with: (i) work – i.e.
bullying, failing exams, iii) love and relationships – break ups, divorce (parental
divorce); iv) family and close friends (i.e. death/major illness of a loved one); v)
personal and social events – such as a decline is social activity, and vi) being a victim
of crime. Gudjonsson et. al. (2008) has also shown similar intense adverse life events
48
The direction of the correlation between iNLE and interrogative suggestibility needs
data by the use of structural equation modelling. The role of attachment anxiety in the
relationship between the reporting of more intense NLEs and sensitivity to pressure
NLEs have again been found to be particularly vulnerable, not only to the GSS's 15
misleading questions, but also to the negative feedback incorporated into the task.
NLEs may be more linked with sensitivity to interrogative pressure than the tendency
investigation but whether these findings do extend to real life suspects is worthy of
investigating .
Evidence” (2007), these findings are particularly noteworthy, since the document not
mental illness) but broadens to the evidential vulnerability that may be caused by what
constitute part of the “normal” general public) with a history of NLEs could be more
chapter 1 (Drake et. al., 2008) may have possibly identified the presence of a new
49
group of vulnerable interviewees. Given Gudjonsson et. al's. (2008) findings, the role
50
CHAPTER 3:
ATTACHMENT ANXIETY, LIFE ADVERSITY AND SENSITIVITY TO
INTERROGATIVE PRESSURE
A link between the reporting of intense negative life events (NLEs) and interrogative
suggestibility on the GSS has been repeatedly observed (Drake, Bull & Boon, 2008;
Drake & Bull, in press). Drake et. al. (2008) hypothesised that self esteem may
mediate the relationship, though this construct did not appear to be significantly
related to either NLEs or GSS scores. Reporting more intensely negative NLEs
of intense NLEs and yield 2 and shift scores also remain when memory recall
accuracy ([chapter 1] Drake et. al., 2008) is considered. Further research into why
the reporting of more intensely negative NLEs may encourage a lesser resilience to
interrogative pressure (leading to elevated GSS scores post negative feedback) seems
needed.
Stephenson, & Williamson, 1992; Ofshe & Leo, 1997; Pearse & Gudjonsson, 1999).
Baxter and Boon (2000), Baxter, Boon and Marley (2006), and Baxter, Jackson and
Bain (2003) have further demonstrated the role of interviewer influence and
interviewer demeanour may encourage relatively high scores on the GSS, through
51
inducing uncertainty and expectations of success (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). How
interviewees perceive and interact with the interviewer may affect interviewee
cognitive mindset during interview and influence their GSS performance. This may
especially be the case once negative feedback has been given by the interviewer.
Larsen & Bunce, 1996; Cyranowski, Bookwala, Houck, Pilkonis, Kostelnik, &Frank,
2002; Feldman Barrett, 1997; Simpson, Rholes & Phillips, 1996). This negative
mindset may be indicated by such individuals reporting their NLEs as more intensely
negative and through higher levels of attachment anxiety and avoidance. A negative
mindset could then result in a greater sensitivity to negative feedback on the GSS.
preoccupied anxious attachment and fearful avoidant attachment; see Bartholomew &
Horowitz, 1991; Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994) tend to under-report the intensity of
Kerns, 2006) and overestimate the intensity of previously experienced negative moods
(Cutler et. al., 1996; Feldman Barrett, 1997). Attachment anxiety seems to relate to
et. al., 1996). This negative/pessimistic mindset may be observed within attachment-
avoidant attachment scores) and through such individuals reporting more intensely
negative NLEs.
52
During the GSS interview, in response to negative feedback, this negative/pessimistic
mindset may lead to a more negative interpretation of the negative feedback, elevated
levels of uncertainty and expectations of success (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986), and a
greater sensitivity or lesser resilience to the negative feedback. This may be measured
No research has yet examined the relationship between adult romantic insecure
attachment style, the reporting of more intensely negative NLEs, and sensitivity to
negative feedback on the GSS. This is therefore the focus of the current study and
chapter.
Research suggests that the reported experience of intense negative life events,
attachment anxiety and GSS scores may co-vary, but exactly how the variables relate
to each other is not clear. Is the relationship between attachment anxiety and GSS
life events (i.e. does intense adverse life events moderate the relationship between
attachment anxiety and GSS scores)? Or is the experience of intense negative events a
mechanism through which attachment anxiety may influence GSS scores (i.e. is the
may not always score high on the GSS; whether they do score high on the GSS
depends on their experience of negative events. If the latter is true, and the (reported)
experience of more intense negative events is a mediator, attachment anxiety may lead
to the experience of more intense negative events, which in turn would cause
relatively high GSS scores. It is important to test for both mediation and moderation
53
as then the exact role of the reported experience of more intense NLEs may be then be
ascertained.
The extent to which iNLE acts as a mediator will be explored using pathway analysis
(PA). PA is a good way of getting a general overview of how the variables (of
has several advantages over correlation and partial correlation analyses; one of which
is that it allows the researcher to investigate the relationship between more than two
variables. With partial correlation you can only investigate the relationship between
difficulties (Cyranowski, et. al., 2002). Attachment anxiety is related to: (a) the
previously experienced negative moods (DeWitte & De Houwer, 2008; Gentzler &
Kerns, 2006). Attachment anxiety may lead to the reported experience of more
intensely negative negative life events (iNLEs). iNLEs has previously been found to
correlate significantly with interview suggestibility ([chapter 1]; Drake, et. al., 2008;
more prone towards conflict avoidance during tasks so wish to avoid potential
negativity (Muller, 2009). This is because attachment behaviour has, as a main aim,
54
the interviewer-interviewee relationship but, as a result, may encourage sensitivity to
Hypotheses:
Hypothesis 1
(preoccupied anxious attachment [PAA], fearful avoidant attachment [FAA] and the
reporting of negative life events as more intensely negative [NLE]) may reflect a
1988). The reporting of more intensely negative NLEs may also reflect this process.
PAA, FAA, and the reporting of NLEs as more intensely negative are hypothesised to
relate positively to APP; high levels of APP are expected to be measured by high
Factor analytic evidence (Gudjonsson, 1992) shows that yield 2 and shift may
hypothesised to relate positively to yield 2 and shift scores; high levels of SNF are
Thus, the exogenous latent variable APP may exert a significant positive and direct
The two-factor model will be tested and compared with a competing one-factor
model, where PAA, iNLE, FAA, Yield 2 and Shift are indicators of a single factor
(APP).
55
Hypothesis 2:
The role of iNLE as a mediator in the relationship between PAA and FAA
Two blended models are hypothesised – one explaining Yield 2 and one explaining
Shift scores on the GSS. PAA and FAA are the independent variable, iNLE is the
mediator, and Yield 2 and Shift scores are the dependent variables.
It is expected that PAA and FAA will be correlated. They are expected to exert
positive indirect as well as direct effects on Yield 2 and Shift scores; the indirect
The two blended models will be tested and compared with both a full-mediation
model (where PAA and FAA only exerts an indirect effect through iNLE on both
Yield 2 and Shift) and a no-mediation model (where PAA and FAA and iNLE exert
Method.
Participants.
The sample consisted of 130 participants, 100 females and 30 males (mean age =
19.35 years, standard deviation = 1.41, range = 18 to 26). Participants were recruited
through the experimental participation scheme within the School of Psychology, and
56
Instruments
Memory Recall
The GSS memory recall task is presented in the form of a narrative, which is made up
„successfully recalled‟ if the interviewee is able to freely recall that instance. The
interviewee does not need to recall each instance in the order with which they are
presented in the story. Furthermore, the words used (by the interviewee) to recall the
is that the concept, that is what occurred within each instance, is correctly recalled.
The maximum free-recall score that can be achieved is 40, which would indicate that
the interviewee has correctly recounted everything that occurred in the story. In the
traditional form of the GSS the “immediate” free-recall phase is followed (after filler
Interrogative Suggestibility.
The questioning phase traditionally begins immediately after the delayed free-recall.
In the present study the delayed free recall phase was omitted due to: (i) the filler task
taking much less than 50 minutes to complete, providing an inadequate time interval
between immediate recall and the conventional delayed recall phase (Gudjonsson,
1997) and (ii) more recent studies having shown the delayed free recall phase being
an unnecessary part of the procedure; with little impact upon overall performance (in
of the procedure, was an additional motivating factor in the decision to exclude the
score, which indicates the number of misleading questions yielded to prior to negative
feedback. (The answers to five „true questions‟ do not contribute to this score).
Immediately after the first round of 20 questions, negative feedback is given by the
interviewer. The interviewee is told “You have made a number of errors, and it is
therefore necessary to go through all of the questions once more and this time try to
be more accurate”. All 20 questions are then repeated, in order to see how readily
interviewees shift their initial (20) answers as a result of the critical feedback and
feedback.
The LEQ contains 82 items in total and is a modification of the instrument developed
by Sarason, Johnson, and Siegel (1978), in that it has nine items of particular
relevance to women. These include items such as “Major difficulties with birth
control pills or devices”. The nine additional items in the LEQ were introduced to
reduce the gender bias in the Sarason et. al. (1978) version. Participants were
required to go through all the events listed, and if they had experienced them at any
point of their life, to circle whether it had been a “good” experience or “bad”
experience. They were then instructed to rate the extent to which those events had an
effect on their lives at the time. The ratings went from 0 (“no effect”) to 3 (“large
effect”). The LEQ has good test-retest reliability, with test-retest reliabilities of 0.78
psychiatric symptoms.
58
The questionnaire was originally designed to examine life events experienced over the
past year. However, research has shown the importance of studying life events during
changes (Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987). To assess the relationship between life
events and IS, it was deemed more appropriate to examine events spanning the entire
life of the individual. Participants were instructed to “read through the events listed,
and mark the ones that have occurred throughout your whole life, not just the past
year”.
The RSQ contains 30 items taken from Hazan and Shaver‟s (1987) attachment
Collins and Read‟s (1990) Adult Attachment Scale. For each item on the RSQ,
participants have to rate on a five point scale the extent to which each statement best
describes their behaviour in close relationships. Out of the 30 items, five contribute to
experience a high level of both attachment anxiety and avoidance), and four to
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991; Brennan, Clark & Shaver, 1998). Each participant
obtains scores for each of the four attachment patterns; the scores are derived by
taking the mean of the four or five items representing each attachment style.
The RSQ shows high internal reliability (α = .83) as well as high test-retest reliability
59
Procedure
immediate free-recall and questioning phases, each participant completed the LEQ
Results.
Descriptive Statistics.
Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations of the GSS, attachment, and NLEs
scores. The GSS scores fall within the normal range expected for participants with an
average or above IQ (see Gudjonsson, 1997). Results indicate normality for all of the
attachment measures. NLEs scores, however, show positive skewness (NLEs; skew Z
= 1.74, and kurtosis Z = 5.78). The NLEs data was transformed by mathematically
modifying the scores (Pallant, 2007, p. 88). This improved the skewness and kurtosis
60
Table 1. Means (M) and standard deviation (SD) scores amongst observed variables.
M SD
1. Y1 4.45 2.32
2. Y2 5.55 2.81
3. S 3.74 2.56
Note: N = 130. Y1 = Yield 1, Y2 = Yield 2, and S = Shift; three subscales from the
GSS1. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; one subscale from the LEQ. PAA
Correlational analyses.
NLEs scores and all three GSS subscales (i.e. Y1; r = 0.265, p < .01, Y2; r = .265, p <
.01, and S; r = .364, p < .01). Preoccupied anxious attachment scores are significantly
related to NLEs reported; r = .205, p < .05, but not any of the GSS subscales (i.e. Y1:
r = -.063, p > .05, Y2: .052, p > .05, and S: r = .106, p > .05). Fearful avoidant
attachment scores are not significantly correlated with either NLEs (r = .142, p > .05)
or any of the three GSS subscales (i.e. Y1: r = .018, p > .05, Y2: .076, p > .05, and S: r
61
= .142, p > .05). Fearful avoidant attachment and preoccupied anxious attachment
The hypothesised two-factor model was tested and compared with a competing one-
factor model (see figure 1 and table 2). The extent to which iNLE may mediate the
relationship between PAA and FAA and Yield 2 and Shift scores was also tested.
In the latent models, one a priori assumption was made; the pathway relating shift
scores to the factor SNF was fixed at 1. This assumption was made on the theoretical
grounds that shift scores are a consistent reliable indicator of sensitivity to negative
feedback on the GSS (see Gudjonsson, 1992; Gudjonsson, 2003). The pathway was
fixed to limit the number of pathways in the model due to sample size vs. path
To allow for deviations from normality, the asymptotically distribution free estimation
criterion was used to estimate the model fit (of the latent and path models). Three
goodness of fit indices were used to evaluate model fit (Quintana & Maxwell, 1999;
Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell & Abraham, 2004): (i) the comparative fit index (CFI;
where values of 0.9 or above are required), (ii) the goodness of fit index (GFI; values
of 0.9 are desirable), and (iii) the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA;
values of .06 or less are wanted). A chi-square difference test (Δχ2) was used, as well
as standard fit indices, to compare the nested models with blended models (BM).
Nested models can be derived from another (i.e. a blended model, which contains
both indirect and direct effects) through restricting parameters. Standard fit indices
62
In order to obtain a degree of freedom (df) within the BMs (considering that, with
only two IVs, the df of the BM is zero) the least significant pathway has been
removed from each of the BMs. This is done to allow for subsequent model
comparison of nested models with the BM) is not possible, as there is no test for the
BM fit.
Table 2 shows the standardised parameter estimates and fit-indices for the two
to the data; each indicator explains a significant proportion of the variance within the
factors. The exogenous factor APP exerts a significant positive direct effect upon the
endogenous factor SNF (β = .820, p < .001). With six predictors and a desired
statistical power level of 0.8, the minimum number of participants is 134 (Cohen,
Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). Model 1 has sufficient power considering the sample
size of 140.
