Topic 6 - Language Processing
Topic 6 - Language Processing
PSYCHOLINGUISTICS
Lecture 10
Language Processing
1
Introduction
• We engage in language processing every day of our
lives.This processing takes place when we watch
television, listen to the radio, read a passing billboard,
write a letter, or have a conversation.
The symbol ʔ represents a ‘glottal stop’ – a brief blockage of air at the back of the back of the
throat.
3
• Here are the levels of
representation through
which a listener might
conceivably need to
proceed.
4
• It is unlikely that the process we have just examined
represents what actually happens. There are a number
of reason. Among them are:
5
In addition…
6
Serial vs. Parallel Processing
• Evidence suggests that a listener begins to process an
utterance about 200 milliseconds after the speaker has
begun to speak.
7
• Let us assume that the listener
is operating at the phonological
level, attributing a standard
from to [ʧeɪnʤ], the last part of
the utterance.
• By this point, they might have
reached the segmentational
level with GOTANY and be
inserting possible word
boundaries.
• They might have reached the
syntactic level with GOT and be
working out its grammatical
role.
• So, different parts of the
utterance are being processed
at different levels – all at the
same time.
8
9
The Role of Context
• The sequence in both serial and parallel processing
operates in one direction – bottom – up – and it is
based entirely on what the listener hears or the reader
sees.
• But it is possible that our analysis of the input is
influenced by external factors in a top-down way.
World Paralinguistic
knowledge evidence
An expectation
based on
Analogy
words already
uttered
Language
Processing
10
From Concept to Expression
• The Speech Chain - The different forms in which a
spoken message exists in its progress from the mind of
the speaker to the mind of the listener.
11
From Concept to Expression
• Two kinds of data in the construction of speech
production models – SPEECH ERRORS and SPEECH
DISFLUENCIES.
12
Sources of Data for Models of
Speech Production
1. Speech error
• All of us have experienced, either as speakers or hearer,
utterances that seem to have gotten mixed up on their
way out.
Intended Utterance Actual Utterance
You have missed all my history. You have hissed all my mystery lectures.
You have wasted the whole term. You have tasted the whole worm.
You are occupying my pew You are occupewing my pie
The dear old Queen The queer old dean
14
• A sufferer of dysgraphia, which is a form of dyslexia -
disturbance of the clear distinction of the sounds of
words - confusion between closely related phonemes.
15
2. Disfluencies
• Many utterances are characterized by hesitations,
repetitions, false starts, and “filler” words such as um,
well, or you know (sometimes called filled pauses).
16
• Such lapses in fluent speech production actually provide
valuable insights into the units of speech production and
permit us to evaluate how much of speech is mentally
planned in advance of its production.
17
The Units in
Speech
Production
Phonemic Phonetic
The Syllable Stress
Segments Features
• Phonemic Segments: There are FOUR (4) kinds of errors at this level:
1. Anticipation errors
• In anticipation errors, sounds which come later in the utterance
inappropriately appear earlier than intended.
Consonant anticipation
Vowel anticipation
18
2. Preservation Errors
• In preservation errors, a sound produced early in an utterance reappears
in an incorrect location later in the utterance.
Consonant preservation
Vowel preservation
3. Phoneme Exchange
• In phoneme exchange errors, phonemes are reversed.
4. Phoneme Deletion
• In phoneme deletion errors, phonemes are deleted.
19
What Speech Error Data Suggest About
The Process of Speech Production
• The lexicon is organized both semantically and
phonologically.
– In word substitution errors and word blends, words
involved are semantically or phonologically similar or
both.
20
What Speech Error Data Suggest About
The Process of Speech Production
– Examples;
• That’s a horse of another color … a horse of
another race (semantic substitution)
21
• Affixes behave differently from contents words in slips of
the tongue/speech errors.
22
What Speech Error Data Suggest About
The Process of Speech Production
– Examples;
• Did you stay up very late last night? Did you
stay up late very last night?
23
The Garrett Model
• In 1975, Garrett proposed a speech production model
based on speech error data.
24
Garrett’s 1975 Model
25
Garrett’s 1975 Model
28
Dell’s Model
– Example : Some swimmers sink
– The speaker’s thought, “swimmers,” activates,
among other things, a class of nouns, nominal
affixes – plural
• It is theoretically
possible to have
interactions between
semantic and
phonological
representations , leading
to slips that share both
phonological and
semantics properties
with the intended output.
• To what extent were you aware of dealing with low level issues
(spelling, letter order, vocabulary, grammar)?
• To what extent were you preoccupied with higher level issues
31
(ordering ideas, planning arguments)?
• It seems that, when we are in the process of writing, we
need to store strings of words in a kind of buffer in the
mind.
33
The Stages of Writing
1. The amount of processing at the formulation stage is one of the
differences between a skilled and a less skilled writer.
35
Eye Movements in Reading
• Here are some assumptions that are sometimes made
about reading as a process – especially in the ‘speed
reading’ literature. Do you agree or disagree with
them?
Efficient readers do not need to read all the words in a text.
They predict many words from the context in which they
appear.
Efficient readers make large sweeps with their eyes as they
read along lines of text.
Words can be identified by their overall shapes. So longer
words often take the same time to read as short ones.
A slow reader is one whose eyes do not move fast enough
from left to right. Increases in reading speed can be achieved
without loss of comprehension.
36
• Reading involves a series of rapid eye movements
(known as saccades) along the line of print or writing,
followed by periods of fixation when the eye rests
upon a point in the text.