Dynamics of Well-Being Module 4.1
Dynamics of Well-Being Module 4.1
Dynamics of Well-Being
[PSYC238]
1
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Module IV : Cultural
Perspective
2
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
‘Cognitive appraisal’ describes how we consider our global (overall) life satisfaction
and our satisfaction with specific domains (e.g. family life, career, and so forth).
Other unfortunate people are missing all three. Most of us also know individuals
who have one type of happiness but not another. For example, imagine an
elderly person who is completely satisfied with her life—she has done most
everything she ever wanted—but is not currently enjoying life that much because
of the infirmities of age.
There are others who show a different pattern, for example, who really enjoy life
but also experience a lot of stress, anger, and worry. And there are those who are
having fun, but who are dissatisfied and believe they are wasting their lives.
In contrast, it is easier to be happy if one has supportive family and friends, ample
resources to meet one’s needs, and good health.
But even here there are exceptions—people who are depressed and unhappy
while living in excellent circumstances. Thus, people can be happy or unhappy
because of their personalities and the way they think about the world or because
of the external circumstances in which they live.
Can you guess what might make some societies happier than others? Much of
North America and Europe have relatively high life satisfaction, and much of
Africa is low in life satisfaction.
However, other factors, such as trusting and being able to count on others, are
also crucial to the happiness within nations. Indeed, for enjoying life our
relationships with others seem more important than living in a wealthy society.
Will money make you happy? A certain level of income is needed to meet our
needs, and very poor people are frequently dissatisfied with life (Diener &
Seligman, 2004).
However, having more and more money has diminishing returns—higher and
higher incomes make less and less difference to happiness. Wealthy nations
tend to have higher average life satisfaction than poor nations, but the United
States has not experienced a rise in life satisfaction over the past decades, even
as income has doubled.
The goal is to find a level of income that you can live with and earn. Don’t let your
aspirations continue to rise so that you always feel poor, no matter how much
money you have. Research shows that materialistic people often tend to be less
happy, and putting your emphasis on relationships and other areas of life besides
just money is a wise strategy.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
There are stories of wealthy people who are unhappy and of janitors who are very
happy. For instance, a number of extremely wealthy people in South Korea have
committed suicide recently, apparently brought down by stress and other negative
feelings.
On the other hand, there is the hospital janitor who loved her life because she felt
that her work in keeping the hospital clean was so important for the patients and
nurses.
When people think money is more important than everything else, they
seem to have a harder time being happy. And unless they make a
great deal of money, they are not on average as happy as others.
Most of the evidence so far suggests that happy people are healthier, more
sociable, more productive, and better citizens (Diener & Tay, 2012;
Lyubomirsky, King, & Diener, 2005).
Research shows that the happiest individuals are usually very sociable. The
table below summarizes some of the major findings.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
At times a bit of worry mixed with positive feelings makes people more
creative. Most successful people in the workplace seem to be those
who are mostly positive but sometimes a bit negative. Thus, people
need not be a superstar in happiness to be a superstar in life. What is
not helpful is to be chronically unhappy.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
If you feel mostly positive and satisfied, and yet occasionally worry and
feel stressed, this is probably fine as long as you feel comfortable with
this level of happiness.
Most people are fairly happy, but many of them also wish they could be a bit
more satisfied and enjoy life more.
For instance, between the years 1958 and 1987 Japanese life satisfaction
fluctuated around 6 on a 10-point scale (Veenhoven 1993).
For instance, Oishi (2001a) found that European Americans were significantly
more satisfied with their lives than Asian Americans.
Similarly, Okazaki (2000) observed that Asian Americans reported higher levels
of depression and anxiety than did European Americans.
Thus, there are differences between nations, and between ethnic groups within
nations.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
For instance, Oishi (2001a) found that European Americans were significantly
more satisfied with their lives than Asian Americans.
Similarly, Okazaki (2000) observed that Asian Americans reported higher levels
of depression and anxiety than did European Americans.
Thus, there are differences between nations, and between ethnic groups within
nations.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
In more collectivist societies (e.g., East Asia, Central/South America), the goals
and needs of a significant in-group tend to take priority over the thoughts,
values, and preferences of an individual.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
In collectivist cultures, on the other hand, strong social support may buffer
stressful events, but the drawback is that there is less freedom to pursue
personally rewarding goals.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Why? Again, a popular explanation is that individualist nations are richer than
collectivist nations, implying that differences in objective life conditions
affecting their inhabitants account for this cultural difference.
As mentioned earlier, however, there are too many affluent collectivist nations
(Japan, Hong Kong) that go against this simple economic interpretation. Also,
according to Diener et al., when a nation's degree of individualism is
statistically controlled, income no longer predicts SWB.
There clearly seem to be other reasons, besides income, that contribute to the
high SWB of individualist cultures. What are they?
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Various factors beyond personal control, such as luck or family background, are
thought to play significant roles in determining the ultimate happiness of an
individual.
Being more eager to be happy, individualists might organize their lives in ways that
would give them the best chance to be happy.
These ideas need to be tested rigorously with much more empirical data.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Cultures not only differ in mean levels of SWB, they also tend to base happiness on
somewhat different experiences.
In the past when SWB research was conducted primarily among North American participants,
many researchers assumed that high self-esteem was the single most important ingredient of
happiness.
After all, what could be more important to happiness than having a positive self-view, that is,
high self-esteem? Many Westerners might be surprised to learn, however, that the term self-
esteem does not even exist in some cultures (for instance, in the Japanese, Chinese, or
Korean language).
We might wish to ask the question again: Is self-esteem equally critical for happiness across
cultures? The answer is no, according to recent findings (Diener & Diener, 1995; Kwan, Bond,
& Singelis, 1997). Self-esteem relates strongly with SWB in individualistic cultures, but the link
becomes considerably weaker in collectivist cultures. Among female college students in India,
for instance, Diener and Diener failed to find any significant association between self-esteem
and SWB
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
This idea fits well with the dynamics of individualist cultures, where the inner self is
believed to be the primary source of personal meaning and guidance. Because the self
plays such a vital role in everyday life, it becomes necessary to build and maintain a
self-system that is well-organized and consistent.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
In order to achieve this goal of interpersonal harmony, the self needs to be highly
sensitive to social cues, and in many cases, adjust the self according to the needs and
expectations of other people.
In other words, the self is required to be quite flexible across social situations in cultures
where values promoting harmony (e.g., modesty, obedience) often overshadow the
importance of consistency.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Interestingly, a recent study found that Asian Americans and Japanese who were
pursuing their goals to make others happy became happier over time when they attained
their goals, whereas those who were pursuing their goals for fun and enjoyment did not
become happier over time, even when they achieved their goals (Oishi & Diener, 2001).
Asakawa and Csikszentmihalyi (1998) found that Asian-American students were happy
when they were engaging in an activity that was related to important future goals (e.g.,
academic achievement).
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Interestingly, a recent study found that Asian Americans and Japanese who were
pursuing their goals to make others happy became happier over time when they attained
their goals, whereas those who were pursuing their goals for fun and enjoyment did not
become happier over time, even when they achieved their goals (Oishi & Diener, 2001).
Asakawa and Csikszentmihalyi (1998) found that Asian-American students were happy
when they were engaging in an activity that was related to important future goals (e.g.,
academic achievement).
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences
Furthermore, Caucasian students tended to be less happy when they were engaging in
an activity that was related to important future goals.
These findings suggest that (a) there are cultural variations in motivation to be happy
"now" versus "in the future, " and (b) pathways to happiness seem to vary across
cultures, depending on socially desirable forms of motivation.
Amity Institute of Psychology and Allied Sciences