Table 2 also shows that the one-factor model (figure 1; model 2) also shows an
acceptable fit to the data. The standardised pathway coefficients of the measured
variables onto the latent factors are statistically significant. The indicators, apart from
yield 2, explain the same significant amount of variance in the two-factor model as
well as the one-factor model. The indicators are positively related to APP.
Theory however (see Bowlby, 1969; 1988; Gudjonsson, 1984; 1992; 2003) would
63
Table 3 provides the standardised beta values for the blended models as well as the
FMM and NMM models. The Yield 2 and Shift subscales will be discussed
separately:
Yield 2.
Table 3 shows that the FMM demonstrates the best fit to the data (see Figure 2)
(compared with BM and NMM; although the BM also shows acceptable fit statistics).
The FMM (and BM) shows that PAA and FAA are significantly correlated, in the
positive direction. PAA exerts a significant indirect effect on Yield 2, through iNLE
– the mediator. FAA does not affect iNLE significantly. Δχ2 tests show that
compared with the BM, the FMM does not provide a significantly better fit: FMM vs.
Shift:
Table 3 shows that the BM demonstrates the best fit to the data (see Figure 2)
(compared with FMM and NMM; although the FMM also shows acceptable fit
statistics). The BM (and FMM) show (and with Yield 2) that PAA and FAA are
effect on Yield 2, through iNLE – the mediator. FAA does not affect iNLE
significantly. Δχ2 tests show that compared with the BM, the FMM does not provide a
significantly better fit: FMM vs. BM: Δχ2 [1] = .913; p > .05.
64
Figure 1. A two-factor and one-factor model. Note: N = 130. Y2 = Yield 2, and S =
Shift. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events. PAA = Preoccupied-Anxious
Attachment and FAA = Fearful-Avoidant Attachment. APP = Anxious-Pessimistic
Perception. SNF = Sensitivity to Negative Feedback.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
e e e e e
1 1 1 1 1
.33*** .24**
.85** .45** 1
e
.82**
APP SNF
e e e e e
1 1 1 1 1
.24** .85**
.45**
.33** 1
APP
65
Table 2. Standardised parameter estimates and fit indices for the measurement
models
Y2 .55** .45**
S 1 1
χ2 3.022 3.022
df 4 4
66
Figure 2. Path diagrams to show the relationship between attachment anxiety,
reporting intense NLEs, and sensitivity to negative feedback on the GSS. Note: N =
130. Y2 = Yield 2, and S = Shift. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events. PAA =
Preoccupied-Anxious Attachment and FAA = Fearful-Avoidant Attachment.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
PAA
.255* e
e
1 1
.185* .381***
iNLE Y2
.116
FAA
Shift: The BM
PAA
.255* e
e
1 1
.192* .381***
iNLE S
.114 .081
FAA
67
Table 3. Standard beta values and fit indices for path-diagrams
Yield 2 Shift
df 1 2 2 1 2 2
Note: N = 130. Empty cells = pathways not included in the model. BM = Blended
Model. FMM = Full Mediation Model. NMM = No Mediation Model. Y2 = Yield 2,
and S = Shift. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events. PAA = Preoccupied-
Anxious Attachment and FAA = Fearful-Avoidant Attachment.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001.
68
Hierarchical Multiple Regression (HMR).
Since PAA and iNLE are the stronger of the measures (compared with FAA), in
relation to both Yield 2 and Shift scores, a HMR was conducted to test for a two-way
interaction effect of PAA x NLE on shift and yield 2 scores. Two separate analyses
were conducted – one for Yield 2 (HMR model 1) and one for Shift (HMR model 2).
Variables were entered into the regression models in the following order: (i) NLEs,
(ii) PAA, and (iii) the interaction term for NLEs and PAA, computed as the product of
scores of NLEs and PAA (i.e. NLE x PAA). Preliminary analyses were conducted to
homoscedasticity.
HMR model 1.
NLEs was entered into Step 1, explaining 14.4% of the variance in yield 2 scores F (1,
128) = 7.09, p < .001. After entering PAA at Step 2, the total variance explained by
the model as a whole was 14.4%; F (2, 127) = 10.7, p < .001; R squared change =
.000; F change (1, 127) = .092, p > .05. The interaction of NLEs with PAA explained
an additional 0% of the variance; F change (1, 126) = .009, p > .05. Within the model
scores (β = .386, p < .001). PAA was not a significant independent contributor (β =
.026, p >.05). The interaction of NLEs with PAA did not make significant
HMR model 2.
NLEs was entered into Step 1, explaining 22.0% of the variance in shift scores F (1,
128) = 36.04, p < .001. After entering PAA at Step 2, the total variance explained by
the model as a whole was 22.0%; F (2, 127) = 17.92, p < .001; R squared change =
69
.000; F change (1, 127) = .063, p > .05. The interaction of NLEs with PAA explained
none of the additional variance: R squared change = .000; F change (1, 126 = .023, p
> .05. Within the model only NLEs made a significant independent contribution to
the variance in shift scores (β = .414, p < .01). PAA was not a significant independent
contributor (β = .003, p >.05). The interaction of NLEs with PAA did not make a
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the relationship between adult romantic
attachment anxiety, the reporting of more intensely negative NLEs, and sensitivity to
The current findings show that the reporting of more intensely negative NLE
correlates significantly with both yield 2 and shift scores on the GSS. This is in
accordance with previous studies (Drake et. al., 2008; in press), and provides further
evidence suggesting that yield 2 and shift scores may be governed by the extent to
negative perception of events and situations may manifest in the reporting of NLE as
anxiety alone; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1994) correlates with the reporting of more
intensely negative NLEs, which is in-line with what was hypothesised. Individuals
scoring high in attachment anxiety tend towards a more pessimistic and negative
perception of events (and are therefore more likely to report [negative] events as more
intensely negative).
The hypothesised two-factor model shows an acceptable fit to the data. It illustrates
that: (i) PAA, FAA and NLEs load significantly onto the factor “anxious-pessimistic
70
perception” and (ii) the latent exogenous factor; “anxious - pessimistic perception”
exerts a significant and positive direct effect on the endogenous latent factor
negative negative life events) that may lead to sensitivity to negative feedback on the
Negative feedback seems to enhance state anxiety and the perceived difficulty of the
interview (McGroarty & Baxter, 2007; 2009). Individuals scoring high on attachment
anxiety (which is related to the reporting of more intensely negative NLEs) may
interpret the negative feedback more negatively and perceive the task of answering
Clarke, 1986), and a lesser resilience to the negative feedback. This is observed by
answer shifting and the acceptance of misleading information in response to the GSS
negative feedback.
(Burnette et. al., 2007; 2009; Fraley & Shaver, 2000). (Anxious-pessimistic)
anxious individuals (Bowlby, 1969; 1988). Attachment theorists have long asserted
that the IWM is a principle factor, affecting perception of situations and subsequent
(measured by FAA and PAA) and sensitivity to negative feedback on the GSS
(measured by Yield 2 and Shift) was also investigated. Findings show that both the
71
FMM and BM provide the best account of both Yield 2 and Shift scores (see table 2
and figure 2). iNLE seems to be a (partial) mediator between attachment anxiety and
sensitivity to negative feedback on the GSS. High degree of attachment anxiety may
may give rise to the reporting of more intense negative live events (shown by
reporting iNLEs).
During interview such interviewees may be more prone to viewing the task of
answers) as relatively arduous (McGroarty & Baxter, 2007; 2009). This may be due
(Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986) (which may ultimately stem from their insecure
attachment tendencies). They may also feel less able to trust their memory as a result
(Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 197). To cope with the negative feedback, their resultant
uncertainty and distress levels, vulnerable interviewees may become more accepting
Due to the limited number of participants in the sample relative to the number of
FAA are latent variables with multiple measures of each latent (the measures being
the corresponding [questionnaire] item indicators) was not estimated (see Magnus et.
al., 1993). The current SEMs shown in figure 1 assume therefore that PAA, iNLE and
FAA are perfectly reliable measures of the latent constructs PAA, iNLE and FAA. As
a result of this, the parameter estimates in both the latent and path models should be
72
taken as conservative estimates. This is because they are not corrected for
The correlation between iNLE and GSS scores in this study also seems lower in
comparison to previous studies within this thesis (Drake, Bull & Boon, 2008
[chapter1]; Drake & Bull, in press [chapter 2]). Reasons for this could be one of
experience a high degree of intense adversity yet who are cognitively hardy do not
necessarily become vulnerable; in contrast those who experience intense adversity but
who are relatively low on cognitive hardiness tend to be more vulnerable. The sample
size of each study has also been relatively small. This variation in relationship
different opportunity samples. It may well be that this current sample is relatively
more cognitively hardy that the previous two, which could explain why the
relationship between iNLE and Yield 2 and Shift scores is weaker in this chapter.
The findings demonstrate the role of both adult romantic attachment anxiety, and the
negative feedback on the GSS. The findings imply that such interviewees could be
pressure. Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson and Sigfusdottir (2008; 2009) have shown a link
between the reported experience of major adverse life events and reported false
confessions. False confessions may come about through such individuals being
73
Baxter et. al. (2000; 2003; 2006) has shown the adverse effect of a negative
reporting more intensely negative NLEs, may be more susceptible to any perceived
the Yield 1 subscale of the GSS. Previous research (Drake, Bull & Boon, 2008
[chapter 1]; Drake & Bull, in press [chapter 2]) has also failed to adequately explain
74
CHAPTER 4:
INTERROGATIVE SUGGESTIBILITY: LIFE ADVERISTY, NEUROTICISM AND
COMPLIANCE.
Chapter 3 demonstrates the importance of adult romantic attachment anxiety and the
reporting of intense NLE in governing sensitivity to interview pressure (i.e. the Yield
2 and Shift subscales of the GSS). The psychological mechanism underpinning the
yield 1 dimension of the GSS seems less adequately explained by attachment and the
experience of adverse life events alone. Previous studies (in chapters 1-3) have also
focussed on the impact measure of negative life events; that is, the relationship
suggestibility.
This study, however, investigates the relationship between both the number and
sensitivity to interrogative pressure (see chapter 3). They suggest that answer-shifting
on the GSS may result from a negative mindset within interviewees, a desire to
interviewees reporting iNLEs, could also result from compliance with interviewer-
75
Introduction
2007). In the light of concern over securing reliable convictions and protecting
important.
Factor analytic evidence suggests two types of interrogative suggestibility: (i) The
of the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale (GSS; Gudjonsson, 1984; 1987) and (ii)
life events (iNLEs) and interrogative suggestibility (Drake, Bull & Boon, 2008; Drake
& Bull, in press). Reporting iNLEs was found to be particularly linked with
sensitivity to negative feedback. When controlling for memory recall accuracy (see
Drake, et. al., 2008), the significant correlations between iNLEs and the three
subscales of the GSS remain. Further research investigating the link between NLEs
and interrogative suggestibility therefore seems warranted. What is also unclear about
the Drake et. al. (2008; in press) studies is whether the relationship between the
reporting of iNLE and relatively high GSS scores may be in fact attributed to trait
compliance (especially in response to the negative feedback incorporated into the GSS
76
Trait compliance has been demonstrated across situations (Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson,
Abramson, & Crossfield, 2007). Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) recognise the
underlying negative mindset has been found to predict negative life events and stress
& McCrae, 1992). Evidence suggests further that individuals tending towards a
negative mindset can sometimes be more prone to experiencing more frequent NLEs
and vice versa (due to a self-fulfilling prophecy and depending on their level of
cognitive hardiness; Beasley, Thompson & Davidson, 2003; Cohen, Cohen & Bjork,
1987). Both the reporting of iNLEs and nNLEs may therefore be related and lead to
compliant behaviour.
Interviewees who display higher levels of trait compliance can also be more
suggestible (Richardson & Kelly, 2004). Compliance could therefore manifest during
the GSS interview and mediate the relationship between the reported experience of
NLEs (frequency and intensity) and GSS scores. The first objective is to investigate
suggestibility.
The role of neuroticism (N) in the relationship between nNLEs and iNLEs,
compliance, and GSS scores will also be explored: Evidence relates N to the
experience of more frequent NLEs (Magnus, Diener, Fujita & Pavot, 1993). This may
77
be because individuals high in N conduct their lives in such a way as to encourage
interpersonal stressors (Elander, French & West, 1993). N appears to relate to stress
generation and the experience of more NLEs, which seems to be the precipitant of a
negative cognitive set (NCS) within such individuals (Safford, et. al., 2007). This
and reporting frequent NLEs). Research shows that N is also linked with a decreased
tendency towards risk-taking behaviour (Maner et al., 2007). This may lead to
N also reflects a susceptibility to distress (Costa & McCrae, 1992). This enhanced
negative experiences (Fraley & Shaver, 2000; Noftle & Shaver, 2006). Such
may be related to the reporting of more iNLEs (as well as nNLE), which may reflect a
NCS within such interviewees. During interview, this NCS may lead to high levels of
ineffective coping (e.g. compliance). These factors may lead to higher Yield 1 scores.
It appears especially that negative feedback may enhance state anxiety and the
scoring high on N (and reporting iNLEs) may perceive and interpret the feedback
more negatively. Sensitivity to the negative feedback during the GSS interview may
78
manifest as compliance, surfacing in response to a perceived increase in task-
difficulty from the first to the second round of GSS questions (Costa & McCrae,
1992: McGroarty & Baxter, 2007). Higher yield 2 scores and answer shifting may
result.
Hypotheses:
Yield 1:
nNLE reported, iNLE reported and N are hypothesised to correlate significantly in the
positive direction. Compliance may fully mediate the relationship between the
observed independent variables nNLE, iNLE and N and the dependent variable yield
1. nNLE, iNLE and N are expected to exert a significant and positive direct effect
upon C, which in turn is expected to exert a significant and positive direct effect upon
yield 1.
nNLE reported, iNLE reported and N are hypothesised to correlate significantly in the
positive direction. C may fully mediate the relationship between nNLE, iNLE, N and
both yield 2 and shift scores. However, it is expected that only iNLE and N may exert
79
Method
Participants.
