Chapter 1 Part One
Chapter 1 Part One
Patsy Lightbown
&
Nina Spada
The first three years:
Milestones and developmental sequences
At interesting thing about first language acquisition
is the high degree of similarity in the early language
of children all over the world.
The earliest vocalization are simply the involuntary
cry that babies do when they are hungry or
uncomfortable.
Soon, however, we hear the cooing and gurgling
sounds of contented baby, lying in their beds looking
at fascinating shapes and movement around them.
continued
By the age of two, most children reliably produce at least
50 different words and some produced many more.
About this time, they begin to combine words into simple
sentences such as ‘Mommy juice’ and ‘baby fall down’.
These sentences are sometimes called ‘telegraphic’
because they leave out such things as articles,
prepositions, and auxiliary verbs.
We recognize them as sentences, even though function
words and grammatical morphemes are missing.
continued
Researchers have shown that tiny babies can hear
the difference between ‘pa’ and ‘ba’, for example.
By the end of their first year, most babies
understand quite a few frequently repeated words.
They wave when someone say ‘bye-bye’; they
eagerly hurried to the kitchen when ‘juice and
cookies’ are mentioned.
At 12 months, most babies will have begun to
produce a word or two that everyone recognizes.
continued
As children progress through the discovery of language in
their first three years, there are predictable patterns in the
emergence and development of many features of the
language they are learning.
For some language features, these patterns have been
described in terms of developmental sequences or
‘stages’.
These stages in language acquisition are related to
children’s cognitive development.
For example, children don’t use temporal adverbs like
‘tomorrow’ until they understand time.
Grammatical morpheme
In the 1960s, several researchers focused on how
children acquire grammatical morphemes in English.
In his book (1973), Roger Brown studied
(longitudinally) the language development of 3
children (Adam, Sarah and Eve).
Brown found that 14 grammatical morphemes were
acquired in a remarkably similar sequence.
The following list shows some of the morphemes
Brown’s studied:
Continued
Present progressive –ing (Mommy running)
Plural –s (Two books)
Irregular past forms (Baby went)
Possessive –s (Daddy’s hat)
Copula (Annie is happy)
Articles the and a
Regular past –ed (She walked)
Third person singular simple present –s (She runs)
Auxiliary be (He is coming)
Continued
Brown found that a child who had mastered the
grammatical morpheme at the bottom of the list was
sure to have mastered those at the top, but the
reverse was not true.
Thus, there was evidence for a ‘developmental
sequence’ or order of acquisition.
However, the children didn’t acquire the
morphemes at the same age or rate.
Eve mastered all the morphemes before she was 2.5,
while Sarah and Adam after 4.
Continued
Jill and Peter de Villiers (1973) confirmed Brown’s
study in a cross-sectional study of 21 children.
Many hypotheses have been advanced to explain
why these grammatical morphemes are acquired in
the observed order.
However, there has been no simple satisfactory
explanation for the sequence.
Most researchers agree that the order is determined
by an interaction of different factors.
Continued
Some carefully designed procedures have been
developed to further explore children’s knowledge
of grammatical morphemes.
One of the first and best known is the so-called
‘wug test’ developed by Jean Berko Gleason in the
1950s.
This kind of experiment shows that children’s
language is not just a list of memorized words; but it
demonstrates that they know the rule for the
formation of plural in English.
Negation
Children learn the functions of negation very early.
Although children understand and express
negation, it takes them some time to express them
in sentences using the appropriate word order
(Bloom 1991).
The following stages of negation development have
been observed in the acquisition of English. Similar
stages have been observed in other languages as
well.
Continued
Stage 1
Negation is usually expressed by the word ‘no’.
No. No cookie. No comb hair.
Stages 2
Utterances are longer and may include a subject.
Daddy no comb hair. Don’t touch that!
Stages 3
Negation is used with complex sentences. New
forms of negation appear.
I can’t do it. He don’t want it.
Continued
Stage 4
Children attach negative elements to correct forms
of auxiliary verbs.
You didn’t have supper.
She doesn't want it.
Even though their language system is by now
quite complex, they may still have difficulty with
other features related to negatives; e.g.,
I don’t have no more candies.
Questions
There is a remarkable consistency in the way
children learn to form questions in English.
There is a predictable order in which the wh-
words emerge (Bloom 1991).
The first form of a wh-question seems to be the
chunk ‘whassat?’.
‘Where’ and ‘who’ emerge very soon. These seem
to be the first wh-questions to be asked by adults,
for example; ‘where’s mommy?’, ‘who’s that?’
Continued
Child when can we go outside?
Parent in about 5 minutes.
Child 1-2-3-4-5!! can we go now?