Object Relations Theory
Object Relations Theory
(Melanie Klein)
I. Biography
B. Positions
D. Internalizations
I. Biography
1. Phantasies
2. Objects
B. Positions
1. Paranoid-Schizoid Position
2. Depressive Position
1. Introjection
2. Projection
3. Splitting
4. Projective identification
D. Internalizations
1. Ego
2. Superego
3. Oedipus Complex
A. Psychic Life of the Infant
1. Phantasies (fantasies)
• Infants, even at birth, possess an active phantasy life ( should not be confused with the conscious fantasies of
older children and adults; hence, the spelling)
• Are psychic representations of unconscious id instincts represented in images of good and bad (unconscious
images of good and bad)
• Earliest object relations are with the mother’s breast; then, face and hands
• In their active fantasy, infants introject, or take into their psychic structure, these
external objects, including body parts
• Introjected objects are more than internal thoughts about external objects; they are
fantasies of internalizing the object in concrete and physical terms.
• For example, children who have introjected their mother believe that she is
constantly inside their own body.
B. Positions
• Klein (1946) saw human infants as constantly engaging in a basic conflict between the life
instinct and the death instinct (between good and bad, love and hate, creativity and
destruction.)
• As the ego moves toward integration and away from disintegration, infants naturally prefer
gratifying sensations over frustrating ones.
• In their attempt to deal with this dichotomy of good and bad feelings, infants organize their
experiences into positions, or ways of dealing with both internal and external objects.
• Klein chose the term “position” rather than “stage of development” to indicate that
positions alternate back and forth; they are not periods of time or phases of development
through which a person passes.
• Although she used psychiatric or pathological labels, Klein intended these positions to
represent normal social growth and development.
• The two basic positions are the paranoid-schizoid position and the depressive position.
1. Paranoid-Schizoid Position
• 3 – 4 months of life;
• a way of organizing experiences that includes both paranoid feelings of being
persecuted and a splitting of internal and external objects into the good and the
bad;
• ego’s perception of the external world is subjective and fantastic rather than
objective and real;
• persecutory feelings are paranoid – not based on any real or immediate danger.
• Infants, of course, do not use language to identify the good and bad breast.
Rather, they have a biological predisposition to attach a positive value to
nourishment and the life instinct and to assign a negative value to hunger
and the death instinct.
• This preverbal splitting of the world into good and bad serves as a prototype
for the subsequent development of ambivalent feelings toward a single
person.
2. Depressive Position
• When the depressive position is resolved, children close the split between the
good and the bad mother.
• They are able not only to experience love from their mother, but also to display
their own love for her.
positive feelings toward both parents during the oedipal years; establish
positive attitude with the good gratifying object and avoid the bad object.
Margaret Mahler
• Margaret Schoenberger Mahler (1897–1985) was born in Sopron, Hungary, and
received a medical degree from the University of Vienna in 1923.
• In 1938, she moved to New York, where she was a consultant to the Children’s
Service of the New York State Psychiatric Institute.
• She later established her own observational studies at the Masters Children’s
Center in New York.
• From 1955 to 1974, she was clinical professor of psychiatry at Albert Einstein
College of Medicine
Margaret Mahler’s View on ORT
• Mahler was primarily concerned with the psychological
birth of the individual that takes place during the first 3
years of life, a time when a child gradually surrenders
security for autonomy.
• By psychological birth, Mahler meant that the child
becomes an individual separate from his or her primary
caregiver, an accomplishment that leads ultimately to a
sense of identity.
Heinz Kohut (1913–1981)
• Heinz Kohut was born in Vienna to educated and talented Jewish
parents (Strozier, 2001).
• Kohut believed that human relatedness, not innate instinctual drives, are at the core
of human personality.
• infants require adult caregivers not only to gratify physical needs but also to satisfy
basic psychological needs.
• In caring for both physical and psychological needs, adults, or self-objects, treat
infants as if they had a sense of self.
• The self gives unity and consistency to one’s experiences, remains relatively
stable over time, and is “the center of initiative and a recipient of impressions”
• The self is also the child’s focus of interpersonal relations, shaping how he or
she will relate to parents and other selfobjects.
• Nuclear Self
• is developed through a positive interactive process between the child and
the parents within the first 2-3 years of life;
Internal Objects
Transmuting internalization
• The early self becomes crystallized around two basic narcissistic needs:
• The idealized parent image is opposed to the grandiose self because it implies that
someone else is perfect.
• Nevertheless, it too satisfies a narcissistic need because the infant adopts the attitude “You
are perfect, but I am part of you.”
John Bowlby’s Attachment Theory
• he realized that object relations theory could be integrated
with an evolutionary perspective.
• He also took childhood as starting point for his theory and
then extrapolated it forward to adulthood.
• Bowlby firmly believed that the attachments formed during
childhood have an important impact on adulthood.
• Because childhood attachments are crucial to later
development, Bowlby argued that investigators should
study childhood directly and not rely on distorted
retrospective accounts from adults
Three stages of separation anxiety
• The origins of attachment theory came from Bowlby’s observations that both human
and primate infants go through a clear sequence of reactions when separated from
their primary caregivers.
• Bowlby observed three stages of separation anxiety: (1) protest, (2) apathy and
despair, and (3) emotional detachment from people, including the primary caregiver.
• Children who reach the third stage lack warmth and emotion in their later
relationships.
Two fundamental assumptions
• First, a responsive and accessible caregiver (usually the mother) must create a
secure base for the child.
• The infant needs to know that the caregiver is accessible and dependable.
• If this dependability is present, the child is better able to develop confidence and
security in exploring the world.
• She received her BA, MA, and PhD, all from the University of
Toronto, where she also served as instructor and lecturer.
• Klein believed that an important part of any relationship is the internal psychic representations of early significant
objects, such as the mother’s breast or the father’s penis.
• Infants introject these psychic representations into their own psychic structure and then project them onto an external object,
that is, another person. These internal pictures are not accurate representations of the other person but are remnants of
earlier interpersonal experiences.
• a number of other theorists have expanded and modified object relations theory.
• Margaret Mahler believed that children’s sense of identity rests on a three-step relationship with their mother. First, infants
have basic needs cared for by their mother; next, they develop a safe symbiotic relationship with an all-powerful mother; and
finally, they emerge from their mother’s protective circle and establish their separate individuality.
• Heinz Kohut theorized that children develop a sense of self during early infancy when parents and others treat them as if they
had an individualized sense of identity.
• John Bowlby investigated infants’ attachment to their mother as well as the negative consequences of being separated from
their mother.
• Mary Ainsworth and her colleagues developed a technique for measuring the type of attachment style an infant develops
toward its caregiver