The opportunity sample consisted of 127 participants, 78 females and 49 males (mean age =
21.28 years, standard deviation = 5.18). All participants were either undergraduates,
recruited through the experimental participation scheme within the School of Psychology, or
members of the public through the School of Psychology‟s participant panel. All participants
Instruments.
The GSS was presented to each participant individually as a memory task. Several functions
may be measured within the task: (i) immediate and delayed recall; (ii) confabulation; and
(iii) suggestibility. Participants are read a narrative describing a fictitious robbery, followed
Interrogative Suggestibility.
The questioning phase begins immediately after the delayed free-recall condition. Responses
to the first round of 15 misleading questions (out of a total of 20 questions) provide the yield
1 score which indicates the number of misleading questions yielded prior to negative
feedback. (The answers to five „true questions‟ does not contribute to this score.)
Immediately after the first round of 20 questions, negative feedback is given by the
interviewer. The interviewee is told “You have made a number of errors, and it is therefore
necessary to go through all of the questions once more and this time try to be more accurate”.
80
The 20 questions are then repeated to see how readily interviewees shift their initial answers
as a result of the critical feedback given by the interviewer. A yield 2 score is then obtained
answer “shift” score. The interview phase of the GSS generates three measures of
suggestibility:
(i) Yield 1. For each of the misleading questions that are answered in the affirmative the
first time round, or in the case of false alternative questions where one of the
alternatives is chosen, one Yield point is obtained. Thus, the range of possible Yield
(iii) Shift. Changes in response to any of the 20 questions (i.e. including the five „true
questions‟), after their administration the second time, contribute to the „shift‟ score.
According to Gudjonsson (1997), the only changes in answer not coded as such are
those from “no” to non-committal responses (i.e. don‟t know, not sure, maybe,
possibly, or other synonymous words) or vice versa. The „shift‟ score can range from
0 to 20.
The LEQ contains 82 items in total and is a modification of the instrument developed by
Sarason, Johnson, and Siegel (1978), in that it has nine items of particular relevance to
women. These include items such as “Major difficulties with birth control pills or devices”.
The nine additional items in the LEQ were introduced to reduce the gender bias in the
81
Sarason et. al. (1978) version. Participants were required to go through all the events listed,
and if they had experienced them at any point of their life, to circle whether it had been a
“good” experience or “bad” experience. They were then instructed to rate the extent to which
those events had an effect on their lives at the time. The ratings went from 0 (“no effect”) to
3 (“large effect”). The LEQ has good test-retest reliability, with test-retest reliabilities of
psychiatric symptoms.
The questionnaire was originally designed to examine life events experienced over the past
year. However, research has shown the importance of studying life events during
adolescence, as this period is characterized by many physical, social and cognitive changes
(Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987). To assess the relationship between life events and IS, it was
deemed more appropriate to examine events spanning the entire life of the individual.
Participants were instructed to “read through the events listed, and mark the ones that have
occurred throughout your whole life, not just the past year”.
The NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992).
The NEO PI-R is a 240 item self-report measure of the five-factor model of personality. The
traits measured by this measure are N, extraversion (E), openness to experience (O),
agreeableness (A), and conscientiousness (C). Each personality dimension comprises six
items, within the A domain. [Compliance] items such as “I would rather co-operate with
others than compete with them” are answered on a five point scale, ranging from “strongly
disagree” to “strongly agree”. Individual facet scores are obtained by summing the items
corresponding to each facet. Domain scores are derived from summing the appropriate
82
A high level of internal consistency is observed for each domain/factor: N = 0.92, E = 0.89, O
=0 .87, A = 0.86, C = 0.90. The internal consistency of each of the facets ranges from 0.56 to
0.81. Test-retest reliability is also good: Costa & McCrae report that after six years time
interval, N = 0.83, E = 0.82, O = 0.83, A = 0 .63, C = 0.79. This demonstrates both the
reliability as well as the relative stability of each of the factors across time.
The NEO PI-R normally takes 35 minutes to complete in total (Costa & McCrae, 1992). To
reduce the likelihood of participant fatigue (through minimising the length of the procedure,
as participants are also completing the GSS1 and the LEQ) and to ensure interviewees
engaged as effectively as possible with the subsequent GSS interview, it was decided to
Procedure
Chartered Forensic Psychologist in the administration of the GSS. In between the immediate
free-recall and delayed recall phase, each participant completed the LEQ and the NEO PI-R.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for the GSS, NLE, N domain and
individual facet scores and compliance NEO-PI-R scores. The normality of the data was
checked, revealing univariate normality for the GSS and NEO PI-R scores. Measures of
NLEs showed positive skewness (iNLE; Skew Z = 2.09, and kurtosis Z = 6.39). As a result,
the use of parametric statistics was abandoned in favour of the non parametric alternatives
(Pallant, 2007).
83
Table 1. Means (M) and Standard Deviation (SD) scores amongst the observed variables.
M SD
1. IR 14.5 5.71
2. DR 12.9 5.74
3. Y1 4.19 2.17
4. Y2 5.64 2.94
5. S 4.15 2.19
8. N 96.3 13.1
9. C 18.7 4.18
= Shift; iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; nNLE = number of Negative Life Events.
N = Neuroticism. C = Compliance.
Table 2 shows the zero order correlations between the measured variables. A non-parametric
correlation was used because iNLE scores were positively skewed; a method of dealing with
this is to use non-parametric statistical tests (Pallant, 2007, p.88). Results indicate significant
positive correlations between iNLE scores and all three GSS subscales. nNLEs is
84
significantly correlated with yield 2 and shift only. C scores on the NEO PI-R are
significantly correlated with shift scores on the GSS, but not with either the nNLEs or iNLEs
reported. N scores correlated significantly with iNLEs reported, but not significantly with
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
3. S
- .364*** .304** .267** .264**
4.iNLE
- .843** .096 .226*
5.nNLE
- -.029 .048
6. C
- .139
7. N
Note: N = 127. Y1 = Yield 1; Y2 = Yield 2; S = Shift; three subscales from the GSS1.
iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events, nNLE = number of Negative Life Events, N =
Neuroticism, C = Compliance.
*
p < .05; **p < .01; ***
p < .001.
85
Structural Equation Modelling.
Table 3 shows the standardised beta values and fit indices of the models:
The asymptotically distribution free estimation criterion was used to estimate the parameters
and the model fit. Three goodness of fit indices were used to evaluate model fit (Quintana &
Maxwell, 1999; Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell & Abraham, 2004): (i) the comparative fit index
(CFI; where values of 0.9 or above are required), (ii) the goodness of fit index (GFI; values of
0.9 are desirable), and (iii) the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA; values of
.06 or less are wanted). A chi-square difference test (Δχ2) was used, as well as standard fit
indices, to compare the nested models with blended models (BM). Nested models can be
derived from another (i.e. a blended model, which contains both indirect and direct effects)
through restricting parameters. Standard fit indices alone are used to compare the models are
not nested.
Table 2 shows that both nNLE and iNLE are extremely highly correlated (r = .843).
Presenting the two NLE measures in the same model could therefore prove problematic. To
prevent the emergence of artefacts (and any subsequent misleading findings and
interpretations), only iNLE (the stronger of the two measures) will be included within the
models alongside N, C, and GSS scores. The models therefore contain two independent
variables (IVs) (N and iNLE), a mediator (C) and a DV (Yield 1, Yield 2 or Shift).
In order to obtain a degree of freedom (df) within the BMs (considering that, with only two
IVs, the df of the BM is zero) the least significant pathway has been removed from each of
the BMs. This is done to allow for subsequent model comparison and evaluation. If the df is
zero, model evaluation of the BM (and comparison of nested models with the BM) is not
86
With three predictors, a desired statistical power level of 0.8, and α = 0.01the minimum
number of participants required 109 (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). The models
Yield 1:
Table 3 shows that the BM demonstrates the best fit to the data. Both the indirect and direct
Since a pathway was removed from the BM, Δχ2 tests comparing the BM with both the FMM
and NMM models were not performed, as the FMM and NMM models are not nested within
the BM.
Yield 2
Table 3 shows that the BM demonstrates the best fit to the data (compared with the not nested
FMM and the nested NMM). It shows that iNLE exerts a significant positive direct effect
upon yield 2 scores. Neither N nor C exerts significant effects upon yield 2.
Δχ2 tests show that, compared with the NMM, the BM provides a significantly better fit: BM
Shift:
Table 3 shows that the BM demonstrates the best fit to the data (compared with the not nested
FMM and the nested NMM). It shows that iNLE, N and C exert a significant positive direct
effect upon shift. The indirect effects are not statistically significant.
Δχ2 tests show that, compared with the NMM, the BM provides a significantly better fit: BM
87
Figure 1. Blended models to show the effects of iNLE and N on Yield 1 (Y1), Yield 2 (Y2)
and Shift (S). N = 127. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events, N = Neuroticism, and C =
Compliance.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
Yield 1:
.135 .088
e e
1 1
.137
C Y1
.051 .116
iNLE
e e
1 1
.151 (Y2 & S)
.070 (Y2), .207** (S)
C Y2/S
iNLE
88
Table 3. Standardised beta values and fit indices for path-diagrams.
iNLE ↔ N .137 .241 .137 .151 .048 .137 .151 -.249 .137
df 1 2 2 1 2 2 1 2 2
CFI 1.000 0.772 0.869 1.000 0.000 0.867 1.000 0.339 0.949
GFI 1.000 0.985 0.988 0.998 0.961 0.990 0.998 0.929 0.987
RMSEA 0.000 0.093 0.070 0.000 0.200 0.070 0.000 0.252 0.070
PCLOSE 0.945 0.207 0.295 0.541 0.008 0.295 0.541 0.001 0.295
Mediation Model. Empty cells = pathway not included in the model. iNLE = intensity of
89
Discussion
The role of N and C in the relationship between the reporting of more frequent (nNLE) and
more intense NLEs (iNLE) and GSS scores was investigated in this chapter.
nNLE appears to be relevant to both Yield 1 and Yield 2 scores; experiencing frequent NLE
may establish a negative cognitive set within such individuals. This may lead to a lesser
(Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). Evidence shows that cognitive hardiness may moderate the
relationship between the experience of negative life events and vulnerability (Beasley et. al.,
2003). Individuals who are relatively low on cognitive hardiness and experience frequent
NLE may be more suggestible on the GSS – this should be investigated further. It may be
beneficial to also investigate which type of negative life events is most associated with
interrogative suggestibility. Current research merely considers the frequency and intensity of
negative life events experienced as a whole concept. It does not assess which particular
iNLEs (the reporting of more intensely negative negative life events) however seem the
stronger of the two NLE measures and are therefore included within the path models
(alongside N, C and GSS scores). The three subscales will be discussed separately. Some
Yield 1
Results show that the BM provides the best explanation of Yield 1 scores. The model shows
an acceptable fit to the data and demonstrates that iNLE and N exert direct effects on Yield 1,
although these are not statistically significant. These findings are contrary to expectation,
considering previous work demonstrating significant relationships between NLE and Yield 1
90
scores ([chapter 1] Drake et. al., 2008; [chapter 2] Drake & Bull, in press). Additional work
Yield 2
The BM explains yield 2 scores the most adequately. Only iNLEs however seems relevant to
mindset ([chapter 3] Drake, Egan & Bull, in submission). Interviewees reporting more
iNLEs may perceive the negative feedback more negatively, which may heighten uncertainty
as to the correct answer to the questions. A lesser resilience to the negative feedback could
result. Evidence shows that the negative feedback may enhance state anxiety and the
perceived difficulty of the interview (McGroarty & Baxter, 2007). An increase in perceived
difficulty may lead to an increase in uncertainty and any expectations of success (Gudjonsson
& Clarke, 1986). Interviewees reporting more iNLE may perceive an increase in difficulty
from the first round of questions to the second round. This may generate uncertainty and
Shift
The BM provides the best (and an adequate) explanation of shift scores. The model shows
that iNLE, N and C exert significant and positive direct effects on shift. The reporting of
more iNLEs seems to capture a negative mindset ([chapter 3] Drake, Egan & Bull, in
submission). Interviewees scoring high on N are susceptible to distress (Costa & McCrae,
1992). Interviewees reporting more iNLEs may perceive the negative feedback more
negatively. This may heighten uncertainty as to the correct answer to the questions. High
shift scores may result from a desire to reduce distress levels and uncertainty (Gudjonsson &
Clarke, 1986).
91
Findings show that C also exerts a significant direct effect on shift scores (alongside iNLE
and N). Costa and McCrae (1992) define C as a coping mechanism in response to
interpersonal conflict or stress. iNLE and N may generate uncertainty and expectations of
success in response to the negative feedback. C may then manifest, in response to negative
feedback, as a way of coping with the negative feedback and the resultant uncertainty that
may be generated when faced with questioning after receiving negative feedback (on
interviewees‟ initial answers). Compliance with the negative feedback may lead to shifting
on the GSS.
A limitation of this study is the use of the compliance facet within the NEO PI-R. The
compliance facet within the NEO PI-R has relatively low test-retest reliability (c.f. other
second limitation is the relatively small sample size. These findings should therefore be
considered conservative estimations. Further research is needed to verify the role of C within
The current findings seem to suggest, however, that shifting on the GSS may well result from
compliant tendencies within interviewees. Interviewees scoring high on iNLE and N may be
prone to a negative cognitive set during interview (iNLE seems to be a measure of this;
[chapter 3]). Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) recognise the importance of the negative
difficulty, uncertainty, and expectations of success during questioning (Gudjonsson & Clarke,
1986; McGroarty, 2007). Shifts in answers may result. A link between the experience of
major adverse life events and reported false confessions has also been found (Gudjonsson,
92
Sigurdsson & Sigfusdottir, 2008; 2009). These findings further imply that false confessions,
in interviewees reporting more intense adverse life events, could also result from compliant
Each chapter up until now has highlighted different psychological variables that may well be
the influence of the (reported) experience of intensely negative adverse life events, chapter 3
demonstrates the role of adult attachment anxiety in creating a negative mindset (thus leading
neuroticism (and the rating of negative life events as more intensely negative (iNLE);
indicating a negative mindset [chapter 3]; also see Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986) may well be
relevant to Yield 2. Chapter 4 also shows that neuroticism and compliance (and iNLE)
significantly affect Shift scores. Yield 1 was not, however, well explained; neuroticism was
included within the model of best fit (see chapter 4) but it did not exert a significant
influence.
The role of chapter 5 which will be to pull together the various inferences that have emerged
from each of the chapters, to reach a conclusion as to the possible psychological mechanism
subscales).
93
CHAPTER 5:
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF INTERROGATIVE SUGGESTIBILITY: A
VULNERABILITY DURING INTERVIEW.
This chapter extends and pulls together findings within the previous chapters. It investigates
and compliance within the Five-Factor personality model (chapter 4), fearful avoidant
attachment (FAA) (chapter 3), the experience of intense negative life events (iNLE) (chapters
Introduction.
(see Gudjonsson, 2003; Gudjonsson, Young & Bramham, 2007). Factor analytic evidence
suggests two types of interview suggestibility: (i) The acceptance of misleading information
and (ii) sensitivity to the negative feedback/ pressure from the interviewer (Gudjonsson,
1992).
The Gudjonsson and Clarke 1986 model has been the long-established theoretical framework
explaining suggestibility. They propose several important factors, central to encouraging the
suggestible response: (i) uncertainty surrounding the correct answer(s) to the question(s), (ii)
expectations of success: interviewees may feel that the interviewer expects them to know the
correct answer, (iii) the use of negative feedback during the interview, designed to unnerve
the interviewee, and (iv) the establishment of interpersonal trust/rapport between the
negative mindset. It is this which may predispose such interviewees to heightened levels of
94
uncertainty and expectations of success in response to questioning (and therefore
suggestibility).
However, the model does not seem to account for the underlying psychological mechanism
has also failed to adequately explain the psychological mechanism governing misinformation
acceptance in the absence of pressure (i.e. Yield 1 scores). Chapters 1 and 2 found that the
significant correlation between the reporting of intense NLEs and yield 1 scores remains
when memory recall accuracy is controlled for. This suggests that high Yield 1 scores in
interviewees reporting intense NLEs may not be down to such interviewees having a poorer
memory. Chapter 4 found that neuroticism, the experience of intense negative life events and
compliance explained Yield 2 and Shift scores on the GSS (but not Yield 1 scores);
neuroticism was a variable within the best fitting model explaining Yield 1 scores, but did not
Further work into this seems needed, as it is important to investigate the cause of this
psychological vulnerability. Knowledge of the mechanism may help to inform the effective
It could well be though that neuroticism may exert an indirect influence on Yield 1. Chapter
4 (Drake [2009]) found that neuroticism and the experience of intense negative life events
correlated significantly. The experience of intense NLEs in turn correlates with GSS scores
(including yield 1) ([chapter 1] Drake et. al., 2008; [chapter 2] Drake & Bull, in press).
95
(Safford, Alloy, Abramson & Crossfield, 2007). This negative perception is also commonly
found within individuals scoring high in attachment anxiety and avoidance (Bowlby, 1969;
1988).
Evidence relates neuroticism with both attachment anxiety and avoidance (Donnellan, Burt,
Levendosky & Klump, 2008; Noftle & Shaver, 2006). Scarr and McCartney (1983)
suggested that certain genetically influenced traits may elicit negative emotionality, worry,
and anxiety from the social environment and appear to encourage hostile dyadic interactions
(Donnellan, Assad, Robins & Conger, 2007). High levels of attachment anxiety and
avoidance as well as neuroticism may lead to the reporting of more intensely negative NLEs
(as a result of such individuals having a more negative mindset) and consequences (such as
Chapter 3, as well as previous research, has shown the investigative interview to be a dyadic
social interaction (Moston, Stephenson, & Williamson, 1992; Ofshe & Leo, 1997; Pearse &
Gudjonsson, 1999). Studies by Baxter and Boon (2000), Baxter, Boon and Marley (2006),
and Baxter, Jackson and Bain (2003) have demonstrated the role of interviewer influence and
interviewer demeanour may encourage relatively high yield 1 scores, through inducing
uncertainty and expectations of success (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). How interviewees
perceive and interact with the interviewer may affect interviewee cognitive mindset during
interview and influence the extent to which interviewees becoming accepting of misleading
information (delivered to the interviewee in the form of leading questions) and responsive to
negative feedback.
96
Individuals high in both attachment anxiety and avoidance (i.e., those classified as expressing
experience more interpersonal difficulties (Cyranowski, et. al., 2002). Attachment avoidance
seems to lead to a negative attitude towards others, heightened hostility, and low self esteem
(Burnette, et. al., 2009; Fraley & Shaver, 2000). Fearful avoidant attachment patterns are
especially is related to: (a) the exaggeration of previous negative experiences and (b) over-
reporting the intensity of previously experienced negative moods (DeWitte & De Houwer,
2008; Gentzler & Kerns, 2006). Attachment anxiety and avoidance may lead to the reported
experience of more intensely negative negative life events (iNLEs). iNLEs has previously
Fearful avoidant attachment patterns may also affect misinformation acceptance (in the
absence of explicit pressure, i.e. Yield 1 scores on the GSS) directly. Vulnerable individuals
may be more prone towards conflict avoidance during tasks; they may wish to avoid potential
negativity (Muller, 2009) since attachment behaviour has, as a main aim, the maintenance of
proximity (Bowlby, 1988). Avoiding negative thoughts (linked to their interaction with the
of the neuroticism domain (Costa & McCrae, 1992). It may be that vulnerability is the aspect
of neuroticism that relates to both attachment anxiety and avoidance and encourages the
reported experience of more intensely negative events. Vulnerability and fearful avoidant
attachment patterns may correlate and lead to the reported experience of NLEs. During
interview the (reported) experience of intense negative life events may give rise to
97
misinformation acceptance in the absence of explicit pressure (i.e. high yield 1 scores);
fearful avoidant attachment patterns may also affect yield 1 scores directly.
Once pressure is applied by the interviewer (on the interviewee), the acceptance of
misleading information (yield 2 scores) and answer shifting (in response to pressure) is
expected to be caused by the reported experience of NLE (i.e. by the interviewee‟s perception
of the negative feedback). Vulnerability and fearful avoidant attachment may correlate and
susceptibility to stress, may further influence sensitivity to pressure during interview directly;
heightened uncertainty in response to (and a lesser ability to cope with) that pressure
Compliance
Chapter 4 showed that trait compliance may also have an effect on answer shifting (Drake,
negativity or conflict (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Individuals with an endogenous tendency
towards compliant behaviour may be more susceptible to distress and vice versa (and have
compliance (as well as fearful avoidant attachment patterns) may encourage the reporting of
more intensely negative NLEs. Individuals scoring high in compliance may feel that they
should report more intense NLEs (to please the experimenter). During interview this
patterns, may result in answer shifting on the GSS (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986).
98
Hypotheses:
Yield 1:
V and FAA are expected to correlate in the positive direction. V and FAA are expected to
exert significant effects on iNLE; iNLE will in turn significantly influence Yield 1. FAA will
It is expected that the BM containing V, FAA, iNLE and Yield 1 should provide the most
satisfactory fit to the data (compared with the BMs including N, FATT, iNLE and Yield 2
scores).
Yield 2:
V and FATT are expected to correlate in the positive direction. V and FAA are expected to
exert significant effects on iNLE; iNLE will in turn significantly influence Yield 1. V will
It is expected that the BM containing V, FAA, iNLE and Yield 2 should provide the most
satisfactory fit to the data (compared with the BMs including N, FAA, iNLE and Yield 2
scores).
Shift:
V and C may be correlated. V and FAA may also correlate. V, C, may exert indirect
It is expected that the BM containing V, C, FAA, iNLE and Shift should provide the most
satisfactory fit to the data (compared with the BMs including N, C, FAA, iNLE and Shift
scores).
99
Method.
Participants.
The sample consisted of 120 participants, 94 females and 26 males (mean age = 19.35 years,
standard deviation = 1.41, range = 18 to 26). A proportion (20 participants out of the 120) of
the data is taken from a published data set used in the previous chapter. This was done to
maximise the opportunity sample size, so that the path-diagrams would have sufficient
power. It was also done to try to resolve (at least to an extent) the gender skew, given the
through the experimental participation scheme within the School of Psychology. All were
Instruments
Memory Recall
The GSS memory recall task is presented in the form of a narrative, which is made up of 40
recalled‟ if the interviewee is able to freely recall that instance. The interviewee does not
need to recall each instance in the order with which they are presented in the story.
Furthermore, the words used (by the interviewee) to recall the instances need not be exactly
as written in the narrative. Of fundamental importance is that the concept, that is what
occurred within each instance, is correctly recalled. The maximum free-recall score that can
be achieved is 40, which would indicate that the interviewee has correctly recounted
everything that occurred in the story. In the traditional form of the GSS the “immediate”
free-recall phase is followed (after filler tasks) with a delayed recall of the narrative.
100
Interrogative Suggestibility.
The questioning phase traditionally begins immediately after the delayed free-recall. In the
present study the delayed free recall phase was omitted due to: (i) the filler task taking much
less than 50 minutes to complete, providing an inadequate time interval between immediate
recall and the conventional delayed recall phase (Gudjonsson, 1997) and (ii) more recent
studies having shown the delayed free recall phase being an unnecessary part of the
procedure; with little impact upon overall performance (in terms of suggestibility scores).
Minimising participant fatigue, by reducing the length of the procedure, was an additional
motivating factor in the decision to exclude the delayed free-recall phase from the GSS
procedure.
In terms of the calculating the yield 1, yield 2, and shift scores on the GSS, the first round of
15 misleading questions (out of a total of 20 questions) makes up the yield 1 score, which
indicates the number of misleading questions yielded to prior to negative feedback. (The
answers to five „true questions‟ do not contribute to this score). Immediately after the first
round of 20 questions, negative feedback is given by the interviewer. The interviewee is told
“You have made a number of errors, and it is therefore necessary to go through all of the
questions once more and this time try to be more accurate”. All 20 questions are then
repeated, in order to see how readily interviewees shift their initial (20) answers as a result of
the critical feedback and interrogative pressure applied by the interviewer. A yield 2 score is
also obtained, depicting the number of the 15 misleading questions yielded to post-negative
feedback.
The LEQ contains 82 items in total and is a modification of the instrument developed by
Sarason, Johnson, and Siegel (1978), in that it has nine items of particular relevance to
101
women. These include items such as “Major difficulties with birth control pills or devices”.
The nine additional items in the LEQ were introduced to reduce the gender bias in the
Sarason et. al. (1978) version. Participants were required to go through all the events listed,
and if they had experienced them at any point of their life, to circle whether it had been a
“good” experience or “bad” experience. They were then instructed to rate the extent to which
those events had an affect on their lives at the time. The ratings went from 0 (“no effect”) to
3 (“large effect”). The LEQ has good test-retest reliability, with test-retest reliabilities of
psychiatric symptoms.
The questionnaire was originally designed to examine life events experienced over the past
year. However, research has shown the importance of studying life events during
adolescence, as this period is characterized by many physical, social and cognitive changes
(Cohen, Burt, & Bjorck, 1987). To assess the relationship between life events and IS, it was
deemed more appropriate to examine events spanning the entire life of the individual.
Participants were instructed to “read through the events listed, and mark the ones that have
occurred throughout your whole life, not just the past year”.
The RSQ contains 30 items taken from Hazan and Shaver‟s (1987) attachment measure,
Bartholomew and Horowitz‟s (1991) Relationship Questionnaire, and Collins and Read‟s
(1990) Adult Attachment Scale. For each item on the RSQ, participants have to rate on a five
point scale the extent to which each statement best describes their behaviour in close
relationships. Out of the 30 items, five contribute to secure attachment, five to dismissing
attachment patterns (high scores denoting attachment avoidance), four to fearful attachment
patterns (such individuals experience a high level of both attachment anxiety and avoidance),
102
and four to preoccupied attachment patterns (high scores denoting attachment anxiety)
(Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991). Each participant obtains scores for each of the four
attachment patterns; the scores are derived by taking the mean of the four or five items
The RSQ shows high internal reliability (α = .83) as well as high test-retest reliability (at two
The NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992).
The NEO PI-R is a 240 item self-report measure of the five-factor model of personality. The
traits measured are N, extraversion (E), openness to experience (O), agreeableness (A), and
'facets'. Trait compliance is an individual facet score within the A domain of the NEO PI-R.
Items are answered on a five point scale, ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly
agree”. Individual facet scores are obtained by summing the items corresponding to each
facet. Domain scores are derived by summing the appropriate individual facet scores The
NEO shows a high level of internal consistency for each domain/factor: N = 0.92, E = 0.89, O
=0 .87, A = 0.86, C = 0.90. The internal consistency of each of the facets ranges from 0.56 to
0.81. Test-retest reliability is also good: Costa & McCrae report that after six years time
interval, N = 0.83, E = 0.82, O = 0.83, A = 0 .63, C = 0.79. This demonstrates both the
reliability as well as the relative stability of each of the factors across time.
Procedure
Chartered Forensic Psychologist in the administration of the GSS. In between the immediate
free-recall and the interview phase, each participant completed the LEQ, the RSQ, and the N
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 presents the means and standard deviations for the observed variables. The normality
of the data was checked, revealing univariate normality for the GSS and NEO PI-R scores.
Measures of iNLEs showed positive skewness (iNLE; Skew Z = 2.09, and kurtosis Z = 6.39).
As a result, the use of parametric statistics was abandoned in favour of the non parametric
alternatives.
M SD
Y1 3.89 2.14
Y2 5.13 2.75
S 4.24 2.21
TS 8.13 2.78
N 96.3 12.4
C 18.0 3.93
V 12.9 4.84
104
Spearman’s rho correlations
Table 2 shows the zero order correlations between the measured variables. Results indicate
significant positive correlations between iNLE scores GSS scores. V and C also correlate
significantly with Yield 2. iNLE correlates significantly with both N and FATT scores.
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
8.C - .086
9.V -
Note: N = 120. *p < .05, **p < .01, and ***p < .001. Y1 = Yield 1; Y2 = Yield 2; S = Shift;
105
Structural Equation Modelling.
Table 3 shows the standardised beta values and fit indices of the models:
The asymptotically distribution free estimation criterion was used to estimate the parameters
and the model fit. Three goodness of fit indices were used to evaluate model fit (Quintana &
Maxwell, 1999; Wei, Mallinckrodt, Russell & Abraham, 2004): (i) the comparative fit index
(CFI; where values of 0.9 or above are required), (ii) the goodness of fit index (GFI; values of
0.9 are desirable), and (iii) the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA; values of
With four predictors, a desired statistical power level of 0.8, and α = 0.01the minimum
number of participants required 118 (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). The models
Yield 1:
Table 3 shows that BM (V) provides an excellent (and the best) fit to the data (see Figure 1).
BM (V) shows that V and FAA correlate significantly. FAA exert significant indirect effects,
through iNLE, on Yield 1. FAA also affects Yield 1directly, but in the negative direction. V
Yield 2:
Table 3 shows that BM (N) provides the best account of Yield 2 (see Figure 1). V and FAA
correlate significantly. FAA exerts significant indirect effects, via iNLE, on Yield 2 scores.
The direct effect of FAA on Yield 2 is not statistically significant. V exerts an effect on NLE
106
Shift:
Table 3 shows that BM(N) provides an acceptable fit to the data (see Figure 1) (although the
good fit).
The BM (N) shows that: (i) FAA exerts a significant and positive indirect effect on Shift
through iNLE; (ii) C exerts a significant and positive indirect effect on Shift through iNLE;
(iii) N and C exert direct effects on Shift, but these are not statistically significant; and (iv) N
and FAA are significantly correlated; N and C are not significantly correlated.
Figure 1. Path diagrams to show the psychological mechanism governing Yield 1 (Y1),
Yield 2 (Y2) and Shift (S). Note: N = 120. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; FAA =
Fearful-Avoidant Attachment; N = Neuroticism; V = Vulnerability; C = Compliance.
*
p < .05, **p < .01, and ***p < .001.
Yield 1:
FAA
.184* -.200*
e e
1 1
.352***
.174*
iNLE Y1
.168
107
Yield 2:
FAA
.176*
e e
1 1
.317***
.273**
iNLE Y2
.193 .150
Shift:
e
FAA
.191*
1
iNLE
.337*** .183 .491*** e
1
.110
N S
.144*
-.049
.080
108
Table 3. Standardised beta values and fit indices of the blended models (BM).
V - - - .164 - .111
N - - .150 - .110 -
C - - - - -.049 -.058
V↔ C - - - - - .165*
N↔C - - - - .080 -
df 1 1 1 1 2 2
Note: N = 120. *p < .05, **p < .01, and ***p < .001. Empty cells = pathways not included in
the model. iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; FAA = Fearful-Avoidant Attachment;
The aim of this study is to extend chapter 4 and pull together the findings in previous
Yield 1:
The findings suggest that V and FATT are correlated; FATT indirectly affects Yield 1
through iNLE. V may be the aspect of N that may encourage relatively hostile reactions in
others (Donnellan et. al., 2007; Scarr & McCartney, 1983) and the subsequent formation of
fearful avoidant attachment patterns. This may cause a more negative perception of
situations (Bowlby, 1988), leading to vulnerable interviewees rating negative events as more
an underlying negative mindset). This negative mindset during interview may lead to the
There is also a significant direct effect of FATT on Yield 1, but in the negative direction.
Certain interviewees scoring high on FATT may have a tendency towards a negative
perception of others and, as a result, may become more suspicious of the interviewer (Rydell
& Bringle, 2007). Suspiciousness of the interviewer‟s motives may reduce cooperation
during interview (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 28). This may lead to a reduction in uncertainty and
1986).
Yield 2:
As with Yield 1, V and FATT correlate significantly and there is a significant indirect effect
susceptibility to distress, measured by V (or N in the case of Yield 2), as well as a high
110
degree of attachment anxiety and avoidance, which may lead to a more negative perception
(shown by reporting iNLEs) of the interview post negative feedback. Such interviewees may
be more prone to viewing the task of answering questions (after receiving negative feedback
in response to their previous answers) as relatively arduous (McGroarty & Baxter, 2007).
This may be due to those interviewees experiencing heightened uncertainty and expectations
of success (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986) (which may ultimately stem from their insecure
They may also feel less able to trust their memory as a result of being exposed to negative
feedback in response to their initial answers (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 197). To cope with the
negative feedback, their resultant uncertainty and distress levels, vulnerable interviewees may
become more accepting of the misleading information and answer shift (Gudjonsson, 1995).
Shift:
Findings show that FAA exerts a significant and positive indirect effect on shift through
iNLE. As with Yield 1 and 2, FAA seems to encourage the reporting of more intense NLE;
attachment anxiety and avoidance appears to generate a negative mindset and perception of
situations. This negative mindset may lead to a lesser ability to cope with critical feedback
during the GSS interview (in response to their initial answers). Such individuals may be less
Results also show that C exerts a significant and positive indirect effect on shift through
iNLE. Individuals scoring high on trait C may expect negativity and have a pessimistic
mindset (C and V appear to correlate significantly; the more susceptible to stress, the more
compliant individuals appear to be). This pessimistic mindset may encourage the reporting of
more intense NLE. Alternatively, perhaps such individuals feel that they should report more
111
intense NLEs (to please the experimenter) when completing the LEQ (although it should be
noted that no such instruction was given though to participants). During interview, in
response to negative feedback, this tendency towards compliance may manifest (once again)
in response to perceived interpersonal conflict (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Interviewees may
comply with the negative feedback – the feedback that they have made errors - and change
their answers.
Interviewees scoring high in C may perceive compliance [with the negative feedback] as
being more beneficial to them than not complying. Evidence suggests that vulnerable
individuals tend to perceive the task of answering questions post negative feedback as more
arduous and stressful (McGroarty & Baxter, 2007). Compliance with negative feedback may
FAA and C seem to affect Shift independently. They lead to a negative mindset and a desire
to avoid conflict (Bowlby, 1969; 1988). Attachment patterns have, as their main aim, the
2009). This negative mindset and conflict avoidance may mean that, during perceived
interpersonal conflict (i.e. in response to negative feedback from the interviewer), trait C – an
established pattern of behaviour – may surface and, in some cases, lead to shifting on the
GSS.
This finding does need to be verified and replicated though through further research, as there
A limitation may be the gender skew of the current sample. This may have affected the
112
suggestibility on the GSS1 however indicate non-significant differences (Gudjonsson 2003,
p. 379). This suggests that gender may not affect performance on the GSS significantly; it
may just be the relationship between the observed measures that could be affected.
Nevertheless, the parameter estimates and fit statistics should thus be considered conservative
The NEO PI-R is used to measure compliance (rather than more reliable measures such as the
Gudjonsson Compliance Scale; Gudjonsson, 1989). The NEO PI-R normally takes 35
minutes to complete in total (Costa & McCrae, 1992). To reduce the likelihood of
participant fatigue (through minimising the length of the procedure, as participants are also
completing the GSS1, the RSQ and the LEQ) and to ensure interviewees engaged as
effectively as possible with the subsequent GSS interview, it was decided to measure both N
Difficulties in recruiting sufficient male participants also meant that a proportion of the data
is shared with the previous study. The previous study uses the NEO PI-R to measure both N
and compliance. This study also uses the NEO PI-R for the same purposes.
The correlation between iNLE and GSS scores in this study also seems lower in comparison
to previous studies within this thesis (Drake, Bull & Boon, 2008 [chapter1]; Drake & Bull, in
press [chapter 2]). However, it is more similar to the correlations found in the previous
studies (Drake, Egan & Bull, under review [chapter 3]; Drake, 2009 [chapter 4]). A further
similarity between this chapter and the previous is that the sample sizes are almost identical
(and significantly larger than in the Drake et. al., 2008 study and in chapter 2). These weaker
correlations that seem to be emerging throughout the latter studies could be to do with sample
size; as sample size increases the relationship between iNLE and GSS scores weakens
opportunity samples. The larger the sample, the more likely it is to recruit participants who
may well report intense adverse life events but who, at the same time, experience different
levels of cognitive hardiness. This would, in theory, render the correlation between iNLE
An implication is that some interviewees may still believe their recollection of the GSS
narrative – so not believe they have made errors – and therefore not feel uncertain as to their
memory (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). Such interviewees may just feel that compliance with
the instruction that they have made errors may lead to a better outcome for them (or be more
beneficial). In some cases, uncertainty may not necessarily be a pre-requisite for shifting on
the GSS.
These current findings suggest further that attachment anxiety and avoidance (as well as trait
turn, these factors may be the basis of the negative mindset (measured by iNLE), which
Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) consider central to bringing about the suggestible response
during questioning. Such behaviour may manifest as false statements, recollections, and
114
CHAPTER 6: CONCLUSION
The overall aim of this thesis has been to gain insight into the possible psychological
possible reason why vulnerable interviewees may be more prone to developing a negative
mindset during interview, leading to expectations of success and (sometimes) uncertainty (see
My first study (my BSc dissertation) found a correlation between the [reported] experience of
intensely negative NLEs and interrogative suggestibility. This study suggests that
interviewees reporting intense NLEs are significantly more susceptible to the leading
questions, as well as to negative feedback, administered during the GSS interview. Chapter 2
(Drake & Bull, in press) replicated the above conclusion. It also provides additional evidence
suggesting that the reported experience of intense adversity may be linked to increased
interrogative suggestibility, as a result of such interviewees being less able to cope with
pressure during questioning. The link between NLEs and interrogative suggestibility on the
GSS seems not to be attributable to field dependence. Further research by Gudjonsson et. al.,
(2008; 2009) has also found links between the experience of intense adverse life events and
reported false confessions amongst student populations. To an extent this replicates (and
corroborates) findings from chapter 1 and 2, especially since interrogative suggestibility and
false confessions can be related (see Gudjonsson, 1991; Henkel & Coffman, 2004; Kassin,
governing performance on the yield 1, yield 2 and shift dimensions of the GSS. Chapters 3
and 4 (Drake, 2009; Drake, Egan & Bull, in submission) investigated, independently, the
effects of attachment anxiety (chapter 3), neuroticism and compliance (chapter 4) and iNLE
115
on interrogative suggestibility. Chapter 4 identified the role of neuroticism and compliance
in bringing about suggestible behaviour. It also concluded that it seems to be the impact or
perception of adversity (an indicator of an underlying negative mindset; see chapter 3) which
seemed to be the most central in encouraging suggestible responses on the GSS. The number
of NLEs, or the fact that NLEs occur, seems less relevant to explaining suggestible behaviour
Each chapter prior to chapter 5 has reached a slightly different conclusion as to the
Chapter 3 proposed that attachment anxiety and iNLE contribute significantly to Yield 2 and
Shift, but not Yield 1. When this model was extended in Chapter 4 evidence for the role of
neuroticism (N) (and iNLE) being relevant to Yield 2 emerged; it also shows that neuroticism
and compliance (and iNLE) significantly affect Shift scores. Yield 1 was not, however, well
explained; neuroticism was included within the model of best fit (see chapter 4) but it did not
Chapter 1 to 4 provided the impetus for an integrative chapter 5, the role of which was to
unify the various inferences emerging from each of the previous chapters. Chapter 5
analysed the relationship between each of the variables explored independently within each
of the previous chapters integrating results into an overall structure. Using structural
equation modelling this chapter explored the effects of attachment anxiety and avoidance
(chapter 3), Neuroticism (chapter 4), iNLE (chapter 1-4), on GSS scores. It also explored
which facet of neuroticism was most relevant to explaining GSS performance (considering
that chapter 4 highlights the role of neuroticism in GSS scores). Chapter 5 showed that
and iNLE contributed significantly to performance on all three GSS subscales, yield 1
116
included. The neuroticism domain best explains both yield 2 and shift (alongside attachment
anxiety and avoidance [FAA], C and iNLE), which corroborates findings in chapter 4.
Vulnerability (a facet of neuroticism), however, provides the best account of yield 1 scores
the mechanism may help to inform the effective treatment and management of suggestible
behaviour during interview. It may also help us understand why vulnerable individuals are
more inclined towards making false statements and/or confessions during interview.
This final chapter will discuss and evaluate the findings to emerge from this thesis. It will do
so by addressing the question of why (and when) innocents can make false
confessions/statements during questioning. This chapter will also reflect on what has been
learned about the concept of interrogative suggestibility, discuss the limitations of the current
work, as well as offer suggestions for further research into understanding the psychology of
To briefly re-iterate what was mentioned in chapter 1: recent research suggests police
induced false confessions are present in 15-20% of DNA exoneration cases (Kassin et. al., in
press). Gary Dotson was the first to be exonerated in 1989 through the use of DNA testing,
and there have been around 200 further wrongful conviction cases overturned since then.
This figure only represents those false confessions that are: (i) not disproved before trial, (ii)
do not result in a guilty plea, (iii) those in which DNA evidence is not available, and/or (iv)
117
those given in minor criminal cases, or from juveniles. Further studies have revealed that
false confessions can occur even within the more educated student population when
questioned by police (Gudjonsson et. al., 2006; 2008; 2009). This suggests the 15-20% of
false confessions probably represents a minority of the cases that have come to light over the
decades; the tip of the iceberg in terms of the actual number of false confessions made during
police interview.
Over 100 years Hugo Munsterberg (1908) wrote an entire chapter on “untrue confessions”, in
which he attempted to try to understand the cause of such occurrences; some of the words he
used to describe the possible causes of this phenomenon were promises, threats and
suggestion. The idea that suggestions and coercion may be a factor in inducing false
research has since continued and plays a significant role in the study and prevention of
wrongful convictions. The aim of such work has been to understand why certain individuals
are more prone to being induced to making false statements, during questioning – this
forming the basis of subsequent wrongful convictions (Drizin & Leo, 2004).
Sean Hodgson‟s recently quashed conviction highlights the troubling and controversial nature
confessions to police during custodial interview (although he pleaded not guilty at trial) and
was sentenced back in 1982, spending nearly 30 years behind bars as an innocent man. Little
did Police know that Sean Hodgson actually had mental health problems, a history
of/tendency towards pathological lying, and an obsession with confessing to crimes that he
was not involved with. It appears that these factors made it almost impossible for him to
withstand the pressure of questioning. An eagerness to please, perhaps a need for attention
118
(even negative), amongst other things may have exacerbated this, leading to these voluntary
The majority of police induced false confessions occur due to suspects being unable to cope
with the pressure of police questioning, which can sometimes involve harsh tactics with
interviewers sometimes have certain ideas/beliefs about who may have perpetrated the
offence, and then seek evidence for this in their subsequent questioning of suspects.
Alternatively interviewers may interpret any evidence through their existing beliefs regarding
The Central Park Jogger (CPJ) case is an example of this (see Kassin et. al., in press). In
1989, five teenagers were videotaped confessing to CPJ murder, but later said they were
coerced. Five false confessions were made within a single police investigation to the brutal
rape and assault of a young female jogger. Each confession was immediately withdrawn by
the teenagers. The teenagers claimed that they confessed due to police coercion; the promise
that they could go home. Persuasive tactics can be (and were) used, to the point where the
innocent(s) felt compelled to confess. The five were, of course, eventually exonerated in
Confessions can be wholly unreliable, yet extremely weighty in the eyes of the court (Drizin
& Leo, 2004). Since that time the Courts have set guidelines regarding the admissibility of
through coercion are not permitted to be used as evidence. This is fine, if it were not for the
119
fact that evidence is starting to emerge suggesting that the reliability of
statements/confessions may ultimately come down to how the suspect perceives and
interprets the interview (and not necessarily what actually takes place) ([chapter 4] Drake,
2009; [chapter 3] Drake, Egan & Bull, in submission; and chapter 5).
Even if there are no obvious signs of coercive tactics used during interview, vulnerable
suspects may still feel that they are not given the chance to tell the truth and continue to feel
under pressure (Jakobsson-Öhrn & Nyberg, 2009), which may well have an impact on the
reliability of their evidence. A confession may well seem voluntary on the surface (and
therefore be admissible in Court – and given significance) but it may still, in reality, be the
product of perceived coercion and therefore unreliable. Proving though that the so-called
voluntary confession is, in fact, a product of the suspect feeling coerced is difficult, since it
would be very hard to reliably determine whether or not a suspect feels or perceives pressure
during an interview. There is a danger then that seemingly voluntary (but actually police
induced) false confessions may still contribute to wrongful convictions, despite the
Regardless of the age, capacity or state of the confessor, what (false) confessors tend to have
in common is a decision (at some point during the interview process) that confession will be
more beneficial to them than continuing to maintain their innocence. Being sensitive to
2003). This sensitivity can lead to the acceptance of misleading suggestions, false statements
and confessions.
The Gudjonsson and Clarke 1986 model is the established theoretical framework explaining
response: (i) uncertainty surrounding the correct answer(s) to the question(s), (ii)
120
expectations of success: interviewees may feel that the interviewer expects them to know the
correct answer, (iii) the use of negative feedback during the interview, designed to unnerve
the interviewee, and (iv) the establishment of interpersonal trust/rapport between the
interviewer and interviewee; this seems to enhance the believability of the negative feedback,
rendering it more penetrative. Central to their proposal is that the suggestible individual has a
relatively negative mindset both at the start and throughout the interview. It is this which
The aim of this thesis has been to try to understand the underlying psychological mechanism
that may encourage this vulnerability to occur during interview, under inappropriate
interview conditions (Home Office, 2007). As mentioned, chapters 1 and 2 uncovered a link
between the reported experience of intense negative life events and interrogative
suggestibility on the GSS. The reported experience of intense adverse life events appears
particularly related to sensitivity to negative feedback (chapter 3); this sensitivity can be
observed by interviewees changing their initial answers (to questions) in response to negative
work reveals an association between the experience of major adverse life events and reported
false confessions (Gudjonsson, Sigurdsson & Sigfusdottir, 2008; 2009). These studies
demonstrate that the experience of intense life adversity might be linked to interrogative
adversity appears to create a lesser ability to cope with pressure. Vulnerable interviewees
may be more inclined towards ineffective coping methods (e.g. compliance) in the face of
121
An investigative interview may be considered a dyadic social interaction (Ofshe & Leo,
1997). Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) recognise the importance of this interviewer-
provide some support for this (Baxter & Boon, 2000; Baxter, Jackson and Bain, 2003). They
demonstrate that a negative interviewer demeanour can have an adverse effect on GSS
performance, which shows that the interviewer, even in absence of explicit pressure, can
bring about a suggestible response in an interviewee. Negative (or even neutral) interviewer
demeanour may be perceived more negatively by vulnerable individuals due to their insecure
attachment patterns (Bowlby, 1988), rendering these individuals more open to any inaccurate
information.
The studies in the later chapters of this thesis shows that vulnerable interviewees also seem to
have an endogenous, and possibly even partly genetically influenced (Donnellan et. al., 2007;
2008), tendency/predisposition towards distress (see chapter 5). This may be the basis of
(and lead to the formation of) insecure attachment patterns within such individuals. Insecure
avoidance.
Vulnerable interviewees are more prone towards conflict avoidance during interview; they
may wish to avoid (expected) negativity and thus may be eager to please the interviewer
emotional, psychological and physical proximity with a significant other person (Bowlby,
1988). Avoiding negative thoughts (i.e. that the interviewer may have ulterior/negative
motives for asking the questions) linked to the interviewer may help to preserve the
misleading suggestions during questioning (even in the absence of explicit negative feedback
122
or pressure).
There is also a direct influence of attachment anxiety and avoidance (i.e. fearful avoidant
attachment patterns [FAA]) on Yield 1 within the GSS which is significant, but in the
negative direction. This is contrary to what was hypothesised in chapter 5. Perhaps certain
interviewees scoring high on FAA may have a tendency towards a negative perception of
others and, as a result, may become more suspicious of the interviewer (Rydell & Bringle,
2007). Suspiciousness of the interviewer‟s motives may reduce cooperation during interview
(Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 28). This may lead to a reduction in uncertainty and therefore
1986).
It seems then that both high V and FAA may lead to relative interrogative resistance within
interviewees as well as suggestibility. FAA patterns result from a relatively high level of
both attachment anxiety and avoidance (Griffin & Bartholomew, 1994). Attachment anxiety
is related to emotion oriented coping and the regulation of effective processes (Fraley &
(Donnellan et. al., 2008). Perhaps if attachment anxiety is dominant relative to attachment
suspicious cognitive set and relative resistance. Further research is needed to investigate this
possibility.
123
Yield 1
FAA
e e
1 1
Path diagrams to show the psychological mechanism governing Yield 1 (Y1) subscale on the
GSS (see chapter 5). iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; FAA = Fearful-Avoidant
Attachment (observed by a high attachment anxiety and avoidance); V = Vulnerability (an
endogenous susceptibility to stress). U = Uncertainty, EoS = Expectations of Success – an
assumed latent variable (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986).
Once negative feedback is given on the GSS, vulnerable behaviour seems to be caused solely
by the negative mindset/perception [of the negative feedback] within vulnerable individuals
(and less by insecure attachment tendencies directly, as with Yield 1) (see chapters 3 to 5).
information, and change their answers, in response to pressure may depend upon their
perception of the negative feedback; how negatively they interpret the negative feedback.
The experience of intense adverse life events, their insecure attachment style, and
fundamentally a (partly biological; Donnellan et. al., 2007; 2008) tendency towards distress
appears to contribute to this negative mindset (see chapter 5). An endogenous susceptibility
to distress (Costa & McCrae, 1992) appears to lead to the formation of insecure attachment
124
patterns within vulnerable interviewees (Scarr & McCartney, 1983). Negative emotionality,
worry, and anxiety appear to encourage hostile interactions (Donnellan, Assad, Robins &
Conger, 2007), and ergo seem to lead to insecure inter-personal attachment patterns. Insecure
attachment patterns may in turn lead to a negative perception of events and others (causing
such individuals to report more intensely negative adverse life events) (Bowlby, 1988).
vulnerability (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). This vulnerability could manifest as false
Yield 2:
FAA
e e
1 1
Path diagrams to show the psychological mechanism governing Yield 2 (Y2) subscale of the
GSS (see chapter 5). iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; FATT = Fearful-Avoidant
Attachment (observed by a high attachment anxiety and avoidance); N = Neuroticism (an
endogenous susceptibility to stress); U = Uncertainty, EoS = Expectations of Success – an
assumed latent variable (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986).
125
Shift:
FAA
e e
1 1
C iNLE U & EoS S
Path diagrams to show the psychological mechanism governing the Shift (S) subscale of the
GSS (see chapter 5). iNLE = intensity of Negative Life Events; FAA = Fearful-Avoidant
Attachment (observed by a high attachment anxiety and avoidance); V = Vulnerability (an
endogenous susceptibility to stress); C = Compliance (an inclination towards compliant
coping during interpersonal conflict. This may manifest during interview in response to U &
EoS; Drake, 2009); U = Uncertainty, EoS Expectations of Success – an assumed latent
variable (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986).
elements of the brain (Corbalis & Lea, 1999; Oler et. al., 2009; Pape, Jungling, Seidenbecher,
Lestling & Reinscheid, 2010) – maybe these occasionally unhelpful susceptibilities maintain
themselves within the human population due to some evolutionary value. It could well be
malleable may create (and preserve) relationships with (perceived) authority figures (Muller,
2009). This may perhaps be the ultimate reason why suggestibility and false confessions
(within the context of a custodial interview) is so strongly and consistently related to the
126
experience of intense adversity (see chapters 1-5; Drake, 2009; Drake & Bull, in press; Drake
et al., 2008; Drake, Egan & Bull, in submission. Also see Gudjonsson, 2008; 2009).
Apart from during police interview (where this behaviour can lead to negative consequences),
being easily influenced in the face of adversity (in other situations) may pay advantageous
perceived by such individuals as more beneficial to them. It may allow such individuals to
successfully negotiate and cope with their (hostile) environment. Through repeated exposure
to negative feedback and/or reactions from others, such individuals may learn to distrust their
own judgment (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 197). This environment may help cultivate an
individual that may be more prone to suggestible behaviour during dyadic interactions –
especially high stake or stressful interactions (such as a police interview). Research shows
that stress and state anxiety tends to correlated with interrogative suggestibility (Gudjonsson,
1988).
Previous work shows that state anxiety is more important than trait anxiety (in influencing the
GSS see Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 385); however, those studies (e.g. my BSc dissertation [2004-
2005]; Gudjonsson, 1983; 1988; Haraldsson, 1985) only examined direct correlations
between trait anxiety and interrogative suggestibility. When considering neuroticism not just
in isolation but within a network of (other relevant) psychological variables using structural
equation modelling, although neuroticism does not have a direct effect on interrogative
negative mindset of the individuals; such interviewees may then be more prone to state
127
anxiety during interview (being more susceptible to stress), evoking expectations of success
[police] interview could prove difficult, and becoming ever more so with age (see chapter 2).
These findings suggest that, during interview, the emphasis should be on boosting the
interviewee‟s perception of their ability. This may be achieved by the interviewer being
interviewees also tend to feel the need to provide an answer, thinking it unsatisfactory to say
that they cannot remember (Gudjonsson, Young & Branham, 2007; Home Office, 2007).
The danger is that, in response to open questions such as “tell me....” vulnerable interviewees
may fabricate information in order to comply with the implicit instruction communicated in
The interviewer should ideally be a facilitator, assisting the interviewee so that they do not
choose compliance as an option – but, instead, feel in control of the interview. This should
ensure the retrieval of reliable information from the interviewee. The interviewee‟s
perception of the interview seems fundamental to the accuracy of the information obtained.
These implications are supported by the current guidelines in Achieving Best Evidence (UK
Home Office, 2007) as well as by other applied forensic research into the effective
interviewing of children and vulnerable adults (Fisher & Geiselman, 1992; La Rooy & Lamb,
2008; La Rooy, Lamb, & Pipe, 2008; Milne, 1999; Milne & Bull, 1999).
When considering suspect vulnerability within the UK it is critically important that there are
Initially at interview, one such safe guard is the presence of an appropriate adult (for
communication) and a legal representative. At Court, if the Judge considers the information
128
in interview was unfairly obtained, then it can be excluded under Section 76 and 78 PACE
1984. Another avenue of protection is the training of the interviewers. This is an area which
is particularly sparse with regard to research. Even when interviewers ask open ended or
open specific questions, vulnerable interviewees could still give misleading and/or unreliable
answers. Therefore the interviewer needs to consider their questioning strategy and how they
may reduce the affects of suggestibility or compliance. Additional problems also tend to
come through when “the principles of investigative interviewing are not reflected in standard
police practice” (Williamson, 1993, p. 98). It is essential that investigative interviewers are
properly trained and that this is maintained and refreshed. The introduction and use of
'ground rules' (i.e. telling the interviewee that it is Ok to say "Don't know") could also be
The aim of this thesis has been to explain/offer preliminary insight into the underlying
during interview. Based upon the theoretical findings, insight is offered into how vulnerable
behaviour may be best managed; this appears to coincide with what (other) applied research
in the area of investigative interviewing has also concluded (based upon actual suspects) (La
Rooy & Lamb, 2008; La Rooy, Lamb, & Pipe, 2008; Milne, 1999; Milne & Bull, 1999).
This provides evidence suggesting that (at least to an extent) the current theoretical models
of suggestible behaviour; the current models seem to have predictive validity. The models
seem to predict “what works” most effectively when interviewing vulnerable individuals
Vulnerability during interview may essentially be the product of merging influences that
129
and avoidance; Donnellan et. al., 2008) with environmental influences (parental attachment
patterns/adverse life experiences). This may well be the fundamental mechanism which,
when combined with the Gudjonsson and Clarke (1986) model, may bring us a deeper insight
into the psychology of vulnerability during police interview. These findings may help us
understand one of the major reasons why innocents sometimes make false confessions and
Over the course of this thesis it has become very apparent that it is the perception of adversity
which is critical in bringing about vulnerability during police interview) not so much the fact
that it occurs or which type of event takes place. Even when one considers the types of major
life events such as the ones Gudjonsson et. al. (2008; 2009) found to be linked with reported
false confessions (e.g., victimisation, being sexually abused by an adult within or outside of
the family, the death of parent or sibling, being witness to or experiencing violence at home
involving adults, amongst others), the primary reason why such events may exert such a
strong influence upon individuals may well be due to how they are perceived and internalised
by the individual. Most people regard events such as these as serious; however, if
internalised and attributed to factors within the person (Ross, 1977) such as their own [lack
of] intelligence or other variables that make the individual feel responsible for having brought
the traumatic event on themselves, these serious adverse life events are likely to be the most
damaging.
Attribution tendencies (and coping) may therefore be highly relevant, moderating the extent
to which events regarded as intense, serious, or major exert their influence on interview
130
suggestibility levels. This may be likely considering the link between self perception and
attribution tendencies (Peterson, Maier, & Seligman, 1995); insecure attachment may bring
about a negative self perception of self, others and events, which may lead to internal
attribution tendencies – such individuals may experience more intense adverse life events
and, most crucially, attribute their cause to factors within themselves (particularly
preoccupied and fearful individuals, with a high level of attachment anxiety). On the
contrary, those with a tendency to attribute cause externally (e.g., bad teaching when failing
an exam), may also experience harrowing events, due to their relatively positive self
perception (perhaps down to a more secure attachment pattern), but those events are less
likely to make their mark as easily. These individuals may be more resilient to adversity,
meaning that during interview they would be better able to withstand any pressure, and be
Cognitive hardiness may also have a moderating effect on the degree to which individuals
become vulnerable as a result of (reportedly) experiencing intense adversity (Beasley et. al.,
2003). Individuals who perceive intense adversity but are relatively cognitively hardy may
be less likely to be vulnerable during questioning; they may be less easily influenced by
leading questions and/or pressure during interview. Overall, though, although there are
moderating influences, it seems to be the extent to which situations and the negative feedback
is negatively perceived which dictates the degree to which interviewees may be suggestible
during questioning.
131
The Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale.
Common to all three subscales is that attachment anxiety and avoidance indirectly affects
suggestible responses (i.e. Yield 1, Yield 2 and Shift), through the experience of intense
adversity. This indirect effect may reflect the formation of the negative mindset within
vulnerable interviewees (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). iNLE seems to indicate a negative
mindset (or the presence of a negative mindset; see chapter 3); high levels of attachment
anxiety and avoidance seem to result in the negative perception of events, situations, and
others – and thus the reporting of more intensely negative adverse life events. This negative
observed as the acceptance of misleading information, both in the absence of and in response
There are though some differences between the three GSS subscales. This may explain why,
across this thesis, Yield 2 and Shift tends to correlate more strongly than Yield 1 and Shift
(also see Gudjonsson, 2003). The psychological mechanism encouraging Yield 2 and Shift is
quite similar, in that there is a direct pathway from neuroticism to Yield 2/Shift. Both Yield
2 and Shift seem to come about through a negative mindset within vulnerable interviewees,
induced by fearful avoidant attachment patterns, (which influences the interpretation of the
negative feedback). A direct endogenous sensitivity to distress seems also pertinent to Yield
2 and Shift, but to a lesser extent. Yield 2 and Shift have similar psychological mechanisms;
they seem to be governed by similar factors, which may be a reason why they tend to be
highly correlated. Yield 1 and 2 also share a common element; the extent to which
Yield 2 is marginally different from Shift, as compliance seems to not exert a significant
influence within the best fitting model. Compliance seems significant when accounting for
132
answer-shifts in response to negative feedback, but not to explaining why vulnerable
interviewees may accept misleading information during GSS interview. There seems a
degree of variation, however, as to the role of compliance within suggestibility post negative
feedback. Chapter 5 found that Yield 2 and compliance correlate significantly (when looking
interpersonal conflict and is a coping mechanism (Costa & McCrae, 1992; Gudjonsson,
1989). Compliance seems to have an effect, but as to its extent and whether it influences
Shift and/or Yield 2 needs to be verified. A limiting factor within this work is that it uses the
C facet from the NEO PI-R and not a more reliable measure of compliance (i.e. the
Gudjonsson Compliance Scale; Gudjonsson, 1989). Usage of such an instrument, and with a
larger sample of participants, may help to clarify the role of compliance within GSS scores
If Yield 1 and Shift are compared, the psychological mechanisms are more dissimilar: (i)
Neuroticism has a direct effect on shift (which may lead to a lesser resilience to the negative
feedback; Gudjonsson, 1995) (Yield 1 does not have this). (ii) Yield 1 has the direct effect of
attachment patterns onto suggestibility (Shift does not). Yield 1 seems to be governed
predominantly by the attachment patterns and negative mindset of the interviewee. This
suggests that, even when it comes to the acceptance of misleading information in the absence
of explicit pressure, the two-way interaction (how the interviewee engages and perceives the
interviewer) seems to still be a factor. This supports Gudjonsson and Clarke‟s (1986)
assertion that rapport – or the presence of rapport – is fairly essential for suggestible
behaviour to emerge. Research conducted by Baxter et. al. (2000; 2003; 2006) also supports
this showing the influence of negative interviewer demeanour on Yield 1. The negative
interviewer demeanour may be perceived and interpreted more negatively by the relatively
133
more suggestible interviewees (see chapter 3). These findings further suggest that there may
well still be some implicit interviewer influences affecting the cognitive set of the
interviewee, which in turn may dictate the extent to which misinformation (in the absence of
Performances on the GSS as a whole may well come down to sensitivity to interviewer
influence or feedback, which may affect the degree to which interviewees accept the
influence (i.e. The mere presence of the interviewer and being more likely to perceive [and
more accepting of any misleading information prior to negative feedback (yield 1).
determined by the extent to which interviewees are sensitive to both the explicit negative
feedback (delivered after the first round of questions) as well as the presence of the
interviewer. Interviewees will still be answering questions from the interviewer, so the
interviewer-interviewee relationship may still be a factor affecting the cognitive set of the
interviewee during the second round of questions (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). My findings
suggest that attachment anxiety and avoidance is a dominant factor, alongside the negative
Shift scores appear determined largely by the degree to which individuals are sensitive to
explicit negative feedback (these interviewees may not necessarily accept much of the
misleading information [prior to negative feedback], but just be more likely to change
answers in response to it). This group would not be so pervious to implicit interviewer
influence. They may not be so sensitive to subtle changes in interviewer behaviour – and
134
therefore do not yield during the first round of questions; yet, on receipt of explicit negative
feedback they comply, as a way of coping, and shift (see chapter 4).
Compliance and Yield 1 are not significantly related although there is evidence, albeit
tentative, that compliance might be related to Shift (and maybe Yield 2 – see chapter 5).
negativity; the GSS negative feedback from the interviewer may be perceived as
“interpersonal conflict” and could induce compliant coping. Yield 1 scores though are
obtained in the absence of explicit negative feedback, where there is relatively less
“interpersonal negativity”, which could be a reason why compliance may not be a significant
factor in Yield 1 scores. Based upon the above argument, shifting would be predicted to
occur irrespective of high yield 1 scores (as has been found; shift and yield 1 scores
Certain types of vulnerable individuals on the GSS are perhaps relatively more resilient and
therefore require explicit feedback to induce vulnerability. They may not be particularly
perceptive to subtle implicit changes in demeanour (see Baxter et. al., 2000) and therefore not
score high on the yield 1 dimension. With suggestibility it appears that the individual may
experience a gradual decline in their ability to trust their memory in the face of uncertainty
and therefore gradually acquires the tendency to trust others‟ judgments and memories rather
than their own (Gudjonsson, 2003, p. 197). With compliance, this is not the case; there is no
evidence of memory distrust; the suspect/individual merely submits to the other‟s request
(Gudjonsson, 1989). Suggestible behaviour on the surface bares similarity with compliance,
but has a different psychological cause. It may be that shifting on the GSS may be caused by
why yield 1 and shift do not always correlate significantly; compliance and suggestibility do
135
not always correlate significantly (Drake, 2009; Gudjonsson, 2003; Richard & Kelly, 2004).
Interviewees who are compliant (but not suggestible) may not accept much of the misleading
information prior to negative feedback, but still change their answers on receipt of negative
feedback.
This may also be the reason why some vulnerable interviewees on the GSS score relatively
highly on yield 1, but have not shifted. Perhaps those interviewees, scoring high on Yield 1
but not Shifting, are the suggestible individuals, who only need the presence of the
success and therefore suggestible behaviour (Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986). Such
interviewees have already accepted relatively high levels of misinformation during the first
round of questions. When negative feedback is then given, that they have made errors, their
already negative mindset is not so detrimentally affected. The negative feedback induces
relatively little change in negative mindset (c.f. how it was before the negative feedback was
given), therefore those interviewees may not shift answers. Such interviewees distrust their
memory (see Gudjonsson, 2003) and, as a result, even post negative feedback, continue to
yield to the misinformation. Here the negative feedback has little observable impact.
The most vulnerable individuals would score high on Yield 1 and Shift. Those interviewees
would be easily influenced by the interviewer and, when negative feedback is given, believe
that they have made errors. This may well affect their cognitive set detrimentally
(Gudjonsson & Clarke, 1986), and cause shifting. The Shift subscale on the GSS could
Shift scores would be accompanied by high Yield 1 scores; if interviewees are shifting
through being compliant, they would be predicted to score relatively low on Yield 1. These
136
Interrogative suggestibility: when does it matter and what is still to learn?
This research has uncovered a new group of vulnerable interviewees; those who are
considered “normal” i.e. do not have any psychological disorder or disability but, through life
circumstances, have become more easily influenced during social interactions. This can, of
course, have adverse consequences during high stake interactions police interview.
vulnerability, i.e. compliance and acquiescence) needs to be put into context. Just because
certain individuals have a tendency towards being vulnerable (see PACE 1984 for the
specific groups of vulnerable interviewees) does not mean that those vulnerabilities will
definitely manifest during interview. It is the quality of the interview which will ultimately
determine the quality or reliability of the evidence obtained from an interviewee. Only when
the rigorous guidelines (see Home Office, 2007) are not adhered to and interviews with
vulnerable persons are poorly planned, could problems such as suggestibility or compliance
emerge. This is important to understand. An individual may well have a tendency towards
being susceptible to distress, have a high level of attachment anxiety and avoidance, and be
prone to a negative perception of situations, themselves and others. This does not mean
though that those psychological factors will automatically translate into negative performance
There are many factors that could moderate the extent to which vulnerability might be
expressed during an interview (despite the presence of the psychological factors highlighted
over the course of this thesis as perhaps being relevant). To an extent, this thesis can be
considered to represent only one side of a coin. It uncovers the psychological factors, within
an individual, that could give rise to suggestible behaviour if the interview conditions are
137
inappropriate. It would be naive to think though that those factors are the only significant
influences on suggestibility during interview. Even if these preliminary findings turn out to
be repeatedly verified, there is more to suggestibility than this: what about when (potentially)
vulnerable individuals enter the interview room? What are the significant factors that might
The quality of the interview is one factor; interviewer demeanour especially (see research by
Baxter et. al., 2000; 2003; 2006). There may well also be additional factors - external to the
individual (i.e. the complexity of the narrative [in terms of the GSS] or scene witnessed) -
which may interact with the psychological factors uncovered within this thesis as being
important in encouraging suggestibility. Together these may influence the extent to which
external to the interviewee, that occur during interview and interact with the psychology of
the interviewee and effect the extent to which uncertainty and expectations of success are
The models produced within this thesis now need re-testing and developing. These issues will
help to construct a (new) model of interrogative suggestibility. The re-testing of the current
model(s), using the Gudjonsson Compliance Scale (Gudjonsson, 1989) rather than the C facet
138
of the NEO PI-R (alongside the other instruments), is a crucial step in its development. It
should also be noted that the current suggestibility model represents the (possible)
psychological mechanism governing this behaviour in adults within the “normal” population.
Another step would be to: (a) investigate this model in children – to determine how the
mechanism changes across time, from childhood to adulthood and across gender. Gender
may well be a moderator, affecting the iNLE mediated relationship between neuroticism
(Yield 2 and Shift)/vulnerability (Yield 1), FAA and GSS scores, and (b) investigate these
A second objective would be to expand upon my final doctoral study (chapter 5). Like
suggestible behaviour, compliance can also pose a threat to the reliability and credibility of
understand the difference between the two distinct types of psychological vulnerability (in
terms of how they occur; especially since there is tentative evidence within this thesis that
and compliant behaviour can appear identical. Knowledge of the mechanism, and what the
differences are, may help to inform the effective treatment and management of suggestible
This thesis offers an insight into the possible underlying psychological mechanism that may
give rise to vulnerable behaviour during questioning. When combined with the Gudjonsson
and Clarke (1986) model, these findings may bring us a deeper insight into the psychology of
vulnerability during police interview. Using a behavioural genetics and longitudinal research
design (a twin study using both monozygotic and dizygotic twins), the next step will be to
139
investigate the extent to which the measured variables found to be relevant to suggestibility in
the previous research - and the uncovered mechanism - can be accounted for by genetic
and/or environmental influences (shared and non-shared; Donnellan, Burt, Levendosky &
Klump, 2008). The objective: to build upon and develop this current research further, in
suggestibility, compliance and police-induced false confessions. Such research would add to
existing literature and be of use to the practitioners within the applied forensic setting.
140
References:
Andrews, B., & Wilding, J. M. (2004). The relation of depression and anxiety to life-stress
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among young adults: A test
of a four category model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226-244.
Baxter, J.S., Boon, J.C.W., & Marley, C. (2006). Interrogative pressure and responses to
141
Baxter, J.S., Jackson, M. and Bain, S.A. (2003). Interrogative suggestibility: interactions
Beasley, M., Thompson, T., & Davidson, J. (2003). Resilience in response to life stress: The
effects of coping style and cognitive hardiness. Personality and Individual Differences, 34,
77-95.
Becker, M. E. (2006). The impact of childhood adversity factors on posttraumatic stress and
Benton, A. L., & Bandura, A. (1953). Primary and Secondary Suggestibility. Journal of
Biesanz, J.C., & West, S.G. (2004). Towards understanding assessments of the Big Five:
142
Blagrove, M., Cole-Morgan, D., & Lambe, H. (1994). Interrogative suggestibility: the effects
of sleep deprivation and relationship with field dependence. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
8, 169-179.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1 Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
Routledge.
Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46-76). New York: Guilford Press.
Brown, D. (1995). Pseudomemories: the standard science and the standard care in trauma
Bull, R., & Milne, R. (2004). Attempts to improve police interveiwing of suspects. In G. D.
143
Burnette, J. L., Taylor, K., Worthington Jr. E. L., & Forsyth, D. R. (2007), Attachment
working models and trait forgivingness: The mediating role of angry rumination, Personality
Burnette, J. L., Davis, D. E., Green, J. D., Worthington Jr. E. L., & Bradfield, E. (2009).
Insecure attachment and depressive symptoms: The mediating role of rumination, empathy
Cemalcilar, Z., Canbeyli, R., & Sunar, D. (2003). Learned helplessness, therapy, and
personality traits: An experimental study. The Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 65-81.
Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Malloy, L. C., & Allhusen, V. D. (2004). Verbal ability, self-control,
and close relationships with parents protect children against misleading suggestions. Applied
Cohen, L. H., Burt, C. E., & Bjorck, J. P. (1987). Life stress and adjustment: Effects of life
events experienced by young adolescents and their parents. Developmental Psychology, 23,
583–592.
144
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
Collins, N.L., & Read, S.J. (1990). Adult Attachment, Working Models and Relationship
Quality in Dating Couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644-663
Corballis, M.C. & Lea, S.E.G. (1999). The Descent of Mind: Psychological Perspectives on
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R)
and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO FF-I) Professional manual. Odessa, FL:
Cutler, S. E., Larsen, R. J., & Bunce, S. C. (1996). Repressive coping styles and the
experience and recall of emotion: A naturalistic study of daily affect. Journal of Personality,
64, 379-405.
Cyranowski, J. M., Bookwala, J., Houck, P., Pilkonis, P., Kostelnik, B., & Ellen, F. (2002).
psychotherapy in women with recurrent major depression. Journal of Social and Clinical
145
DeWitte, M. & De Houwer, J. (2008). Adult attachment and attention to positive and
Digman, J.M. (1997). Higher-order factors of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and
Donnellan, M. B., Assad, K. K., Robins, R. W., & Conger, R. D. (2007). Do negative
and constraint on relationship satisfaction? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24,
557-573.
Donnellan, M. B., Burt, S. A., Levendosky, A. A., & Klump, K. L. (2008). Genes,
Drake, K. E. & Bull, R. (in press). Life adversity and field-dependence: individual
146
Drake, K. E., Bull, R., & Boon, J. C. W. (2008). Interrogative suggestibility, self-esteem, and
the influence of negative life events. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 13, 299-310.
Drake, K. E., Egan, V., & Bull, R. (in submission). Interrogative suggestibility: linking
Drizin, S. A., & Leo, R. A. (2004). The problem of false confessionsin the post-DNA world.
Dumas, J. A., & Hartman, M. (2003). Adult age differences in temporal and item memory.
Egan, V. (2009). The „Big Five‟: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness, Agreeableness and
Violence: An Evidence-based Approach”, John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., Chichester, UK.
Egan, V., Deary, I.J., & Austin, E. (2000). The NEO-FFI: emerging British norms and an
item-analysis suggest N, A, and C are more reliable than O and E. Personality and Individual
147
Elander, J., West, R. & French, D. (1993). Behavioural correlates of individual differences in
road traffic crash risk: An examination of methods and findings. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
279-294.
Emmett, D., Clifford, B. R., & Gwyer, P. (2003). An investigation of the interaction
Essex, M. J., Klein, M. H., Cho, E. U., & Kraemer, H. C. (2003). Exposure to maternal
depression and marital conflict: Gender differences in children‟s later mental health
symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 728–
737.
148
Fiske, D. W. (1949). Consistency of the factorial structures of personality ratings from
Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2004).
A longitudinal study of implicit and explicit memory in older persons. Psychology and
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments,
154.
Fruzzetti, A. E., Toland, K., Teller, S. A., & Loftus, E. F. (1992). Memory and eyewitness
testimony. In M. Gruneberg & P. Morris (Eds), Aspects of memory, Vol. 1: The practical
Garry, M., Manning, C.G., Loftus, E.F., & Sherman, S.J. (1996). Imagination inflation:
Imagining a childhood event inflates confidence that it occurred. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 3, 208-214.
Gentzler, A. L. & Kerns, K. A. (2006). Adult attachment and memory of emotional reactions
149
Griffin, D., & Bartholomew, K. (1994). Models of the self and other: Fundamental
Gudjonsson, G. H. (2006). First Annual Lionel Haward Lecture 2006. Keynote address
given at the Division of Forensic Psychology Annual conference 2006, University of Central
Chichester: Wiley.
Press.
Suggestibility Scale (GSS 2). Personality and Individual Differences, 13, 479-481.
150
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1991). Suggestibility and compliance among alleged false confessors
and resisters in criminal trials. Medicine, Science and the Law, 31, 147-151.
social evaluative anxiety, state anxiety and method of coping. British Journal of Clinical
Gudjonsson, G. H. (1987). A parallel form of the Gudjonsson Suggestibility Scale. British Journal
151
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., &Bragason, O. O. (2008). Interrogative suggestibility,
compliance and false confessions among prisoners and their relationship with attention deficit
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., & Sigfusdottir, I. D. (2008). False confessions and
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., & Sigfusdottir, I. D. (2009). Interrogations and false
psychological factors best discriminate between false confessors and non-false confessors?
Gudjonsson, G. H., Young, S., & Bramham, J. (2007). Interrogative suggestibility in adults
152
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., Einarsson, E., & Einarsson, J. H. (2008). Personal
versus impersonal relationship compliance and their relationship with personality. Journal of
Gudjonsson, G. H., Sigurdsson, J. F., Lydsdottir, L. B., & Olafsdottir, H. (2008). The
relationship between adult romantic attachment and compliance. Personality and Individual
5, 765-767.
Home Office (1992). Memorandum of Good Practice on Video Recorded Interviews with
153
Home Office (2007). Achieving Best Evidence. Home Office.
Inbau, F. E., Reid, J. E., Buckley, J. P., & Jayne, B. C. (2001). Criminal interrogation and
New findings related to the interviewing of vulnerable persons. iIIRG conference; University
of Teeside; UK.
Joseph, S. & Linley, A. (2008). In Trauma recovery and growth: Positive psychological
perspectives on post-traumatic stress. Joseph, S (Ed.) & Linley, A. (Ed.). pp. 339-356.
responses among Greek basketball referees. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 329–344.
Kassin, S. (1997). The psychology of confession evidence. American Psychologist, 52, 221-
233.
154
Kassin, S. M., Goldstein, C. J., & Savitsky, K. (2003). Behavioral confirmation in the
interrogation room: On the dangers of presuming guilt. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 187–
203.
Kassin, S. M., Drizin, S. A., Grisso, T., Gudjonsson, G. H., Leo, R. A., & Redlich, A. D. (in
press). Police-induced confessions: Risk factors and recommendations. Law and Human
Behavior.
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1994). Memory in naturalistic and laboratory contexts:
Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic
La Rooy, D. & Lamb, M. (2008). What happens when young witnesses are interviewed more
La Rooy, D., Lamb, M. E., & Pipe, M-E. (2008). Repeated Interviewing: A critical
evaluation of the risks and potential benefits. In K. Kuehnle & M. Connell (Eds.) Child
Sexual Abuse: Research, Evaluation, and Testimony for the Courts. Wiley.
155
Leavitt, F. (1997). False attribution of suggestibility to explain recovered memory of
childhood sexual abuse following extended amnesia. Childhood Abuse and Neglect, 21, 265-
276.
Lee, C. M., Beauregard, C., & Bax, K. A. (2005). Child-related disagreements, verbal
Levin, I. P., & Hart, S. S. (2003). Risk preferences in young children: Early evidence of
Linley, A., Joseph, S., & Loumidis, K. (2005). Trauma work, sense of coherence, and
positive and negative changes in therapists. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 74, 185-
188.
of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
Magnus, K., Diener, E., Fujita, F., & Pavot, W. (1993). Extraversion and Neuroticism as
Hinde, J., & Marris, P. Attachment across the life cycle. London: Tavistock/Routledge.
Maner, J. K., Richey, J. A., Cromer, K., Mallott, M., Lejuez, C. W., Joiner, T. E., & Schmidt,
Mazzoni, G.A.L., Loftus, E.F., & Kirsch, I. (2001). Changing beliefs about implausible
McCall, M. E., & Struthers, N. J. (1994). Sex, sex-role orientation and self-esteem as
McConville, M. & Hodgson, J. (1993). Custodial legal advice and the right to silence.
London: HMSO.
The effects of negative feedback. The British Journal of Psychology, 98, 455-465.
157
McGroarty, A & Baxter, J. S. (2009). Interviewer behaviour, interviewee self-esteem and
47, 642-646.
Milne, R. (1999). The Cognitive Interview: Current research and applications. Edited issue
Chichester: Wiley.
Moston, S., Stephenson, G.M., & Williamson, T. M. (1992). The effects of case
23-40.
Noftle, E. E. & Shaver, P. R. (2006). Attachment dimensions and the Big Five personality
158
Norbeck, J. S. (1984). Modification of life event questionnaires for use with female
factor structure in peer nomination personality ratings. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Ofshe, R. J. & Leo, R. A. (1997). The social psychology of police interrogation: the theory
and classification of true and false confessors. Studies in Law, Politics and Society, 16, 189-
251.
Oler, J. A., Fox, A. S., Shelton, S. E., Christian, B. T., Murali, D., Oakes, T. R., Davidson, R.
J., & Kalin, N. H. (2009). Serotonin transporter availability in the amygdale and bed nucleus
of the stria terminalis predicts anxious temperament and brain glucose metabolic activity.
Olvera, N., Remy, R., Power, T. G., Bellamy, C., & Hays, J. (2001). Observed maternal
strategies and children‟s health locus of control in low income Mexican Americans. Journal
Pallant, J. (2007). SPSS survival manual (3rd Ed.). McGraw Hill: Open University Press.
Pape, H. C., Jungling, K., Seidenbecher, T., Lestling, J., & Reinscheid, R. K. (2010).
159
Pearse, J. & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1999). Measuring influential police interviewing tactics: a
Pearse, J. & Gudjonsson, G. H. (1996). Police interviewing techniques at two South London
Peterson, C., Maier, S.F., Seligman, M.E.P. (1995). Learned Helplessness: A Theory for the
Peterson, A., & Taylor, B. (1980). The biological approach to adolescence: Biological change
Quas, J. A., Qin, J., Shaaf, J., & Goodman, G. S. (1997). Individual differences in children‟s
and adult‟s suggestibility and false event memory. Learning and Individual Differences.
equation modelling for counselling psychology. The Counseling Psychologist, 27, 485-527.
160
Rosenman, S. & Rodgers, B. (2004). Childhood adversity in an Australian population.
Rosenman, S. & Rodgers, B. (2006). Childhood adversity and adult personality. Australian
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the
attribution process. 'In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol.
Safford, S. M., Alloy, L. B., Abramson, L. Y., & Crossfield, A. G. (2007). Negative
Santtila, P., Alkiora, P., Ekholm, M., & Niemi, P. (1999). False confessions in a robbery: the
161
Sarason, I., Johnson, J., & Siegel, J. (1978). Assessing the impact of life changes:
Development of the life experiences survey. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
46, 932–946.
Scarr, S. & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A- story of
Francisco: Freeman.
Simpson, J. S., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An
boys and its relationship with intelligence, memory, and cognitive set. Journal of
Stern, W. (1910). Abstracts of lectures on the psychology of testimony and on the study of
Stern, W. (1939). The psychology of testimony. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
34, 3–20.
162
Stukat, K. G. (1958). Suggestibility: a factor and experimental analysis. Stockholm:
Swearing, E. & Cohen, L. (1985). Life events and psychological distress: A prospective
Taylor, L. C., Clayton, J. D., & Rowley, S. J. (2004). Academic socialisation: understanding
Thelwell, R. C., Lane, A. M., & Weston, N. J. V. (2007). Mood states, self-set goals, self-
42, 573–583.
Todman, J., & Drysdale, E. (2004). Effects of qualitative differences in initial and subsequent
Vermigli, P. & Toni, A. (2004). Attachment and field dependence: individual differences in
Wei, M., Mallinckrodt, B., Russell, D. W., & Abraham, T. W. (2004). Maladaptive
perfectionism as a mediator and moderator between adult attachment and depressive mood.
163
Wells, G. L., Memon, A., & Penrod, S. D. (2006). Eyewitness evidence: improving its
Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., & Karp, S. A. (1962).
Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. E. (1971). A manual for the embedded
among anxious patients and normal controls. Personality and Individual Differences, 25,
425–432.
164
APPENDIX I: SUMMARY OF CASES OF WRONGFUL CONVICTION OVER PAST
TEN YEARS IN ENGLAND AND WALES.
165
166
167
168